Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:57:24.722Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cannabis use and episodic memory performance among adolescents: Moderating effects of depression symptoms and sex

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 February 2023

Sarah M. Lehman*
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
Erin L. Thompson
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
Ileana Pacheco-Colón
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
Samuel W. Hawes
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
Ashley R. Adams
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
Karen Granja
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
William J. Pulido
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
Raul Gonzalez
Affiliation:
Center for Children and Families, Department of Psychology, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199-2156, USA
*
Corresponding author: Sarah M. Lehman, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Cannabis use has been linked to poorer episodic memory. However, little is known about whether depression and sex may interact as potential moderators of this association, particularly among adolescents. The current study addresses this by examining interactions between depression symptoms and sex on the association between cannabis use and episodic memory in a large sample of adolescents.

Method:

Cross-sectional data from 360 adolescents (Mage = 17.38, SD = .75) were analyzed at the final assessment wave of a two-year longitudinal study. We used the Drug Use History Questionnaire to assess for lifetime cannabis use, and the Computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Fourth edition to assess the number of depression symptoms in the past year. Subtests from the Wechsler Memory Scale, Fourth Edition and the California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition were used to assess episodic memory performance.

Results:

The effect of the three-way interaction among cannabis use, depression symptoms, and sex did not have a significant impact on episodic memory performance. However, follow-up analyses revealed a significant effect of the two-way interaction of cannabis use and depression symptoms on episodic memory, such that associations between cannabis use and episodic memory were only significant at lower and average levels of depression symptoms.

Conclusions:

Contrary to our hypotheses, we found that as depression symptoms increased, the negative association between cannabis use and episodic memory diminished. Given the use of a predominantly subsyndromic sample, future studies should attempt to replicate findings among individuals with more severe depression.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2023

Introduction

Cannabis remains one of the most widely used substances among adolescents in the United States (Johnston et al., Reference Johnston, Miech, O’Malley, Bachman, Schulenberg and Patrick2021). Notably, episodic memory deficits are one of the most frequently reported consequences of cannabis use (Blest-Hopley et al., Reference Blest-Hopley, Giampietro and Bhattacharyya2020; Broyd et al., Reference Broyd, van Hell, Beale, Yücel and Solowij2016; Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Tolomeo, Steele and Baldacchino2020; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Slomiak, Jones, Rosen, Moore and Gur2018). Given the vulnerability toward substance use in adolescence (Arain et al., Reference Arain, Haque, Johal, Mathur, Nel, Rais, Sandhu and Sharma2013; Blakemore & Choudhury, Reference Blakemore and Choudhury2006; Ghetti & Bunge, Reference Ghetti and Bunge2012) and the central role of episodic memory in the daily functioning of human life (Ghetti & Bunge, Reference Ghetti and Bunge2012), it is important to evaluate factors that may serve to exacerbate episodic memory problems from cannabis use among adolescents.

Multiple systematic reviews and meta-analyses have synthesized the vast literature on the effects of cannabis use on neurocognition with results continuing to suggest adverse effects in the domain of episodic memory. For instance, a recent meta-analysis was performed on 13 adult population studies and found an association between chronic cannabis use and memory impairments (Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Tolomeo, Steele and Baldacchino2020). Additionally, a meta-analysis by Scott et al. (Reference Scott, Slomiak, Jones, Rosen, Moore and Gur2018) examined 69 studies on adolescents and adults and found effect sizes (ranging from mean d, −0.33 to −0.21) across cognitive domains with one of the largest effect sizes in delayed memory (d = −0.26). Furthermore, a recent review focusing exclusively on the effects of cannabis use and the adolescent brain noted impaired cognitive functioning across several domains including episodic memory (Blest-Hopley et al., Reference Blest-Hopley, Colizzi, Giampietro and Bhattacharyya2020). The aforementioned meta-analyses and reviews demonstrate adverse effects of cannabis use on episodic memory. However, inconsistencies were noted across these studies, emphasizing the need for future research to examine the impact of other potentially confounding variables such as mental health and sex (Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Tolomeo, Steele and Baldacchino2020; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Slomiak, Jones, Rosen, Moore and Gur2018). Examination of potential moderating effects has important implications for further understanding the associations between cannabis use and neurocognitive deficits and could serve to inform future prevention and intervention efforts.

Mental health problems often emerge during adolescence, with depression among the most common (Avenevoli et al., Reference Avenevoli, Swendsen, He, Burstein and Merikangas2015; Mojtabai et al., Reference Mojtabai, Olfson and Han2016). Much of the literature on depression and episodic memory performance has focused on adult populations and has found higher levels of depressive symptoms to be associated with poorer episodic memory performance (Ahern & Semkovska, Reference Ahern and Semkovska2017; Goodall et al., Reference Goodall, Fisher, Hetrick, Phillips, Parrish and Allott2018; McDermott & Ebmeier, Reference McDermott and Ebmeier2009). A meta-analysis from Ahern & Semkovska (Reference Ahern and Semkovska2017) focusing on the first episode of major depression in adults found a broad range of impairments across cognitive domains compared to healthy controls, including small to moderate effects across a variety of episodic memory tasks. These results appear to extend to adolescent samples. Indeed, a meta-analysis focusing on adolescents and young adults (ages 12 to 25) with a diagnosis of depressive disorder found small effects of depression diagnosis on performance on episodic memory tasks (Goodall et al., Reference Goodall, Fisher, Hetrick, Phillips, Parrish and Allott2018). Additionally, a study conducted by Barch et al., (Reference Barch, Harms, Tillman, Hawkey and Luby2019) examined the effects of depression on cognitive functioning among a sample of adolescents and found a negative association between depression severity and episodic memory performance. Overall, this expanding literature continues to find significant effects, indicating the need for further exploration of the role of depression as a risk factor for decreased episodic memory function in adolescents.

Few studies have examined the combined effects of cannabis use and depression on memory performance. A study by Roebke et al., (Reference Roebke, Vadhan, Brooks and Levin2014) found main effects of both major depressive disorder (MDD) and cannabis use on episodic memory performance among adults but did not find significant differences in learning and episodic memory performance between adult cannabis dependent participants with and without a comorbid depressive disorder (diagnosed with either [MDD] or dysthymic disorder). In contrast, a similar study among young adults found the same main effects, with both depressed individuals and cannabis users recalling fewer words in short and long-delay recall trials than controls (Radoman et al., Reference Radoman, Hoeppner, Schuster, Evins and Gilman2019); however, they also found an additive effect of MDD and cannabis use on verbal learning and memory recall, such that cannabis users who had a diagnosis of MDD showed the worst performance (Radoman et al., Reference Radoman, Hoeppner, Schuster, Evins and Gilman2019). The conflicting results found from the aforementioned studies highlight the need for further exploration and consideration for adolescent samples.

Finally, it is important to consider that there may be sex differences in the interactive effect of cannabis use and depression on episodic memory. The literature reports differences among male and female depression rates, with females almost twice as likely to experience symptoms of depression starting in adolescence (Ferrari et al., Reference Ferrari, Somerville, Baxter, Norman, Patten, Vos and Whiteford2013; Salk et al., Reference Salk, Hyde and Abramson2017). Additionally, the association between frequency of cannabis use and poorer episodic memory performance has been documented more often among females than males (Crane et al., Reference Crane, Schuster and Gonzalez2013). To our knowledge, the current study will be the first to examine interactive effects of cannabis use, MDD symptoms, and sex on episodic memory among adolescents.

The current study aimed to replicate existing literature on cannabis and episodic memory in an adolescent sample (N = 360), and extend results by examining the moderating roles of both past year MDD symptoms and sex on this association. The examination of these interactions among adolescents is needed to advance the understanding of risk factors that might negatively impact episodic memory, and to better understand individual differences that may place teens at risk for adverse outcomes from cannabis use. We hypothesized that (a) greater lifetime frequency of cannabis use would be associated with worse episodic memory performance, and that (b) this association would be stronger among individuals with more past year MDD symptoms. Furthermore, we hypothesized that (c) the impact of past year MDD symptoms on the association between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and episodic memory would be stronger among females than males.

Method

Participants and procedures

Participants were 360 adolescents (M age = 17.38, SD = .75) whose data were collected as part of a larger longitudinal study examining the associations between decision-making, episodic memory performance, and cannabis use trajectories (R01 DA031176; PI: RG). Participants were recruited throughout the greater Miami area at middle and high schools, parks, movie theaters, and word-of-mouth. Participants were 14–17 years old at baseline and ranged from 15–19 years old at the final assessment wave [time point five (T5)], approximately 2 years after the initial baseline visit. Phone screens were used to assess study eligibility with inclusion criteria consisting of the ability to read and write in English. Additionally, the goal of the sample was to recruit adolescents who were at risk for cannabis use escalation; therefore, participants needed to self-report any lifetime use of alcohol, cigarettes, or cannabis – even if minimal (i.e., only a sip or a puff). However, we recruited about 10% of the sample with no substance use history to eliminate the risk of participants being identified as substance users through study participation. Briefly, exclusion criteria consisted of any history of psychiatric or mood disorders (i.e., received a diagnosis and were either prescribed psychotropic medication for or underwent therapy in relation to specific diagnosis), self-reported neurological condition, history of cannabis or alcohol use disorder, birth complications, or traumatic brain injury or loss of consciousness > 10 min. However, participants diagnosed with a psychiatric or mood disorder, or cannabis or alcohol use disorder, after completing the baseline assessment were maintained in the study. Participant inclusion and exclusion criteria at baseline have been described previously in further detail (Pacheco-Colón et al., Reference Pacheco- Colón, Hawes, Duperrouzel and Gonzalez2021). Data were collected over a two-year period consisting of five different visits scheduled approximately six months apart. Participants completed semi-structured interviews and self-report measures assessing mental health and substance use. Neuropsychological testing was administered at three separate time points each one year apart. The sample population was majority Hispanic/Latino (89.2%), consistent with the demographics of Miami-Dade County. All participant characteristics are reported in Table 1. Due to the greatest amount of cumulative cannabis use reported at the final time point of the larger longitudinal study, data for the current study was analyzed at T5. At T5, 4.4% of the sample reported experiencing a major depressive episode (MDE) in the past year and 23.1% met criteria for current cannabis use disorder. Written adolescent assent and parental/guardian consent were obtained prior to each assessment, and the Institutional Review Board at Florida International University provided approval for the study. Additionally, this study was completed in compliance with institutional research standards for human research and in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Table 1. Participant demographics and characteristics at time point 5 (T5)

Note. All numbers are reported from time point 5, unless otherwise specified. SD = Standard Deviation; Md = Median; IQR = Interquartile Range; WRAT-4 = Wide Range Achievement Test; CDISC-IV = Computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Fourth Edition; CVLT-2 = California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition; WMS = Wechsler Memory Scale; MDD; Major Depressive Disorder; MDE = Major Depressive Episode; GAD = Generalized Anxiety Disorder; Other Race = American Indian/Alaska Islander, Asian, Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander, or Unknown.

Measures

Participant History Questionnaire (PHQ)

This semi-structured interview was administered at baseline to assess participant and parental demographic information such as age, sex, ethnicity, race, and history of psychological disorders. Age at T5 was calculated and used as a covariate in all analyses.

Wide Range Achievement Test 4 – Word Reading Subtest (WRAT-4)

The Word Reading subtest of the WRAT-4 (Wilkinson & Robertson, Reference Wilkinson and Robertson2006) was administered at baseline and used to calculate participants’ estimated intelligence.

Computerized Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children-IV (CDISC-IV)

The CDISC-IV (Shaffer et al., Reference Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan and Schwab-Stone2000) is a structured interview designed to assess symptoms and diagnoses of mental health disorders among youth within the last year, including generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and MDD. The CDISC-IV has previously demonstrated strong reliability and construct validity (Malgady et al., Reference Malgady, Rogler and Tryon1992). The current study used the total number of past year MDD symptoms at T5 as a main variable of interest.

Drug Use History Questionnaire (DUHQ)

The DUHQ (Gonzalez et al., Reference Gonzalez, Schuster, Mermelstein, Vassileva, Martin and Diviak2012; Rippeth et al., Reference Rippeth, Heaton, Carey, Marcotte, Moore, Gonzalez, Wolfson and Grant2004) is a semi-structured interview used to obtain a detailed self-report history of substance use during a participant’s lifetime, the past six months, and past 30 days. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use (i.e., number of days used), and lifetime amount of cannabis use (i.e., number of grams) were calculated at baseline, with past 6-month use further assessed at subsequent measurement waves. These were summed to obtain lifetime frequency of cannabis use and lifetime amount of cannabis use variables at the last assessment; we used the former as our main measure of cannabis use, while the latter was used for post hoc analyses.

Wechsler Memory Scale – Fourth Edition (WMS-IV): Logical Memory and Designs Subtests

Two subtests were used from the WMS-IV (Logical Memory and Design Memory) to assess episodic memory performance (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2009a). The Logical Memory subtest consists of two brief stories that are read aloud to the participant, who is then asked to recall the story immediately after each is presented (Logical Memory-I). After a 20- to 30-min delay the participants are again asked to recall the stories (Logical Memory-II).

The Designs subtest consists of four trials of abstract designs presented on a grid to the participants for 10 s. Participants are then asked to immediately select the designs from a set of cards and place the cards on the grid in the same place as previously shown (Designs-I). After a 20-min delay, participants are again asked to reproduce the designs on the grid (Designs-II). The age-corrected scaled scores from the immediate and delayed free recall trials of the Logical Memory and Designs subtests at T5 were first transformed to Z-scores, and then transformed into T-scores to be used as part of the outcome measures for the current study (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2009b). This was done so that all episodic memory measures were on the same scale to facilitate interpretation of findings.

California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition (CVLT-II)

The CVLT-II (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000) was used to assess episodic verbal learning and memory performance at T5. Each participant was presented with a list of 16 words over five trials (List A) and asked to recall as many words as possible after each list was read aloud (Immediate Free Recall). An interference list of 16 words was then read aloud for one trial (List B) and participants were asked to recall as many words from the interference list as possible. Participants were then asked to recall freely as many words from List A as possible. After a 20-min delay, participants were asked again to freely recall the words from List A (long-delay free recall). Normative data for the CVLT-II includes participants as young as 16 years old (Woods et al., Reference Woods, Delis, Scott, Kramer and Holdnack2006). Two participants from the current study were 15 years old, to which we applied the 16-year-old norms and controlled for age in analyses. CVLT-II forced choice recognition was examined using cutoff scores from Delis et al., (Reference Delis, Kramer, Kaplan and Ober2000). We found that only one participant had ≤ 14 correct responses on the forced choice recognition trial. Given that this participant scored correctly on 13 out of 16 responses on the forced choice recognition trial and did not appear to show signs of low effort throughout the protocol, their data were retained in the current study.

Episodic Memory Composites

For data reduction and to minimize Type I error, two composite variables were generated: Immediate Free Recall and Delayed Free Recall. Similar composites have been employed in previously published meta-analyses on cannabis use and neurocognition (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Gonzalez, Carey, Natarajan and Wolfson2003; Schreiner & Dunn, Reference Schreiner and Dunn2012). Specifically, a composite variable was created for immediate free recall by taking the average short-delay free recall T-scores from the WMS-IV Logical Memory and Designs Memory subtests and the average short-delay free recall scores from the CVLT-II to generate an average immediate free recall outcome variable. Similarly, a composite variable was created for delayed free recall by taking the average long-delay free recall T-scores from the WMS-IV Logical Memory and Designs Memory subtests and the average long-delay free recall scores from the CVLT-II to generate an average delayed free recall outcome variable.

Data analytic plan

All analyses were run in version 3.5 of PROCESS (Hayes, Reference Hayes2018). Age of the participants at T5 was added as a covariate in all primary analyses to control for effects of age on the outcome variables of interest. Additionally, a correlation matrix was run to examine potential variables associated with the immediate or delayed free recall outcome variables. Based on the results from the correlation matrix presented in Table 2, lifetime frequency of nicotine (number of days used) was also included as a covariate in all primary analyses. Assumptions for linear regression were evaluated and found that the linearity, normality of residuals, and independence of residuals’ assumptions were met (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Cohen, West and Aiken2002). However, evidence of heteroscedasticity was found after examination of scatter plots of residuals for both outcome variables. Therefore, bootstrapping was performed with robust estimators using 5,000 samples for all primary analyses.

Table 2. Correlations for possible covariates

Note. All correlations use Pearson’s r unless otherwise noted. For sex, 0 = female, 1 = male. Variables were not mean centered. MDD = major depressive disorder; GAD = generalized anxiety disorder; Alc = alcohol; Ni = nicotine; CU = cannabis use.

a Rows use Spearman’s rho correlation coefficients. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Two-tailed.

Two separate multiple linear regression models were run to examine the impact of lifetime frequency of cannabis use, past year MDD symptoms, and sex to predict immediate and delayed free recall. The first model included the variables of interest as well as all two-way interactions and the three-way interaction. The second model subsequently removed the three-way interaction and examined all two-way interactions with the variables of interest. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms were continuous variables that were mean centered prior to conducting analyses. All significant interactions were followed up with simple slopes difference tests (Aiken et al., Reference Aiken, West and Reno1991), with past year MDD symptoms set at one standard deviation above the mean (higher MDD symptoms ≥ 8.58 symptoms), at the mean (average MDD symptoms = 4.36 symptoms), and one standard deviation below the mean (lower MDD symptoms ≤ .13 symptoms). Subsequent multiple linear regression analyses were run in PROCESS to examine two-way interactions and main effects only after the three-way interaction was found to be non-significant.

Missing data procedures

A missing value analysis was conducted on the full sample (N = 401) and found that nine percent of the data were missing for the current study. Fourteen participants did not complete the final measurement wave which accounted for 3.5% of the missing data. The lifetime frequency of substance use variables including cannabis, alcohol, and nicotine accounted for the additional 5.5% of missingness. Mechanisms for missing data were analyzed using logistic regression. Both outcome variables (immediate free recall, delayed free recall) were coded into binary variables for missing and non-missing data (0 = not missing, 1 = missing). The binary outcome variables were then placed in a series of logistic regression analyses examining potential demographic, mental health, and substance use variables for predictors of missingness in the data. None of the variables placed in the logistic regression analyses were significant predictors of missingness and the missing data for the current study were presumed missing at random.

We then applied multiple imputation (MI) for dealing with missing data (Rubin, Reference Rubin1987) using 20 imputed datasets, and the analyses for each dataset were pooled. The results from the linear regression analyses were similar using MI and listwise deletion, so listwise deletion was used for all further analyses for easier interpretation. The final sample used for all analyses after exclusion of participants with missing data was 360 participants.

Results

We first analyzed results from the multiple linear regression for both outcome variables, with all variables of interest included in the model (see Table 3). The three-way interaction between lifetime frequency of cannabis use, past year MDD symptoms, and sex was not significant for either immediate (p = .260) or delayed (p = .203) free recall. However, a significant main effect of lifetime frequency of cannabis use was found on both immediate (p = .015) and delayed (p = .023) free recall, such that an increase in lifetime frequency of cannabis use (more days used) was associated with a decrease in both immediate and delayed free recall. Additionally, there was a main effect of sex on immediate (p = .042) free recall such that males performed better on immediate free recall compared to females. Additional information describing participant demographics and characteristics by sex can be found in Table S1 of the Supplemental Materials.

Table 3. Regression analyses with three-way interaction in model

Note. For sex, 0 = female, 1 = male. Confidence Intervals show lower and upper limits of the unstandardized beta coefficients. b = unstandardized beta coefficient; SE = Standard Error; CU = cannabis use; MDD = major depressive disorder; CI = confidence interval. *p < .05.

Subsequent multiple linear regression analyses revealed no significant two-way interactions between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and sex, or past year MDD symptoms and sex on episodic memory. Nonetheless, there were significant effects of the two-way interaction between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms on both immediate (p = .010) and delayed (p = .003) free recall (see Table 4). As shown in Figure 1, simple slopes difference tests revealed that lifetime frequency of cannabis use was significantly associated with poorer performance on immediate free recall at lower (p < .001), and average (p = .001) levels of MDD symptoms, but not at higher (p = .323) levels. Similarly, lifetime frequency of cannabis use was significantly associated with poorer performance on delayed free recall at lower (p < .001), and average (p = .002) levels of MDD symptoms, but not at higher (p = .592) levels (see Figure 2).

Table 4. Interaction between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms

Note. Confidence Intervals show lower and upper limits of the unstandardized beta coefficients. b = unstandardized beta coefficient; SE = Standard Error; CU = cannabis use; MDD = major depressive disorder; CI = confidence interval. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Figure 1. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms on immediate free recall.

Note. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and depression variables were not mean centered for easier interpretation. b = unstandardized beta coefficient; CU = cannabis use; MDD = major depressive disorder. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Figure 2. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and MDD symptoms on delayed free recall.

Note. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and depression variables were not mean centered for easier interpretation. b = unstandardized beta coefficient; CU = cannabis use; MDD = major depressive disorder. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Post hoc analyses

Post hoc exploratory analyses using ANOVA tests were performed by splitting the continuous lifetime frequency of cannabis use and continuous MDD variables into separate tertile groups to further examine results from the simple slopes difference tests. In contrast to the original analyses, the past year symptoms of MDD and lifetime frequency of cannabis use variables were converted from continuous to categorical variables for all post hoc exploratory analyses. Specifically, the past year MDD variable was split into tertiles to create low (≤ 2 symptoms), average (3–5 symptoms), and high (≥ 6 symptoms) groups. Additionally, the lifetime frequency of cannabis use variable was also split into tertiles to create low (≤ 21 days used), average (22 – 285 days used), and high (≥ 286 days used) groups. The MDD groups consisted of 150 participants in the low depression group (M = .75 symptoms), 91 participants in the average group (M = 3.76 symptoms), and 119 participants in the high group (M = 9.36 symptoms) based on the number of MDD symptoms endorsed in the past year. The lifetime frequency of cannabis use groups were created based on the number of cannabis use days endorsed in a participant’s lifetime. There were 120 participants in the low cannabis group (M = 4.39 days used), 118 participants in the average cannabis group (M = 120.81 days used), and 122 participants in the high cannabis group (M = 696.89 days used). We explored whether there were differences among the MDD groups in lifetime frequency of cannabis use (number of days used), or amount of cannabis used (number of grams) (i.e., whether the group with the highest number of MDD symptoms reported lower frequency of cannabis use or smaller amounts of cannabis used). We found no significant differences in lifetime frequency or amount of cannabis used among the MDD groups. Additionally, we explored whether the cannabis use groups differed significantly on the number of MDD symptoms (i.e., whether the group with the lowest frequency of cannabis use reported more MDD symptoms); however, no significant results were found.

Discussion

The current study examined the interactive effects of lifetime frequency of cannabis use, past year MDD symptoms, and sex on episodic memory in order to understand how MDD symptoms may influence the association between cannabis use and episodic memory among adolescents, and whether this association differs for males and females. Contrary to our hypotheses, we did not find a significant three-way interaction, indicating that the impact of MDD symptoms on the magnitude of the association between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and episodic memory did not differ for males or females.

However, we did find a significant association between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and episodic memory, such that as lifetime frequency of cannabis use increased (more days used), episodic memory performance decreased. This finding is in concordance with prior literature and further highlights the negative relationship between cannabis use and episodic memory (Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Tolomeo, Steele and Baldacchino2020; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Slomiak, Jones, Rosen, Moore and Gur2018). Previous literature has attempted to elucidate this finding by examining alterations in brain structure and function among adolescent cannabis users compared to controls. A previous study found that adolescent cannabis users had significantly smaller bilateral hippocampal volumes compared to non-using controls (Ashtari et al., Reference Ashtari, Avants, Cyckowski, Cervellione, Roofeh, Cook, Gee, Sevy and Kumra2011). However, despite the smaller hippocampal volumes, both the non-using controls and cannabis users performed similarly on a verbal memory task, suggesting an alteration in the functional mechanisms used among the cannabis users. It is important to note that all participants in the aforementioned study met diagnostic criteria for cannabis dependence (Ashtari et al., Reference Ashtari, Avants, Cyckowski, Cervellione, Roofeh, Cook, Gee, Sevy and Kumra2011). Further research is needed to examine possible neurobiological mechanisms underlying the association between subclinical adolescent cannabis use and episodic memory performance.

Furthermore, our results revealed a significant two-way interaction between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms on episodic memory. However, inconsistent with our hypotheses, this significant association was found only at lower (≤ .13 symptoms) and average (4.36 symptoms) levels of past year MDD symptoms, but not at higher (≥ 8.58 symptoms) levels. This finding indicates that at higher levels of MDD symptoms, the negative association between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and episodic memory diminished. To better understand these results, post hoc exploratory analyses using ANOVA tests were performed to examine results from the simple slopes difference tests. We wanted to explore whether the high lifetime cannabis use group (≥ 286 days used) or high MDD symptoms group (≥ 6 symptoms) revealed any significant differences relative to the average and low groups to account for the lack of significant association found between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and episodic memory performance at high levels of MDD symptoms. However, none of the exploratory analyses revealed significant differences between these tertile groups.

Nonetheless, examination of prior literature may provide insight into these results. Two prior studies have examined the interaction between cannabis use and depression on episodic memory (Radoman et al., Reference Radoman, Hoeppner, Schuster, Evins and Gilman2019, Roebke et al., Reference Roebke, Vadhan, Brooks and Levin2014). Both studies found main effects of MDD and cannabis use on episodic memory among adult and young adult samples; however, they did not find significant interactions between these variables. Radoman et al., (Reference Radoman, Hoeppner, Schuster, Evins and Gilman2019) only included participants in the depression group who either met lifetime criteria for MDD or had a current diagnosis of MDD (∼34% of participants met criteria for current MDD). Similarly, all participants in the depression group of the Roebke et al., (Reference Roebke, Vadhan, Brooks and Levin2014) study had a current diagnosis of either MDD or dysthymic disorder, demonstrating high rates of depression among these participant samples. Therefore, results from the current study are somewhat consistent with these prior study findings, illustrating that at higher levels of depression (i.e., ≥ 8.58 symptoms for current study; current or lifetime MDD diagnosis for prior studies), there is no significant interaction between cannabis use and episodic memory.

Nonetheless, the current study did find significant interactive effects at lower and average levels of depression. To our knowledge, these are novel findings within the literature, prompting future studies to replicate these results with additional adolescent samples. Furthermore, given the stringent exclusion criteria at baseline, only 4.4% of participants in the current study met diagnostic criteria for an MDE in the past year. In comparison, at the time of data collection for the current study in 2015, 12.5% of adolescents in the United States experienced an MDE in the past year according to the Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality (2016). The considerably lower rates of MDE from the current sample suggests future studies should examine how both clinical and subclinical levels of depression influence cannabis use and its effects on episodic memory among adolescents.

The current study also examined the influence of sex differences on the interaction between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms on episodic memory among adolescents. Given that prior literature has found greater depression rates among female compared to male adolescents (Ferrari et al., Reference Ferrari, Somerville, Baxter, Norman, Patten, Vos and Whiteford2013; Salk et al., Reference Salk, Hyde and Abramson2017), as well as greater adverse impacts on episodic memory among female cannabis users compared to male cannabis users (Crane et al., Reference Crane, Schuster and Gonzalez2013), we hypothesized that the impact of MDD symptoms on the association between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and episodic memory would be stronger among female than male adolescents. Contrary to our hypotheses, the three-way interactive effect was not significant. This again might be explained by the low rates of depression in our sample and that these associations may vary as a function of sex only among samples with clinical levels of depression. However, a significant main effect of sex was found for immediate free recall, such that males showed a slight advantage over females. This finding is inconsistent with prior literature that has demonstrated a female advantage among tasks of episodic memory in both adolescents and adults (Asperholm et al., Reference Asperholm, Högman, Rafi and Herlitz2019; Boman, Reference Boman2004; Herlitz & Rehnman, Reference Herlitz and Rehnman2008; Kramer et al., Reference Kramer, Delis, Kaplan, O’Donnell and Prifitera1997). Sex differences in age of cannabis use onset may partially explain our finding. A previous study by Crane et al., (Reference Crane, Schuster, Mermelstein and Gonzalez2015) found that earlier age of first use of cannabis was associated with poorer immediate free recall for female compared to male young adults. Given that the participants in the current sample were between 15–19 years of age and had been participating in the larger parent study for two years prior to T5, it is plausible that the mean age of first use for our sample is similar or possibly younger than the mean age of first use (M = 15.80) reported in the Crane et al. (Reference Crane, Schuster, Mermelstein and Gonzalez2015) study. Though age of first use of cannabis may be a confounding variable influencing our findings with sex, particularly given the higher rates of lifetime cannabis use among males than females (see Table S1), age of first use was not controlled for in the current study and should be examined in future research to determine whether it is driving these associations.

Results from the current study should be interpreted considering the following limitations. First, the cross-sectional design of the study prevents the ability to determine timing and causality. Additionally, exclusion of participants at baseline with a history of significant psychological disorders may limit generalizability of the results to adolescents with subclinical or clinical MDD symptoms. As previously noted, only 4.4% of participants in the current sample met criteria for a past year MDE, compared to 12.5% of adolescents in the United States who met criteria for a past year MDE (Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, 2016). Similarly, due to the limited number of participants reporting symptoms of MDD in the sample, the current study focused on symptoms of MDD in the past year instead of current symptoms of MDD. Future studies may attempt to replicate findings from the current study among adolescent samples with higher rates of reported MDD. Additionally, we note that episodic memory was examined as a composite variable in the current study, future studies may reveal different results when examining distinct sub-components of episodic memory such as visual and auditory memory, or the various indices from the CVLT. It is also worth noting that post hoc exploratory analyses did not correct for Type I error; however, none of the post hoc analyses revealed significant results. Furthermore, there are many potential confounding variables that we were not able to examine in the current study, including cannabis potency, method of use, cannabis use age of onset, and ratio of cannabidiol (CBD) to delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), all of which could conceivably influence results. Finally, practice effects may have impacted results given that participants had completed tests of episodic memory on two prior occasions, each one year apart; it is therefore possible that memory tests were less sensitive over time. However, we did find a significant main effect for cannabis use on memory performance.

In summary, findings from the current study extend prior literature on adolescent cannabis use and episodic memory by showcasing the moderating role of MDD symptoms. Although further work is needed to better understand the influence of subclinical and clinical levels of depression symptoms, our findings inform treatment efforts focused on improving episodic memory function by tailoring approaches based on an individual’s history of cannabis use and depression symptoms.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561772300005X.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by R01 DA031176 & U01DA041156 (PI: R.G.), and F31 DA047750-01A1 (PI: Pacheco-Colón), and T32DA043449-02 (PI: Pelham) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Funding statement

None.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Ahern, E., & Semkovska, M. (2017). Cognitive functioning in the first-episode of major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 31, 5272. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000319 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aiken, L. S., West, S. G., & Reno, R. R. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage.Google Scholar
Arain, M., Haque, M., Johal, L., Mathur, P., Nel, W., Rais, A., Sandhu, R., & Sharma, S. (2013). Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 9, 449461. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S39776 Google ScholarPubMed
Ashtari, M., Avants, B., Cyckowski, L., Cervellione, K. L., Roofeh, D., Cook, P., Gee, J., Sevy, S., & Kumra, S. (2011). Medial temporal structures and memory functions in adolescents with heavy cannabis use. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 10551066. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asperholm, M., Högman, N., Rafi, J., & Herlitz, A. (2019). What did you do yesterday? A meta-analysis of sex differences in episodic memory. Psychological Bulletin, 145. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000197 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Avenevoli, S., Swendsen, J., He, J. P., Burstein, M., & Merikangas, K. R. (2015). Major depression in the national comorbidity survey-adolescent supplement: Prevalence, correlates, and treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 3744.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.10.010 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Harms, M. P., Tillman, R., Hawkey, E., & Luby, J. L. (2019). Early childhood depression, emotion regulation, episodic memory, and hippocampal development. Journal of abnormal psychology, 128, 8195. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000392 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 296312. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blest-Hopley, G., Colizzi, M., Giampietro, V., & Bhattacharyya, S. (2020). Is the adolescent brain at greater vulnerability to the effects of cannabis? A narrative review of the evidence. Frontiers in psychiatry, 11, 859. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00859 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blest-Hopley, G., Giampietro, V., & Bhattacharyya, S. (2020). A systematic review of human neuroimaging evidence of memory-related functional alterations associated with cannabis use complemented with preclinical and human evidence of memory performance alterations. Brain Sciences, 10, 102. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10020102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boman, E. (2004). The effects of noise and gender on children’s episodic and semantic memory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 45, 407416. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2004.00422.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broyd, S. J., van Hell, H. H., Beale, C., Yücel, M., & Solowij, N. (2016). Acute and chronic effects of cannabinoids on human cognition - A systematic review. Biological Psychiatry, 79, 557567. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality. (2016). Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2015 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (HHS Publication No. SMA 16-4984, NSDUH Series H-51). http://www.samhsa.gov/data/ Google Scholar
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G. & Aiken, L. S. (2002). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge, New York.Google Scholar
Crane, N. A., Schuster, R. M., & Gonzalez, R. (2013). Preliminary evidence for a sex-specific relationship between amount of cannabis use and neurocognitive performance in young adult cannabis users. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 19, 10091015. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771300088X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crane, N. A., Schuster, R. M., Mermelstein, R. J., & Gonzalez, R. (2015). Neuropsychological sex differences associated with age of initiated use among young adult cannabis users. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 37, 389401. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1020770 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test- Second Edition (CVLT-II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Ferrari, A. J., Somerville, A. J., Baxter, A. J., Norman, R., Patten, S. B., Vos, T., & Whiteford, H. A. (2013). Global variation in the prevalence and incidence of major depressive disorder: A systematic review of the epidemiological literature. Psychological Medicine, 43, 471481. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712001511 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figueiredo, P. R., Tolomeo, S., Steele, J. D., & Baldacchino, A. (2020). Neurocognitive consequences of chronic cannabis use: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 108, 358369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghetti, S., & Bunge, S. A. (2012). Neural changes underlying the development of episodic memory during middle childhood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 381395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.05.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez, R., Schuster, R. M., Mermelstein, R. J., Vassileva, J., Martin, E. M., & Diviak, K. R. (2012). Performance of young adult cannabis users on neurocognitive measures of impulsive behavior and their relationship to symptoms of cannabis use disorders. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34, 962976. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.703642 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J., Fisher, C., Hetrick, S., Phillips, L., Parrish, E. M., & Allott, K. (2018). Neurocognitive functioning in depressed young people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology Review, 28, 216231. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-018-9373-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, I., Gonzalez, R., Carey, C. L., Natarajan, L., & Wolfson, T. (2003). Non-acute (residual) neurocognitive effects of cannabis use: A meta-analytic study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 9, 679689. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617703950016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis (2nd ed.). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2008). Sex differences in episodic memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 5256. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00547.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, L. D., Miech, R. A., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E., & Patrick, M. E., (2021). Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975-2020: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Kramer, J. H., Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., O’Donnell, L., & Prifitera, A. (1997). Developmental sex differences in verbal learning. Neuropsychology, 11, 577584. https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.11.4.577 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malgady, R. G., Rogler, L. H., & Tryon, W. W. (1992). Issues of validity in the Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 26, 5967. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(92)90016-h CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, L. M., & Ebmeier, K. P. (2009). A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. Journal of Affective Disorders, 119, 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mojtabai, R., Olfson, M., & Han, B. (2016). National trends in the prevalence and treatment of depression in adolescents and young adults. Pediatrics, 138, Article e20161878. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1878 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pacheco- Colón, I., Hawes, S. W., Duperrouzel, J. C., & Gonzalez, R. (2021). Evidence lacking for cannabis users slacking: A longitudinal analysis of adolescent cannabis use and motivation. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 27, 637647. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561772000096X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radoman, M., Hoeppner, S. S., Schuster, R. M., Evins, A. E., & Gilman, J. M. (2019). Marijuana use and major depressive disorder are additively associated with reduced verbal learning and altered cortical thickness. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 19, 10471058. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00704-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rippeth, J. D., Heaton, R. K., Carey, C. L., Marcotte, T. D., Moore, D. J., Gonzalez, R., Wolfson, T., Grant, I., & HNRC Group (2004). Methamphetamine dependence increases risk of neuropsychological impairment in HIV infected persons. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 10, 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704101021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roebke, P. V., Vadhan, N. P., Brooks, D. J., & Levin, F. R. (2014). Verbal learning in marijuana users seeking treatment: A comparison between depressed and non-depressed samples. The American Journal of Drug & Alcohol Abuse, 40, 274279. https://doi.org/10.3109/00952990.2013.875551 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, D. B. (1987). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York, NY: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salk, R. H., Hyde, J. S., & Abramson, L. Y. (2017). Gender differences in depression in representative national samples: Meta-analyses of diagnoses and symptoms. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 783822. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreiner, A. M., & Dunn, M. E. (2012). Residual effects of cannabis use on neurocognitive performance after prolonged abstinence: a meta-analysis. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20, 420429. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, J. C., Slomiak, S. T., Jones, J. D., Rosen, A., Moore, T. M., & Gur, R. C. (2018). Association of cannabis with cognitive functioning in adolescents and young adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 75, 585595. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0335 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., & Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children Version IV (NIMH DISC- IV): Description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 2838. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2009a). Wechsler memory scale: WMS-IV: Technical and interpretive manual. London, England: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (2009b). Wechsler Memory Scales – Fourth Edition (WMS-IV): Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, Texas: Pearson Clinical Assessment.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2006). WRAT 4: Wide range achievement test; Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Woods, S. P., Delis, D. C., Scott, J. C., Kramer, J. H., & Holdnack, J. A. (2006). The California verbal learning test—second edition: Test retest reliability, practice effects, and reliable change indices for the standard and alternate forms. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 413420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant demographics and characteristics at time point 5 (T5)

Figure 1

Table 2. Correlations for possible covariates

Figure 2

Table 3. Regression analyses with three-way interaction in model

Figure 3

Table 4. Interaction between lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms

Figure 4

Figure 1. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and past year MDD symptoms on immediate free recall.Note. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and depression variables were not mean centered for easier interpretation. b = unstandardized beta coefficient; CU = cannabis use; MDD = major depressive disorder. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Figure 5

Figure 2. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and MDD symptoms on delayed free recall.Note. Lifetime frequency of cannabis use and depression variables were not mean centered for easier interpretation. b = unstandardized beta coefficient; CU = cannabis use; MDD = major depressive disorder. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Supplementary material: File

Lehman et al. supplementary material

Table S1

Download Lehman et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.8 KB