Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:40:19.848Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Speed as a dimension of manner in Estonian frog stories

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 December 2022

Piia Taremaa*
Affiliation:
Institute of Estonian and General Linguistics, University of Tartu, Jakobi 2–446, 51005 Tartu, Estonia
Johanna Kiik*
Affiliation:
Institute of Estonian and General Linguistics, University of Tartu, Jakobi 2–446, 51005 Tartu, Estonia
Leena Karin Toots*
Affiliation:
Institute of Estonian and General Linguistics, University of Tartu, Jakobi 2–446, 51005 Tartu, Estonia
Ann Veismann*
Affiliation:
Institute of Estonian and General Linguistics, University of Tartu, Jakobi 2–446, 51005 Tartu, Estonia

Abstract

Focusing on the expression of manner and path in the ‘frog story’ narrations of Estonian native speakers, this study shows that Estonian – a morphologically rich satellite-framed Finno-Ugric language – is characterised by high manner and high path salience. Furthermore, when analysing one of the core qualities of manner – speed – we show that when the participants were asked to narrate a story as if the events developed slowly, they also spoke slowly and their stories tended to be long (both in time duration and word count) and include many details. When they were asked to tell the story as if the events developed fast, they also spoke faster and used more verbs of caused motion and verbs of vertical motion. Thus, the speed of motion in the physical world seems to be mimicked by speech rate, indicating mental simulation and iconic prosody. The exact nature of speed effects in linguistic choices for expressing motion remains to be studied in future works.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nordic Association of Linguists

1. Introduction

Recent decades have substantially expanded our knowledge about the linguistic realisation of motion events within and across languages. Even though the domain of motion has long attracted linguists’ attention (Mirambel Reference Mirambel1950, Tesnière Reference Tesnière1959, Ikegami Reference Ikegami1969, Hughes Reference Hughes1970, to name just a few), interest in the expression of motion got a boost from Talmy’s (Reference Talmy1972, Reference Talmy1975, Reference Talmy and Shopen1985, Reference Talmy2000b, Reference Talmy and Shopen2007) typology of motion events. In this typology, Talmy divided languages into verb- and satellite-framed languages based on how they encode the core component of motion: Path (for the most recent comprehensive treatment of motion events, see Talmy Reference Talmy2000b and Reference Talmy and Shopen2007). Path, in turn, is defined as ‘the path followed or site occupied by the Figure object with respect to Ground object’ (Talmy Reference Talmy2000b:25). If a language expresses Path predominantly in the verb (e.g. the Spanish path verb entrar ‘enter’), it is a verb-framed language (e.g. Spanish). If a language mainly expresses Path outside the verb with a so-called satellite (e.g. a verbal particle such as out), it is a satellite-framed language (e.g. English).

Early studies of motion events in the vein of Talmy mainly dealt with establishing the typological profile of individual languages based on their expression of path and manner (e.g. Aske Reference Aske1989, Choi & Bowerman Reference Choi and Bowerman1991, Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996), but they soon triggered an avalanche of studies focusing on the fine-grained differences between and within languages in their lexical choices when expressing motion in relation to their main profile of lexicalisation patterns (e.g. Berthele Reference Berthele and Kortmann2004, Slobin Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Zlatev & Yangklang Reference Zlatev, Yangklang, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Filipović Reference Filipović2007, Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009, Goschler & Stefanowitsch Reference Goschler and Stefanowitsch2013, Fagard et al. Reference Fagard, Stosic and Cerruti2017). Recently, more and more in-depth analyses have been conducted on the dimensions of motion descriptions that go beyond the general categories of path and manner (e.g. Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano2017, Matsumoto & Kawachi Reference Matsumoto and Kawachi2020, Stosic Reference Stosic2020, Kopecka & Vuillermet Reference Kopecka and Vuillermet2021, Łozińska Reference Łozińska2021, Montero-Melis Reference Montero-Melis2021, Tuuri Reference Tuuri2021), which significantly broadens the scope of studies of motion language.

The current study adds to this growing body of knowledge by applying a well-established data elicitation task – ‘frog stories’ – in EstonianFootnote 1 and focusing on a primary dimension of motion: speed. The term ‘frog stories’ refers to a narration task in which speakers tell a story based on the wordless picture book Frog, Where Are You? (Mayer Reference Mayer1969). As a data elicitation task, it has been used in linguistics for nearly forty years (see also Berman & Slobin Reference Berman and Isaac Slobin1994) and has proved to be an excellent tool for obtaining comparable data from various languages (Strömqvist & Verhoeven Reference Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Guo et al. Reference Guo, Elena Lieven, Ervin-Tripp, Nakamura and Özçalişkan2009). Importantly, this experiment is useful for studying and establishing both the path salience and manner salience of individual languages (Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996, Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009). Path salience shows the extent to which spatial aspects of motion are elaborated upon in language (Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009). Manner salience shows the extent to which a language details manner-related aspects (Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996, Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004). Both of these saliences are typically measured by means of the frog stories task. Thus, to relate Estonian data to cross-linguistic data and to establish Estonian degree of path salience and manner salience, we applied the same task.Footnote 2

As for speed, it is one of the main dimensions of manner relating to any motion event. This is because motion is always characterised by how slowly or fast it progresses. In this paper, we define speed as a characteristic that shows how fast or slowly a mover changes its location in space. It is a sub-category of manner, the latter of which can be very broadly defined as the way motion is conducted (for in-depth treatments of the manner dimensions, see e.g. Cardini Reference Cardini2008, Slobin et al. Reference Slobin, Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Kopecka and Majid2014, Stosic Reference Stosic, Aurnague and Stosic2019, Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022). Thus, the encoding of speed is a matter of encoding manner. Needless to say, speed is essentially a non-linguistic notion that can be expressed in language by various devices (e.g. verbs, adverbs, larger constructions). As such, speed is a prime domain to investigate the language-cognition interface. We address speed here in the light of embodiment approaches suggesting that language is grounded in perception and action (Johnson Reference Johnson1989, Glenberg & Kaschak Reference Glenberg and Kaschak2002, Gibbs Jr Reference Gibbs2006, Barsalou Reference Barsalou2008, Zwaan Reference Zwaan2009, Pulvermüller Reference Pulvermüller2013). We understand embodiment as mental simulation in that the use of language evokes sensorimotor simulation similar to performing the described action, and physical experiences, in turn, influence the structure of language (for a recent overview of embodiment and mental simulation, see Speed et al. Reference Speed, Vinson, Vigliocco, Dąbrowska and Divjak2019).

The further rationale for focusing on speed in this study is driven by its central relevance to motion (Ikegami Reference Ikegami1969, Slobin et al. Reference Slobin, Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Kopecka and Majid2014) and is also supported by the fact that motion verbs tend to form a continuum from those expressing very slow to those expressing very fast motion (Taremaa Reference Taremaa2017). Furthermore, fast motion tends to have more lexical resources in terms of adverbs and adjectives in many languages (Dixon Reference Dixon1982, Plungian & Rakhilina Reference Plungian and Rakhilina2013, Hallonsten Halling Reference Hallonsten Halling2018, Schäfer Reference Schäfer2020), which indicates the fast-over-slow asymmetry (Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022). A recent corpus study on Estonian motion verbs (Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2023) further suggests that manner verbs expressing fast motion (e.g. kihutama ‘dash’) have somewhat distinct clausal patterns from manner verbs expressing slow motion (e.g. lonkima ‘stroll, saunter’), and they often resemble goal-oriented path verbs (e.g. suunduma ‘head’) in their constructional behaviour.

Taking these aspects into account, the current study has two aims:

  1. (i) To situate Estonian in a broader cross-linguistic context in its expression of motion, and particularly so regarding its manner salience and path salience.

  2. (ii) To reveal any substantial differences between Estonian linguistic descriptions of motion events that evolve slowly and events that evolve fast.

Section 2 provides the background and rationale for these two study goals relating to (i) manner salience and path salience and (ii) speed as an essential dimension of motion. Section 3 describes the frog stories experiment and data coding. Based on these data, Section 4.1 contextualises the Estonian language amongst languages that are high-manner and high-path-salient. Section 4.2 addresses the study’s second goal regarding speed effects. This subsection shows that such effects occur in the length and rate of narrations, and in the expression of manner-related information, whereas spatial information is – at least based on the frog stories data – not affected by speed. Section 5 discusses the results in the light of cross-linguistic findings and then addresses the impact of the elicitation tool on the results we achieved.

2. Background

In this section, we will first elaborate on the two clines of salience as proposed in the literature. Then we will discuss the issues related to the expression of speed. Finally, the linguistic inventory of expressing motion in Estonian is briefly described.

2.1 Manner salience and path salience

Our first goal relates to cross-linguistic differences of manner salience and path salience. In the literature, these are known as the two clines of salience that reveal differences between languages in expressing motion. Manner salience, as suggested by Slobin (Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996, Reference Slobin, Hickmann and Robert2006), stands for a language’s tendency to express manner-related information frequently and in a fine-grained way. Broadly divided, manner can be expressed by verbs resulting in manner verbs (e.g. jooksma ‘run’ in Estonian) and by other expressions (e.g. aeglaselt ‘slowly’ in Estonian). These other expressions are often termed ‘manner modifiers’ and they are understood to cover all expressions in a clause (apart from verbs) that specify how motion is conducted. Speakers of a manner-salient language have easy and quick access to their large mental lexicon of manner verbs, and they detail manner-related aspects frequently in language. This tendency for manner-richness characterises satellite-framed languages, whereas in verb-framed languages, expressing manner is much more optional (Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996, Reference Slobin, Hickmann and Robert2006).

Manner salience has been measured by the size of the manner lexicon and usage frequency of manner verbs and modifiers. The data in earlier crosslinguistic studies by Slobin (Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996, Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Reference Slobin, Hickmann and Robert2006) suggest that, for example, Spanish and French have low manner salience, German and English are manner-salient, and Russian is an extremely manner-salient language (Slobin Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004). A recent improvement in research on manner salience comes from Akita and Matsumoto (Reference Akita, Matsumoto, Matsumoto and Kawachi2020), who compared manner salience in English and Japanese by examining the fine-grained distinctions of manner with a special focus on sound as a dimension of manner. Based on two experiments (one of which was a frog stories task and the other was a video-based elicitation task), they concluded that English is more manner-salient than Japanese.

Estonian is a satellite-framed language that exhibits a large set of manner verbs and expresses manner-related information frequently and in a nuanced way (Pajusalu et al. Reference Pajusalu, Neeme Kahusk, Veismann, Vider, Õim, van der Zee and Vulchanova2013, Taremaa Reference Taremaa2017, Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022). This suggests that along the cline of manner salience (Slobin Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Reference Slobin, Hickmann and Robert2006), Estonian can potentially be situated amongst the languages that are highly manner-salient.

Path salience, as described and investigated by Ibarretxe-Antuñano (Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009; see also Ibarretxe-Antuñano & Hijazo-Gascón Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Hijazo-Gascón, Filipović and Jaszczolt2012), refers to the language’s ability to express path-related information in a fine-grained way. It is measured by the presence of spatial expressions other than the verb and its satellite (e.g. prefix, verbal particle). Languages that routinely express path outside the verb and its possible satellite (by so-called plus-ground clauses: Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996) are understood to be path-salient languages. The notion of path salience was proposed by Ibarretxe-Antuñano (Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009), who based the cline of salience on a number of studies including her own. In her 2009 paper, she presented 21 languages and showed that high-path-salient languages include, for example, Basque and Swedish, but also English. Low-path-salient languages include, for example, West Greenlandic, Tagalog, and Chinese.

Building upon the cline of salience, the following languages have more recently found their place in this cline of path salience: Jaminjung (an Australian language), which is inclined towards the low-path-salient languages with its close to 40% of plus-ground clauses (Hoffmann Reference Hoffmann, Filipović and Jaszczolt2012), and Ilami Kurdish as a language that uses approximately 50% plus-ground clauses (Karimipour & Rezai Reference Karimipour and Rezai2016). Furthermore, in Finnish frog stories, plus-ground clauses were used in 87% of clauses (Pasanen & Pakkala-Weckström Reference Pasanen, Pakkala-Weckström, Garant, Helin and Yli-Jokipii2008). This finding places Finnish within the most high-path-salient languages.

Moreover, as can be seen from these examples, path salience is not strictly correlated to the typological profile of a language (i.e. satellite-framed or verb-framed) nor to its manner salience, as both verb-framed and satellite-framed languages can be path-salient. For instance, amongst the languages of high path salience, Basque is a verb-framed language and Swedish and English are satellite-framed languages. However, an important factor that is associated with a language’s degree of path salience is its lexical and morphological richness (Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009, Ibarretxe-Antuñano & Hijazo-Gascón Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Hijazo-Gascón, Filipović and Jaszczolt2012). That is, high-path-salient languages tend to have a large lexical and morphological inventory to express space. These characteristics apply to Estonian as well. Thus, similarly to its kindred language Finnish, we can also expect that Estonian is a high-path-salient language.

2.2 Speed of motion as a dimension of manner

Our second goal – to establish if there are any principal differences between describing slow and fast motion – is concerned with a specific manner dimension that characterises any motion event: speed. More specifically, we aim to determine whether the encoding of space and manner is influenced by the speed of described motion.

Differences in speed can be expressed through lexical choices, such as verbs of fast vs. slow motion (compare kihutama ‘dash’ and lonkima ‘stroll’) or adverbs of fast vs. slow motion (compare kiiresti ‘fast’ and aeglaselt ‘slowly’). Moreover, it has been shown for Estonian motion verbs that speakers attribute to them speed meanings so that the verbs fill the continuum from slow to fast verbs (Taremaa Reference Taremaa2017). Regarding adverbs and adjectives, studies have shown that the lexicon of fast motion adverbs and adjectives is much larger than that of slow motion in a number of languages (Ikegami Reference Ikegami1969, Dixon Reference Dixon1982, Plungian & Rakhilina Reference Plungian and Rakhilina2013, Hallonsten Halling Reference Hallonsten Halling2018). This suggests the predominance of explicit expression of fast motion. Based on written corpus data, a similar asymmetry has been shown to occur in Estonian: manner modifiers of fast motion are almost five times more frequent than those of slow motion, and they are also more diverse in terms of their lexical inventory and morphosyntactic realisation (Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022). Thus, the fast-over-slow bias has been suggested (Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022).

Furthermore, this preliminary investigation (Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022) suggests that the expression of fast motion is more prone to redundancy in that speed is frequently conveyed by both the verb and the manner modifier (e.g. ta kihutas ruttu koju [(s)he rush.pst.3sg fast home] ‘(s)he rushed home fast’; Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022). The expression of slow motion is much more flexible in that verbs of slow motion can easily be combined not only with manner modifiers of slow motion (e.g. ta roomas aeglaselt [(s)he crawl.pst.3sg slowly] ‘(s)he was crawling slowly’) but also with modifiers of fast motion (e.g. ta roomas kiiresti [(s)he crawl.pst3sg fast] ‘(s)he was crawling fast’). Finally, verbs of fast motion in Estonian occur frequently in combination with Goal expressions similarly to goal-oriented path verbs (compare ta kihutas koju [(s)he rush.pst.3sg home] ‘(s)he rushed home’ and ta suundus koju [(s)he head.pst.3sg home] ‘(s)he headed home’, making manner and path verbs similar in terms of their preferable clausal patterns; Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2023).

2.3 Linguistic inventory to express motion in Estonian

Estonian is a Finno-Ugric language spoken by approximately one million people. Structurally, it is very similar to Finnish in terms of its morphosyntactic richness (for a general overview of Estonian, see Erelt Reference Erelt2003; for more detailed accounts of Estonian, see Tauli Reference Tauli1973, Reference Tauli1983, Erelt & Metslang Reference Erelt, Erelt and Metslang2017, Viht & Habicht Reference Viht and Habicht2019). In terms of its typological profile in Talmy’s (Reference Talmy2000b) sense, it is a prime example of a satellite-framed language (Pajusalu et al. Reference Pajusalu, Neeme Kahusk, Veismann, Vider, Õim, van der Zee and Vulchanova2013).

Relevant to spatial language, Estonian has motion verbs that can occur either as bare verbs (e.g. jooksma ‘run’) or in combination with a satellite (adverb as a verbal particle), forming particle verbs (also termed as phrasal verbs, e.g. välja jooksma ‘run out’). The transparency of particle verbs varies from full idiomaticity (e.g. peale käima [lit. onto walk] ‘insist’) to full transparency (e.g. välja jooksma ‘run out’). The line between verbal particles and free adverbs is vague (see also Rätsep Reference Rätsep1978, Veismann & Sahkai Reference Veismann and Sahkai2016, Aedmaa Reference Aedmaa2019). Motion verbs can also occur as a part of other complex verbs, such as catenative verbs (e.g. hakkab jooksma [start.prs.3sg run.inf] ‘(s)he starts running’), serial verbs (e.g. läheb Footnote 3 jookseb [go.prs.3sg run.prs.3sg] ‘(s)he goes and runs’), and idiomatic phrasal verbs (e.g. jalga laskma [leg.ill let/shoot.inf] ‘escape, run away’). Verbal morphology includes various categories, such as tense, person, number, voice, and mood. The sentences from the current study’s experiment exemplify these categories (see (1)) in that all the verbs are in indicative mood, personal voice, and third person plural. They differ in tense, in that the verbs in (1a) and (1c) are in the present tense and the one in (1b) is in the simple past tense.

In the nominal sphere, nouns in Estonian can be inflected in 14 cases, including six spatial cases known as interior cases (i.e. in-cases) and exterior cases (i.e. on-cases). The former consists of illative, inessive, and elative. The latter includes allative, adessive, and ablative. In addition to these, terminative also encodes spatial information by expressing motion until something. An example of a case-inflected spatial expression is in (1a), where Goal is expressed by a noun inflected in illative case (vette ‘into the water’). In addition, there are adpositions in Estonian (they mostly occur as postpositions) and a large proportion of these express spatial information, as the postposition peale ‘onto’ in (1b) and the preposition üle ‘over’ in (1c). Frequently, the same spatial lexical items can function as verbal particles (e.g. alla jooksma ‘run down’) and adpositions (e.g. laua alla [desk.gen under.goal] ‘under the desk’).

From the perspective of the central semantic notions relating to motion, ‘path’ can be expressed by verbs. In this study, all verbs that predominantly encode directional information are considered to be path verbs. This also includes deictic verbs such as minema ‘go’ and tulema ‘come’ (see also Levin Reference Levin1993). Apart from verbs, path can be expressed by satellites (i.e. a morpheme closely related to the verb) which in Estonian are verbal particles (e.g. välja (jooksma) ‘(run) out’). The semantically defined term ‘manner’ can represent various types of linguistic realisations. In Estonian, adverb phrases (e.g. kiiresti ‘fast’), noun phrases (e.g. kiire-l sammu-l [fast-ade step-ade] ‘at fast pace’), and gerund forms (e.g. kiirusta-des [hurry-ger] ‘running’) are most commonly used to express manner (besides verbs which are then called ‘manner verbs’).

Furthermore, word order is relatively free in Estonian, noun phrases can be long and complex, and clauses can simultaneously incorporate a number of spatial or manner expressions. In a constructed example (2), an event is described by a manner-of-motion verb (jooksma ‘run’) in combination with a Location, Source, Trajectory, Goal, and Manner expression. In this example, the expression of Source illustrates a lengthy noun phrase, and the expression of Trajectory illustrates an adpositional phrase.

As can be seen in (2), Estonian allows combinations of manner verbs to express boundary-crossing events, which is a typical characteristic of satellite-framed languages (see also Aske Reference Aske1989, Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996). Despite the predominance of the satellite-framing strategy and rich inventory of manner verbs and modifiers, Estonian routinely also uses verb-framing strategies, as exemplified in (1c) by the path verb suunduma ‘head’.

All in all, Estonian is a morphologically rich and flexible satellite-framed language. Being morphology-rich, Estonian is likely to be a highly path-salient language. Similarly, being a satellite-framed language, Estonian is also likely to be a highly manner-salient language. It is this high degree of manner salience that allows speed as a dimension of manner to be expressed in a nuanced way in Estonian.

3. Method and data

In this section, we describe (i) the implementation of the frog stories task, (ii) the data coding decisions, and (iii) the statistical techniques used to analyse the data.

3.1 Method

Participants. The experiment was conducted with 45 adult participants (39 female, 5 male, 1 non-binary). All participants were native speakers of Estonian. The mean age of the participants was 26 years (SD 10; range 19−60). Participants were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions of the experiment: A (Control Condition), B (Slow Condition), or C (Fast Condition). Each condition had an equal number of participants (N = 15).

Materials and design. To collect data, we asked the participants to narrate a story based on the wordless picture book Frog, Where Are You? (Mayer Reference Mayer1969) following the experiment design of Berman and Slobin (Reference Berman and Isaac Slobin1994). With the introduction of video clips as experimental stimuli where the variability of motion events can be captured and presented in a more natural way (e.g. Vuillermet & Kopecka Reference Vuillermet, Kopecka, Lahaussois and Vuillermet2019, Lewandowski Reference Lewandowski2021, Matsumoto et al. Reference Matsumoto, Kimi Akita, Kiyoko Eguchi, Miho Mano, Morita, Nagaya, Takahashi, Sarda and Fagard2022), the frog stories design seems to have been used less often in cross-linguistic research over the past ten years. However, this task is a basic elicitation task for measuring a language’s degree of manner salience and path salience. Thus, we chose the frog stories design to obtain comparable data for Estonian for two main reasons. Firstly, there are a number of languages for which frog stories have been used to examine the expression of motion. Secondly, as the task elicits descriptions of path and manner in the scenes of horizontal and vertical motion, the frog stories design was deemed to be a prime tool for preliminary investigation of speed effects in language.

The pictures of the frog story book were digitised and presented to the participants on a computer screen. In addition to the standard experiment in which participants can narrate the story as they wish (Berman & Slobin Reference Berman and Isaac Slobin1994), we tested two more conditions. Control Condition corresponds to the standard design in that the participants were asked to narrate the story based on the pictures as they saw fit. As such, the participants of this condition serve as a control group. Slow Condition and Fast Condition were designed to elicit speed-related language of slow and fast motion respectively. In Slow Condition, the participants were told to follow the pictures and tell the story as if the events evolved very slowly (the exact wording of the instructions can be found in the Appendix). In Fast Condition, the participants were told to tell the story as if the events evolved very fast. The picture stimuli were identical across the three conditions. The only difference between the conditions was in the instructions given to the participants.

Procedure. The experiments were conducted in the Phonetics Lab at the University of Tartu where the narrations were audio-recorded. The participants’ task was to narrate a story based on the sequence of frog story pictures. The instructions (see the Appendix) were given orally as well as on the computer screen. The pictures were presented on the computer screen, with one picture per slide. Prior to the task, the participants could go through all the pictures and ask questions. When narrating, they could change the slides at their own pace. They sat alone in the recording studio but could ask questions from the researcher using a microphone during the experiment (no participant used this opportunity, though; all the participants narrated the story without communicating with the researcher).

3.2 Data

The audio data were automatically transcribed using the Estonian speech transcription system of the Tallinn University of Technology (Alumäe, Tilk & Asadullah Reference Alumäe, Muischnek and Müürisep2018), and then manually checked and corrected. The written utterances were entered into a spreadsheet with each clause placed in a separate row. Clauses were defined as chunks of text in which a finite verb occurs together with all other sentential units associated with it (see also Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996). Occasionally, when a motion event was described with a converb construction, it was also considered to be a clause (e.g. akna-st alla kukku-des [window-ela down fall-ger] ‘falling down the window’). After that, the data (i.e. each clause) were coded for motion-related variables, verb-related variables, and variables of space and manner. In a separate spreadsheet, the length of the narrations (by minutes and clauses) and the participants’ speech rate were automatically coded. In the following subsections, all relevant variables are explained in detail alongside their general frequencies in our data.

3.2.1 Variables characterising the narrations

The narrations are captured by five variables (see Table 1). They stand for the length of the narrations in terms of speech time (TotalSpeechTimeInMinutes), number of clauses produced by a participant (ClausesPerParticipant), number of motion clauses produced by a participant (MotionClausesPerParticipant), speech rate (WordsPerSecond), and experiment condition (Condition).

Table 1. Variables describing the whole data of the narrations

3.2.2 Variables of motion

Two variables of motion were tagged: Motion and MotionType (see Table 2). Motion specifies whether the clause depicts motion or not. If coded as ‘yes’, we have a motion clause. This variable enables us to examine motion descriptions of translational motion while leaving the rest of the clauses out. Importantly, in line with earlier studies using frog stories (e.g. Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996, Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009), we only consider translational motion, which is understood as motion in which the mover changes their position in space by moving entirely from one point to another (see also Talmy Reference Talmy2000b:35–36). Descriptions of activities that comprise motion but in which motion is not the main purpose (e.g. searching) are not analysed as motion descriptions in our study (for a different and broader approach, see Pasanen and Pakkala-Weckström Reference Pasanen, Pakkala-Weckström, Garant, Helin and Yli-Jokipii2008). Descriptions of self-contained motion (i.e. motion in which the mover stays in the same location), including expressions of moving one’s hand or leg, were excluded from motion clauses (i.e. they were labelled as ‘no’ or ‘unclear’). The rest of the variables of this study presented below were only coded if Motion was coded as ‘yes’.

Table 2. Motion variables of general type

MotionType (see Table 2) was coded for the purposes of distinguishing between ‘self-motion’, in which the mover is the sole main participant expressed and motion can be agentive or non-agentive (e.g. ta jookseb välja [run.prs.3sg out] ‘(s)he is running out’, ta kukub alla [fall.prs.3sg down] ‘(s)he falls down’; not to be confused with self-contained motion as explained above), and ‘caused motion’, in which the motion of an entity is caused by another entity (e.g. öökull ajab poissi taga [owl.nom drive.prs.3sg boy.part behind] ‘the owl is chasing the boy’).

3.2.3 Verb-related variables

Verb-related variables stand for the form and meaning of verbs used in the motion clauses (see Table 3). Four variables were coded: Verb, VerbTypeSem, VerbTypeMorhpSynt, and Particle.

Table 3. Verb-related variables coded in motion clauses

Verb refers to the verb lemmas without their optional verbal particles (e.g. minema ‘go’, kukkuma ‘fall’). Regarding the semantic type of motion verbs (VerbTypeSem), the main distinction was made between path and manner verbs. In our analysis, path verbs lexicalise directional meanings (e.g. minema ‘go’, suunduma ‘head’) and manner verbs lexicalise how motion is conducted (e.g. jooksma ‘run’, ronima ‘crawl’, hüppama ‘jump’). Because deictic verbs express directional information, they were analysed as path verbs. In addition to the two main types of motion verbs, we coded path+manner verbs as verbs that saliently express both directional and manner meanings, making their classification into discrete categories of path or manner verbs difficult, if not impossible. This mainly concerns verbs of vertical motion (e.g. kukkuma ‘fall’). The label ‘unclear’ was assigned to verbs that we were unable to classify unambiguously. This category exclusively contains verbs of caused motion (e.g. ajama ‘chase, drive’, võtma ‘take’).

VerbTypeMorhpSynt distinguishes between bare verbs and particle verbs. Verbs occurring with verbal particles (i.e. satellites, e.g. alla kukkuma ‘fall down’) are coded as ‘particle verb’. All other verbs are coded as ‘bare verb’ (e.g. kukkuma ‘fall’). Particle specifies the verbal particle (if present). The coding of verbal particles followed Estonian reference grammars (Erelt et al. Reference Erelt, Kasik, Metslang, Rajandi, Ross, Saari, Tael and Vare1993, Reference Erelt, Erelt, Metslang, Rajandi, Ross, Saari, Tael and Vare1995, Erelt Reference Erelt, Erelt and Metslang2017).

3.2.4 Variables of space and manner

Variables of space and manner specify the semantic structure of motion clauses (see Table 4). The main variable – Ground – was taken from Slobin (Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996), who, in turn, used the term in the vein of Talmy (Reference Talmy2000b). Talmy (Reference Talmy2000b:25) defines Ground as a ‘reference object’ with respect to where the Figure object is located or moving. To differentiate clauses in which only the verb (together with its optional particle, i.e. satellite) was used to express spatial settings of motion from those in which Ground was elaborated upon outside the verb, Slobin applied the terms minus-ground clauses and plus-ground clauses, respectively. In our analysis of the Estonian data, ‘plus-ground clauses’ are coded if space is expressed as Source, Location, Trajectory, Direction, or Goal. Otherwise, ‘minus-ground clauses’ are coded. If a category such as Source or Direction is expressed with a verbal particle, it is not considered a plus-ground clause. Other variables of space include Source, Location, Trajectory, Direction, and Goal. In defining Manner, we rely on previous research (Cardini Reference Cardini2008, Slobin et al. Reference Slobin, Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Kopecka and Majid2014, Stosic Reference Stosic, Aurnague and Stosic2019, Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022) and define it as pertaining to various dimensions of the way in which a mover progresses that, one way or another, relate to the body-movements of a moving object.

Table 4. Clause-related variables of space and manner in motion clauses

3.3 Statistical tools

In analysing the data, we use descriptive statistics and frequency analysis. Even though our study is predominantly exploratory, we have chosen simple frequency analysis techniques. This enables us to relate our results to results obtained by similar previous studies, and to present plots that are reader-friendly and easy to interpret. We also applied statistical tests when examining the manifestation of a variable with respect to the experimental conditions, to better account for differences that are statistically significant. In particular, we applied independent two-tailed Wilcoxon tests for analysing continuous variables (this test was chosen because our data are not normally distributed) and Chi-square tests for analysing categorical variables. The latter are accompanied by Cramér’s V to account for the effect sizes of the associations. The data were analysed and the figures were created in R using the packages ‘base’ (R Core Team 2020), ‘dplyr’ (Wickham et al. Reference Wickham, François, Henry and Müller2020), ‘sjPlot’ (Lüdecke Reference Lüdecke2021), ‘ggplot2’ (Wickham et al. Reference Wickham, Winston Chang, Thomas Lin Pedersen, Claus Wilke, Yutani and Dunnington2021), and ‘ggpubr’ (Alboukadel Reference Alboukadel2020). The coded data and R code are available through the data repository DataDOI.Footnote 4

4. Results

In this section, we will first establish the degree to which Estonian is a manner-salient and path-salient language. For this purpose, we examine the data from Control Condition and analyse the same subsets of the data as in previous studies that address manner salience and path salience in languages. After that, we will address any possible speed effects in frog stories by comparing narrations (and motion clauses in particular) of the three conditions: Control, Slow, and Fast Condition.

4.1 Manner salience and path salience in Estonian

We hypothesised that Estonian is a high-manner-salient and high-path-salient language based on its framing profile (satellite-framed language) and linguistic inventory (morphosyntactic richness: see Section 2.1). As a measure of manner salience, we applied a similar approach to that used by Akita and Matsumoto (Reference Akita, Matsumoto, Matsumoto and Kawachi2020) and calculated the proportion of all manner expressions (i.e. manner verbs and modifiers) in motion clauses that the participants produced. To compare our results with Akita and Matsumoto (Reference Akita, Matsumoto, Matsumoto and Kawachi2020:151), we only examine clauses of translational self-motion in Control Condition (304 clauses in total) and also exclude verbs of vertical motion (e.g. fall) from manner verbs. As explained in Section 3.2.3, to best represent the semantics of the verbs of vertical motion, we call verbs of vertical motion ‘path+manner verbs’. As such, we only analyse clauses that describe self-motion that is translational and horizontal (234 clauses in total).

The results indicate that roughly half of the clauses specify manner of motion one way or another: manner is expressed either by a verb (as by ronima ‘climb’ in (3a)), modifier (as by vaikselt ‘quietly’ in (3b)), or both (as by the verb jooksma ‘run’ and modifier suure hooga ‘with great speed’ in (3c)) in 125 clauses (53%), and is not expressed in 109 clauses (47%). This indicates that Estonian is a high-manner-salient language. In comparison, the results obtained by Akita and Matsumoto (Reference Akita, Matsumoto, Matsumoto and Kawachi2020:153) for English and Japanese show that 42% of clauses contained a manner expression in the English data and 27% of clauses contained a manner expression in the Japanese data. If considering only manner verbs (i.e. leaving manner modifiers aside) in the Estonian data, we find that they occur in 115 clauses of self-motion out of the total of 234 (49%; see examples (3a, 3c)). This result positions Estonian close to English, as according to Slobin’s (Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004:231) data, manner verbs were used in English frog stories in approximately 45% of clauses.

Furthermore, if we analyse the proportion of manner verbs used in depicting the owl’s exit (see Slobin Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004), we can see that path verbs (used in 7 clauses, as in (4); 78%) are preferred over manner verbs. In fact, manner verbs are particularly infrequent (they are used only in 2 clauses; 22%), but given that the number of clauses expressing the owl’s exit in our data is extremely small (only 9 clauses in total in Control Condition), these results should be interpreted with caution. When comparing with Slobin’s (Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004:225) cross-linguistic data, Estonian would be similar to Dutch and German. In these two languages, manner verbs were used in close to 20% of clauses describing the owl’s exit. Roughly 30% of clauses contained manner verbs in Slobin’s (Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004) English data. Thus, along the cline of manner salience, Estonian can be situated close to the Germanic languages, between German and English.

To relate our study to crosslinguistic findings on path salience, we present the results for falling scenes similarly to Ibarretxe-Antuñano & Hijazo-Gascón (Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Hijazo-Gascón, Filipović and Jaszczolt2012) and follow Slobin’s (Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996:200–201) distinction between minus- and plus-ground clauses. As explained in Sections 2.1 and 3.2.4, minus-ground clauses refer to constructions in which the verb is not accompanied by an additional spatial expression (excluding verbal particles). Plus-ground clauses refer to constructions in which the verb occurs in a clause with an additional spatial expression of a ground other than the verb and its optional particle. In this analysis, we include both self-motion and caused motion (i.e. 356 clauses), of which clauses of falling scenes in Control Condition occur in 63 instances in total. The results reveal that the control group produced minus-ground clauses in 19% (N = 12) and plus-ground constructions in 81% (N = 51) of all motion clauses that describe the falling scenes (N = 63). These two structures are exemplified in (5a) by a minus-ground clause in which alla kukkuma ‘fall down’ is used and in (5b) by a plus-ground clause in which the same particle verb combines with a Source expression (aknast ‘from the window’). The high proportion of plus-ground structures (81%) suggests that along the cline of path salience (Ibarretxe-Antuñano & Hijazo-Gascón Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Hijazo-Gascón, Filipović and Jaszczolt2012:354), Estonian is a language of high-path salience following Chantyal (plus-ground 100%), Basque (88%), and English (82%).

To summarise, Estonian is a high-manner and high-path-salient language. In terms of its use of manner verbs and modifiers in horizontal self-motion (altogether in 53% of clauses), Estonian is more manner-salient than English (for which Akita and Matsumoto (Reference Akita, Matsumoto, Matsumoto and Kawachi2020) report such usage in 42% of clauses). If considering only manner verbs (used in 49% of clauses), Estonian is similar to English (where they were used in 45% of clauses, according to Slobin Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004). This suggests that it is the frequent use of manner modifiers that makes Estonian a particularly high-manner-salient language.

As for path salience, we found that in the falling scenes of the frog stories, plus-ground constructions were used in 81% of clauses. In these clauses, the verb (and its optional particle) co-occurred with an additional spatial expression (e.g. a noun phrase expressing the source of motion). This indicates that not only is Estonian a high-path-salient language, but it is also similar to English in this respect (as reported by Ibarretxe-Antuñano (Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009), English used plus-ground constructions in 82% of clauses). In comparison, Finnish has been reported to have plus-ground constructions in 87% of clauses. However, it should be considered that in the analysis by Pasanen & Pakkala-Weckström (Reference Pasanen, Pakkala-Weckström, Garant, Helin and Yli-Jokipii2008), all motion clauses from all scenes were included while some motion clauses were excluded in the current study (similarly to Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009).

4.2 Speed effects in the Estonian frog stories

Our second aim was to establish any possible speed effects in expressing motion. We measure the speed effects by means of (i) the length of the narrated stories and speech rate of the participants, (ii) lexical choices of motion verbs, and (iii) clausal characteristics of motion descriptions.

4.2.1 Narrations across the conditions

Below, we examine the general characteristics of the data across the three conditions (see Figures 1 and 2). The stories in Slow Condition tend to be the longest and those in Fast Condition the shortest in terms of average speech time in minutes and the number of clauses produced by a participant (see panels A and B in Figure 1). The stories in Control Condition are in between the two. An independent two-tailed Wilcoxon testFootnote 5 confirms that speech time in minutes relative to Control Condition is significantly longer for Slow Condition (W = 42, p < 0.01) and significantly shorter for Fast Condition (W = 165, p = 0.03). Similarly, as can be inferred from panel B in Figure 1, the length of the narrations in terms of clauses relative to Control Condition is greater for Slow Condition (W = 58, p = 0.02), but not significantly smaller for Fast Condition (W = 142.5, p = 0.21). The difference between Slow and Fast Condition in length is significant (W = 184, p < 0.01). The same pattern is reflected for the number of motion clauses produced by the participants (Control vs. Slow: W = 60.5, p = 0.03; Control vs. Fast: W = 127, p = 0.55; Slow vs. Fast: W = 180.5, p < 0.01; see panel C in Figure 1).

Figure 1. Panel A: length of the narrations produced by the participants in three conditions measured in minutes. Panel B: length of the narrations in the number of clauses in total. Panel C: the number of motion clauses. The horizontal lines indicate median values. The diamond figures stand for mean values.

Figure 2. Speech rate of the narrators across the conditions. The horizontal lines indicate median values. The diamond figures stand for mean values.

Furthermore, the speech rate is fastest in Fast Condition and slowest in Slow Condition, as shown in Figure 2. However, an independent two-tailed Wilcoxon test indicates that the number of words per second relative to Control Condition is not significantly smaller for Slow Condition (W = 136, p = 0.35) and also not significantly larger for Fast Condition (W = 66, p = 0.06). Nevertheless, there is a significant difference between Slow and Fast Conditions (W = 49, p < 0.01).

Taken together, speed effects manifest themselves in the length of narrations and in the speech rate. The stories told in Slow Condition were considerably longer and narrated at a slower pace than those in Fast Condition.

4.2.2 Lexical diversity of motion verbs across the conditions

The list of all verbs that occurred in motion clauses (whether depicting self-motion or caused motion) together with their optional particles (satellites) is given in the Supplemental Materials. The top five verbs in terms of their absolute frequencies are presented in Table 5 (bare verbs) and Table 6 (particle verbs). In all conditions, the path verb minema ‘go’ is the most frequently used bare verb and alla kukkuma ‘fall down’ (path+manner verb with a verbal particle) the most frequently used particle verb. As for differences, kukkuma ‘fall’ as a verb of vertical motion does not appear amongst the most frequent verbs in Slow Condition, and alla kukkuma ‘fall down’ is less frequent in Slow Condition than in the other conditions.

Table 5. The five most frequent bare verbs across the three conditions (absolute frequencies)

Table 6. The five most frequent particle verbs across the three conditions (absolute frequencies)

The frequencies of types and tokens of motion verbs (regardless of whether they occurred with or without particles) across the conditions are presented in Table 7. It shows that the number of different verbs (types) is highest in Slow Condition. This is to be expected because the stories narrated in Slow Condition were much longer than those in Control and Fast Conditions. When we look at the mean frequencies of tokens per type, it appears that Fast Condition is somewhat more diverse in its verb choice (approximately 5.6 tokens were used per type) than Control and Slow Conditions (approximately 6.6 and 5.9 tokens per type, respectively) with Control Condition being least diverse. In other words, the participants used the same verbs most frequently in Control Condition and least frequently in Fast Condition.

Table 7. The frequencies of the types and tokens of motion verbs (without their optional particles) used by the narrators

As for semantic verb types used to express motion (i.e. path vs. manner verbs), significant differences appear across the conditions, as shown in Figure 3.

Figure 3. The distribution of verbs across three conditions: manner verbs (= ‘manV’), path+manner verbs (= ‘path+manV’), path verbs (= ‘pathV’), neutral verbs (= ‘neutrV’), and verbs of ambiguous semantics (= ‘unclear’).

In particular, when the participants were asked to pay attention to the speed of motion to tell the story as if the events developed slowly (Slow Condition) or fast (Fast Condition), they used not only more diverse verbs compared to the control group (see Table 7) but also used manner verbs more frequently than the control group (see Figure 3), as in (6).

The comparison of Slow and Fast Conditions indicates that the participants in Slow Condition used path+manner verbs less frequently than those in Fast Condition (and Control Condition). The use of a path+manner verb in Fast Condition is exemplified in (7a) by kukkuma ‘fall’, where it combines with an onomatopoetic manner expression plärtsti ‘with a splash’ which further adds information about forceful and fast motion. Moreover, the participants in Fast Condition used path verbs less frequently than those in Slow and Control Condition. This suggests that speakers in Slow Condition elaborated extensively upon horizontal motion, but they described the scenes of vertical motion (where the path+manner verbs would be needed) as little as possible. In Fast Condition, there are many uses of verbs of caused motion (in terms of expressing path or manner, these are labelled as ‘unclear’: see Section 3.2.3) that describe not only fast but also forceful motion, as exemplified in (7b).

All in all, verb choice in Slow and particularly in Fast Condition was more diverse than in the control group. Furthermore, manner verbs were used more frequently in Slow and Fast Conditions than in Control Condition. Particularly in Fast Condition, verbs of caused motion tended to be used.

4.2.3 Clausal characteristics of motion descriptions across the conditions

In this section, we concentrate on the semantic makeup of motion descriptions and measure how spatial aspects as well as manner features are expressed in the narrations. To start with spatial aspects, the frequencies of plus-ground and minus-ground clauses are provided in panel A in Figure 4. This essentially shows whether the verb is accompanied by a spatial expression other than its optional particle (plus-ground clauses) or not (minus-ground clauses), as explained in Section 3.2.4. The proportions of these two types of clausal structures show no differences between the three conditions. That is, we see no speed effects here.

Figure 4. The characteristics of all motion clauses across three conditions in terms of (i) the frequencies of minus-ground (= ‘minusGr’) and plus-ground clauses (= ‘plusGr’; panel A) and (ii) the expression of spatial categories (panel B).

As for the presence of spatial expressions apart from the verbs, the same result is obtained (see panel B in Figure 4). In other words, speakers in different conditions select the spatial aspects to be described rather uniformly, which is most likely related to the specifics of the elicitation tool (see Section 5).

Regarding manner modifiers (see Figure 5), Slow Condition triggered more frequent mentions of how motion is conducted (i.e. manner expressions other than the verb) than Control and Fast Conditions.

Figure 5. The presence (= ‘yes’) or absence (= ‘no’) of manner modifiers across three conditions.

For instance, in (6), manner is expressed as aeglaselt ja võimalikult vaikselt ‘slowly and as quietly as possible’. If we add to this that manner verbs were also most frequently used in Slow Condition (see Figure 3), we can generalise that Slow Condition is manner-biased. The narrations in Slow Condition were the longest across the three conditions. This may suggest that the participants not only took more time to tell their story but also applied manner expressions as a convenient tool to describe the pictures (which mostly depicted rather fast motion) so that the motion would be described more slowly than it was depicted in pictures. In Fast Condition, the participants presumably relied mainly on expressing speed through motion verbs, and to save time, they omitted as many manner modifiers as possible. Nevertheless, as can be seen in (7a), in which the onomatopoetic adverb plärtsti ‘with a splash’ is used, manner modifiers are possible in Fast Condition and particularly so if they convey the speed or forcefulness of motion.

5. Discussion

In this study, we set out to contribute to cross-linguistic knowledge about motion events by establishing the degree to which Estonian – a Finno-Ugric and satellite-framed language – is manner- and path-salient. Our second goal was to focus on an underlying dimension of manner – speed – to determine any linguistic differences between fast and slow motion. To do this and to directly relate our results to previous cross-linguistic findings, we used frog stories as a data elicitation task.

As for the two clines of saliences, along the cline of manner salience (Slobin Reference Slobin, Shibatani and Thompson1996, Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Reference Slobin, Hickmann and Robert2006, Akita & Matsumoto Reference Akita, Matsumoto, Matsumoto and Kawachi2020), Estonian is a high-manner-salient language with manner being extensively expressed by verbs and other expressions. Along the cline of path salience (Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009, Ibarretxe-Antuñano & Hijazo-Gascón Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Hijazo-Gascón, Filipović and Jaszczolt2012), Estonian can, again, be placed amongst the most high-path-salient languages. These two findings are not surprising. This is because satellite-framed languages tend to be manner-salient (Slobin Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004) and morphology-rich languages tend to be path-salient (Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano and Guo2009), and Estonian is both satellite-framed and morphology-rich. A question for future research would be exactly how the various characteristics of a language (morphological richness, manner salience, and path salience) interact with each other and with the language’s degree to which it is satellite-framed or verb-framed (regarding variation in languages’ typological profiles, see e.g. Berthele Reference Berthele and Kortmann2004, Strömqvist & Verhoeven Reference Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004, Goschler & Stefanowitsch Reference Goschler and Stefanowitsch2013, Ibarretxe-Antuñano Reference Ibarretxe-Antuñano2017).

Speed effects in the frog story data manifest themselves mainly in the length of the narrations and speech rate of the participants. That is, the participants who were told to narrate the frog story as if the events evolved slowly, tended to tell longer stories and at a slower pace than those of the control group. Those who were asked to tell the story as if the events evolved quickly, narrated shorter stories and had a faster speech rate. This indicates that speed effects occur in the granularity of the discourse (if slow motion is expressed, more attention is paid to describing the details, making the descriptions lengthy) and in suprasegmental features of language in terms of speech rate. The speech rate, in turn, may be taken as evidence of embodiment (e.g. Johnson Reference Johnson1989, Gibbs Jr Reference Gibbs2006, Barsalou Reference Barsalou2008, Fischer & Zwaan Reference Fischer and Zwaan2008) in that speakers talk faster if they describe events that evolve fast and more slowly if they describe slowly evolving events. In other words, language is grounded in action and perception in that speakers mimic the perceived motion rate by their speech rate (see also Speed & Vigliocco Reference Speed and Vigliocco2014).

Speech rate varying in relation to event speed can also be seen as an instantiation of iconic prosody. In fact, several studies that were conducted in line with iconic prosody have shown the same effect in speech rate (Shintel, Nusbaum & Okrent Reference Shintel, Nusbaum and Okrent2006, Shintel & Nusbaum Reference Shintel and Nusbaum2008, Perlman et al. Reference Perlman, Clark and Falck2015, Fuchs et al. Reference Fuchs, Savin, Solt, Ebert and Krifka2019). For instance, studies of Shintel et al. (Reference Shintel, Nusbaum and Okrent2006) and Shintel and Nusbaum (Reference Shintel and Nusbaum2008) showed a correlation between speech rate and speed of event, and Perlman et al. (Reference Perlman, Clark and Falck2015) found similarly that English speakers read stories of fast motion faster and stories of slow motion more slowly. In later research, Fuchs et al. (Reference Fuchs, Savin, Solt, Ebert and Krifka2019) provided converging evidence to these findings from spoken language in a written form (blog texts) in which the word slow was more readily lengthened by means of letter replication than the word fast. Speed effects in iconic prosody, in turn, has been explained as a consequence of speed and speech rate being ‘correlated in experience’ (Perlman et al. Reference Perlman, Clark and Falck2015:1360).

As for speed effects on clausal patterns, we found that when people were asked to describe slow events (i.e. as if the events progressed slowly), their narrations were particularly manner-rich. When people had a task to describe fast motion events (i.e. as if the events progressed fast), narrations entailed frequent use of verbs of vertical motion as well as those of caused motion. Thus, providing fine-grained manner information in narrations to describe slow motion seems to be a convenient tool to make motion sound slow. Again, a parallel can be drawn from physical motion in that one can notice much more detail when moving slowly whereas one can observe the surroundings less when moving fast. Nevertheless, it should be noted that all three experimental conditions showed Estonian as a manner-salient satellite-framed language. The differences between the conditions in highlighting variable facets of a highly complex category of manner indicate discourse- and task-related characteristics. In particular, fastness of motion foregrounded manner dimensions related to force dynamics (being closely related to caused motion and vertical motion); slowness foregrounded manner qualities related to horizontal motion.

This indicates that, firstly, manner as a context-sensitive domain should essentially be analysed not only as a general category but from the perspective of its individual closely related dimensions, such as body-movements, force, effort, and speed (see also Narasimhan Reference Narasimhan2003, Cardini Reference Cardini2008, Slobin et al. Reference Slobin, Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Kopecka and Majid2014, Stosic Reference Stosic2020, Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2022). Secondly, language-internal variation in expressing motion events is sentient to various factors including those relating to speech context and task characteristics, all of which can be labelled as discourse-related factors. The impact of discourse on motion language has already been highlighted by Slobin (Reference Slobin, Strömqvist and Verhoeven2004), but has nevertheless received limited attention in linguistics with most studies focusing on word- or clause-level phenomena. Finally, language-internal variation in motion events is closely related to whether self-motion or caused motion is expressed, as shown also by Lewandowski for German, Polish, and Spanish (Reference Lewandowski2021). These two types of motion, in turn, are related to force dynamics as put forward by Talmy (Reference Talmy1988, Reference Talmy2000a), whereas force dynamics itself is closely associated with the direction of motion in terms of horizontality and verticality (see also Glenberg & Kaschak Reference Glenberg and Kaschak2002). Force dynamics is also associated with speed, as evidenced by the current study. Thus, motion events and their linguistic encoding are a complex phenomenon that should ultimately be addressed through the lens of high-dimensional data analysis with discourse factors taken into account.

Apart from the expression of manner, we found no evidence for differences between the three conditions in terms of encoding spatial information, whether considering plus- and minus-ground constructions or the expression of spatial categories such as Source and Goal. This may be due to the visual stimuli that the participants were told to use when narrating the stories. Because the major locations relevant to the actions were depicted in the pictures (e.g. the window out of which the dog fell or the pond where the boy and the dog were thrown), it is likely that these ground objects were salient for the participants and, in order to create a story, essential aspects to describe regardless of the speed at which the events evolved. This suggestion is supported by the fact that across all conditions, Location and Trajectory were rarely mentioned, which does not reflect the general tendencies of describing space as measured in corpus studies (e.g. Pajusalu et al. Reference Pajusalu, Neeme Kahusk, Veismann, Vider, Õim, van der Zee and Vulchanova2013, Taremaa Reference Taremaa2017, Taremaa & Kopecka Reference Taremaa and Kopecka2023).

This leads us to discuss the elicitation task. The main advantages of using the frog stories is that it enables us to draw cross-linguistic conclusions when eliciting path- and manner-sensitive data. The disadvantage of the frog stories is that the depicted scenes are rather Source- and Goal-oriented, and most of the depicted motion events could be interpreted as having fast and forceful motion. Furthermore, it has been argued that stories produced based on such visual stimuli are not as natural as free narrations (Klamer & Moro Reference Klamer and Moro2020). Therefore, one must be cautious when interpreting the results and particularly so concerning spatial language. For example, our experiment indicates that a large number of clauses contained Direction or Goal expressions followed by Source expressions. This could be taken as evidence of the goal-over-source bias (Ikegami Reference Ikegami, Dirven and Radden1987, Dirven & Verspoor Reference Dirven and Verspoor1998), whereas in fact the high number of such clauses is simultaneously also a consequence of the elicitation task. In addition, because the pictures depict rather fast motion, this might have put the participants of Slow Condition into the difficult situation of using language of slow motion to describe pictures that depict fast motion. This difficulty, in turn, could have resulted in slower processing speed affecting the speech rate and lexical choices of the speakers in Slow Condition. To confirm whether there is an internal link between the speed of an event and how this event is linguistically encoded, more thorough future research is needed.

6. Conclusion

We used the frog stories elicitation task to examine Estonian in the context of manner salience and path salience studies and to reveal any speed effects in motion descriptions. As expected from Estonian being a satellite-framed and morphologically rich language, our results situate Estonian in the clines of manner salience and path salience amongst languages that display high manner salience and path salience. The expression of manner-related information was particularly frequent when the participants were asked to narrate the frog story as if the events developed slowly. Furthermore, we attested embodiment effects which can also be analysed in line with iconic prosody. Namely, the length of the stories and speech rate correlated with the experimental task. The participants who narrated the story as if the events evolved slowly told substantially longer stories and spoke more slowly than those who narrated the story as if the events evolved fast.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586522000245

Acknowledgements

This study has been supported by the research fund of Kadri, Nikolai, and Gerda Rõuk, by the Estonian Research Council grant (PSG671) and by the European Union through the European Regional Development Fund (Centre of Excellence in Estonian Studies).

Appendix. Instructions to the participants in the frog stories experiment

Control Condition

1st slide

Sinu ülesanne on jutustada pildiseeria järgi lugu. Kõigepealt saad kõik pildid järgemööda läbi vaadata, kokku on 24 pilti. Kasutades nooleklahve, saad pilte edasi-tagasi kerida. Kui oled pildid läbi vaadanud ning sa midagi küsida ei soovi, algab katse. Jutusta lugu, lähtudes piltidel kujutatust. Pilte saad edasi kerida nooleklahviga. Jutustamise ajal väldi palun piltide tagasikerimist. Kui oled valmis alustama, vajuta nooleklahvi.

‘Your task is to tell a story according to a series of pictures. First, you can review all the pictures. There are 24 pictures in total. Use the arrow keys to scroll the images back and forth. Once you’ve reviewed the pictures and don’t want to ask anything, the experiment will begin. Tell a story based on the pictures. You can scroll through the pictures with the arrow keys. Please avoid rewinding pictures during narration. When you are ready to start, press the arrow key.’

26th slide (presented after the frog story pictures)

Kas sul tekkis pärast piltide läbivaatamist katse kohta küsimusi?

‘After reviewing the images, do you have any questions about the experiment?’

27th slide (after which the pictures are presented again, one picture per slide)

Algab katse. Jutusta lugu nii, nagu ise soovid, ent lähtu piltidel kujutatust. Pilte saad edasi kerida nooleklahviga. Palun väldi piltide tagasikerimist.

‘The experiment begins. Tell the story as you wish, but follow the pictures. You can scroll through the pictures with the arrow keys. Please avoid rewinding the pictures.’

Slow Condition

1st slide

Sinu ülesanne on jutustada pildiseeria järgi lugu. Kõigepealt saad kõik pildid järgemööda läbi vaadata, kokku on 24 pilti. Kasutades nooleklahve, saad pilte edasi-tagasi kerida. Kui oled pildid läbi vaadanud ning sa midagi küsida ei soovi, algab katse. Jutusta lugu nii, nagu toimuks kõik tegevused väga aeglaselt, ent lähtu piltidel kujutatust. Pilte saad edasi kerida nooleklahviga. Jutustamise ajal väldi palun piltide tagasikerimist. Kui oled valmis alustama, vajuta nooleklahvi.

‘Your task is to tell a story according to a series of pictures. First, you can review all the pictures. There are 24 pictures in total. Use the arrow keys to scroll the images back and forth. Once you’ve reviewed the pictures and don’t want to ask anything, the experiment will begin. Tell the story as if all the activities were taking place very slowly, but follow the pictures. You can scroll through the pictures with the arrow keys. Please avoid rewinding pictures during narration. When you are ready to start, press the arrow key.’

26th slide (presented after the frog story pictures)

Kas sul tekkis pärast piltide läbivaatamist katse kohta küsimusi?

‘After reviewing the images, do you have any questions about the experiment?’

27th slide (after which the pictures are presented again, one picture per slide)

Algab katse. Lähtu piltidel kujutatust ning jutusta lugu nii, nagu toimuks kõik tegevused väga aeglaselt. Pilte saad edasi kerida nooleklahviga. Palun väldi piltide tagasikerimist.

‘The experiment begins. Based on the pictures, tell the story as if all the activities were taking place very slowly. You can scroll through the pictures with the arrow keys. Please avoid rewinding the pictures.’

Fast Condition

1st slide

Sinu ülesanne on jutustada pildiseeria järgi lugu. Kõigepealt saad kõik pildid järgemööda läbi vaadata, kokku on 24 pilti. Kasutades nooleklahve, saad pilte edasi-tagasi kerida. Kui oled pildid läbi vaadanud ning sa midagi küsida ei soovi, algab katse. Jutusta lugu nii, nagu toimuks kõik tegevused väga kiiresti, ent lähtu piltidel kujutatust. Pilte saad edasi kerida nooleklahviga. Jutustamise ajal väldi palun piltide tagasikerimist. Kui oled valmis alustama, vajuta nooleklahvi.

‘Your task is to tell a story according to a series of pictures. First, you can review all the pictures. There are 24 pictures in total. Use the arrow keys to scroll the images back and forth. Once you’ve reviewed the pictures and don’t want to ask anything, the experiment will begin. Tell the story as if all the activities were taking place very fast, but follow the pictures. You can scroll through the pictures with the arrow keys. Please avoid rewinding pictures during narration. When you are ready to start, press the arrow key.’

26th slide (presented after the frog story pictures)

Kas sul tekkis pärast piltide läbivaatamist katse kohta küsimusi?

‘After reviewing the images, do you have any questions about the experiment?’

27th slide (after which the pictures are presented again, one picture per slide)

Algab katse. Lähtu piltidel kujutatust ning jutusta lugu nii, nagu toimuks kõik tegevused väga kiiresti. Pilte saad edasi kerida nooleklahviga. Palun väldi piltide tagasikerimist.

‘The experiment begins. Based on the pictures, tell the story as if all the activities were taking place very fast. You can scroll through the pictures with the arrow keys. Please avoid rewinding the pictures.’

Footnotes

1 In the glossed examples of Estonian, Leipzig Glossing Rules are followed. The abbreviations used are as follows:

2 In linguistics, a number of elicitation tools have been used to examine spatial language in general and motion descriptions in particular. The tools include various questionnaires (many of which can be found in the webpage of Max Planck Institute, https://www.eva.mpg.de/lingua/tools-at-lingboard/questionnaires.php, accessed 21 June 2022), picture-based narration tasks and various video-based tasks (such as Pear Stories (Chafe Reference Chafe1980) as an older and Trajectoire (Vuillermet & Kopecka Reference Vuillermet, Kopecka, Lahaussois and Vuillermet2019) as a more recent video-based elicitation tool), and other more sophisticated experimental means that measure the processing or production of motion language (e.g. Kaschak et al. Reference Kaschak, Madden, Therriault, Yaxley, Mark Aveyard, Blanchard and Zwaan2005, Lindsay et al. Reference Lindsay, Scheepers and Kamide2013).

3 Läheb ‘goes’ is the suppletive form of the verb minema ‘go’.

4 The data and statistical code are available in DataDOI: https://datadoi.ee/handle/33/487.

5 Because the data are not normally distributed, a non-parametric test is used.

References

Aedmaa, Eleri. 2019. Detecting Compositionality of Estonian Particle Verbs with Statistical and Linguistic Methods (Dissertationes Linguisticae Universitatis Tartuensis 37). Tartu: University of Tartu Press.Google Scholar
Akita, Kimi & Matsumoto, Yo. 2020. A fine-grained analysis of manner salience: Experimental evidence from Japanese and English. In Matsumoto, Yo & Kawachi, Kazuhiro (eds.), Broader Perspectives on Motion Event Descriptions (Human Cognitive Processing 69), 143180. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.69.05akiCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alboukadel, Kassambara. 2020. ggpubr: ‘ggplot2’ based publication ready plots. R package version 0.4.0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggpubr (accessed 12 October 2022).Google Scholar
Alumäe, Tanel, Ottokar Tilk & Asadullah. 2018. Advanced rich transcription system for Estonian speech. In Muischnek, Kadri & Müürisep, Kaili (eds.), Human Language Technologies: The Baltic Perspective, 18. IOS Press.Google Scholar
Aske, Jon. 1989. Path predicates in English and Spanish: A closer look. Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 15, 114.10.3765/bls.v15i0.1753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59, 617645.10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, Ruth A. & Isaac Slobin, Dan (eds.). 1994. Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study. New York & London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Berthele, Raphael. 2004. The typology of motion and posture verbs: A variationist account. In Kortmann, Bernd (ed.), Dialectology Meets Typology: Dialect Grammar From a Cross-Linguistic Perspective (Trends in Linguistics: Studies and Monographs 153), 93126. Berlin & New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Cardini, Filippo-Enrico. 2008. Manner of motion saliency: An inquiry into Italian. Cognitive Linguistics 19(4), 533569.10.1515/COGL.2008.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, Wallace L. 1980. The Pear Stories: Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Aspects of Narrative Production. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Choi, Soonja & Bowerman, Melissa. 1991. Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition 41(1), 83121.10.1016/0010-0277(91)90033-ZCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dirven, René & Verspoor, Marjolijn. 1998. Cognitive Exploration of Language and Linguistics (Cognitive Linguistics in Practice 1). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dixon, R. M. W. 1982. Where Have All The Adjectives Gone? And Other Essays in Semantics and Syntax (Janua Linguarum, Series Maior 107). Berlin, New York & Amsterdam: Mouton.10.1515/9783110822939CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erelt, Mati (ed.). 2003. Estonian Language (Linguistica Uralica Supplementary Series 1). Tallinn: Estonian Academy Publishers.Google Scholar
Erelt, Mati. 2017. Öeldis [Predicate]. In Erelt, Mati & Metslang, Helle (eds.), Eesti keele süntaks [Estonian syntax] (Eesti Keele Varamu 3), 93239. Tartu: Tartu Ülikooli Kirjastus.Google Scholar
Erelt, Mati & Metslang, Helle (eds.). 2017. Eesti keele süntaks [Estonian syntax]. Tartu: Tartu Ülikooli Kirjastus.Google Scholar
Erelt, Mati, Kasik, Reet, Metslang, Helle, Rajandi, Henno, Ross, Kristiina, Saari, Henn, Tael, Kaja & Vare, Silvi. 1993. Eesti keele grammatika II. Süntaks. Lisa: kiri [Estonian Grammar II. Syntax. Appendix: Orthography]. Tallinn: Eesti Teaduste Akadeemia Keele ja Kirjanduse Instituut.Google Scholar
Erelt, Mati, Erelt, Tiiu, Metslang, Helle, Rajandi, Henno, Ross, Kristiina, Saari, Henn, Tael, Kaja & Vare, Silvi. 1995. Eesti keele grammatika I. Morfoloogia. Sõnamoodustus [Estonian Grammar I. Morphology. Derivation]. Tallinn: Eesti Teaduste Akadeemia Eesti Keele Instituut.Google Scholar
Fagard, Benjamin, Stosic, Dejan & Cerruti, Massimo. 2017. Within-type variation in satellite-framed languages: The case of Serbian. STUF – Language Typology and Universals 70(4), 637660.10.1515/stuf-2017-0027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filipović, Luna. 2007. Talking About Motion: A Crosslinguistic Investigation of Lexicalization Patterns (Studies in Language Companion Series 91). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Martin H. & Zwaan, Rolf A.. 2008. Embodied language: A review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61(6), 825850.10.1080/17470210701623605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, Susanne, Savin, Egor, Solt, Stephanie, Ebert, Cornelia & Krifka, Manfred. 2019. Antonym adjective pairs and prosodic iconicity: Evidence from letter replications in an English blogger corpus. Linguistics Vanguard 5(1), 20180017.10.1515/lingvan-2018-0017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond W. Jr 2006. Embodiment and Cognitive Science. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M. & Kaschak, Michael P.. 2002. Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9(3), 558565.10.3758/BF03196313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goschler, Juliana & Stefanowitsch, Anatol (eds.). 2013. Variation and Change in the Encoding of Motion Events (Human Cognitive Processing 41). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guo, Jiansheng, Elena Lieven, Nancy Budwig, Ervin-Tripp, Susan, Nakamura, Keiko & Özçalişkan, Seyda (eds.). 2009. Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Psychology of Language: Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin. New York & London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hallonsten Halling, Pernilla. 2018. Adverbs: A Typological Study of a Disputed Category. Ph.D. thesis, Department of Linguistics, Stockholm University.Google Scholar
Hoffmann, Dorothea. 2012. Path salience in motion descriptions in Jaminjung. In Filipović, Luna & Jaszczolt, Kasia M. (eds.), Space and Time in Languages and Cultures: Linguistic Diversity (Human Cognitive Processing 36), 459480. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.36.24hofCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, John P. 1970. Expressions of direction in Modern Irish. Word 26(1), 8893.10.1080/00437956.1970.11435583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Iraide. 2009. Path salience in motion events. In Guo, Jiansheng et al. (eds.), Crosslinguistic Approaches to the Psychology of Language: Research in the Tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin, 403414. New York & London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Iraide (ed.). 2017. Motion and Space Across Languages. Theory and Applications (Human Cognitive Processing 59). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Iraide & Hijazo-Gascón, Alberto. 2012. Variation in motion events: Theory and applications. In Filipović, Luna & Jaszczolt, Kasia M. (eds.), Space and Time in Languages and Cultures: Linguistic Diversity (Human Cognitive Processing 36), 349371. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.36.19ibaCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ikegami, Yoshihiko. 1969. The semological structure of the English verbs of motion. Linguistic Automation Project, Yale University, New Haven, CT.Google Scholar
Ikegami, Yoshihiko. 1987. ‘Source’ vs. ‘goal’: A case of linguistic dissymmetry. In Dirven, René & Radden, Günter (eds.), Concepts of Case (Studien Zur Englischen Grammatik 4), 122146. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark. 1989. Embodied knowledge. Curriculum Inquiry 19(4), 361377.10.1080/03626784.1989.11075338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karimipour, Amir & Rezai, Vali. 2016. Typological analysis of Ilami Kurdish verbs of motion. STUF – Language Typology and Universals 69(3), 411435.10.1515/stuf-2016-0017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, Michael P., Madden, Carol J., Therriault, David J., Yaxley, Richard H., Mark Aveyard, Adrienne A. Blanchard, & Zwaan, Rolf A.. 2005. Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition 94(3), B79B89.10.1016/j.cognition.2004.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klamer, Marian & Moro, Francesca R.. 2020. What is ‘natural’ speech? Comparing free narratives and Frog stories in Indonesia. Language Documentation & Conservation 14, 238313.Google Scholar
Kopecka, Anetta & Vuillermet, Marine. 2021. Source–goal (a)symmetries across languages. Studies in Language 45(1), 235.10.1075/sl.00018.kopCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, Beth. 1993. English Verb Classes and Alternations: A Preliminary Investigation. Chicago & London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lewandowski, Wojciech. 2021. Variable motion event encoding within languages and language types: A usage-based perspective. Language and Cognition 13(1), 3465.10.1017/langcog.2020.25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindsay, Shane, Scheepers, Christoph & Kamide, Yuki. 2013. To dash or to dawdle: Verb-associated speed of motion influences eye movements during spoken sentence comprehension. PLoS ONE 8(6), e67187.10.1371/journal.pone.0067187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Łozińska, Joanna. 2021. The poverty of manner categories in motion verbs coding vertical relations: Evidence from Polish and Russian. Russian Linguistics 45(1), 93104.10.1007/s11185-021-09237-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, Daniel. 2021. sjPlot: Data visualization for statistics in social science. R package version 2.8.8. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sjPlot.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, Yo & Kawachi, Kazuyuki (eds.). 2020. Broader Perspectives on Motion Event Descriptions (Human Cognitive Processing 69). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, Yo, Kimi Akita, Anna Bordilovskaya, Kiyoko Eguchi, Hiroaki Koga, Miho Mano, Ikuko Matsuse, Morita, Takahiro, Nagaya, Naonori & Takahashi, Kiyoko. 2022. Linguistic representations of visual motion: A crosslinguistic experimental study. In Sarda, Laure & Fagard, Benjamin (eds.), Neglected Aspects of Motion-Event Description: Deixis, Asymmetries, Constructions (Human Cognitive Processing 72), 4367. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.72.03matCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, Mercer. 1969. Frog, Where Are You? New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Mirambel, A. 1950. Remarques sur l’expression linguistique de la notion de mouvement [Linguistic expression of the concept of motion]. Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique 43(1), 142156.Google Scholar
Montero-Melis, Guillermo. 2021. Consistency in motion event encoding across languages. Frontiers in Psychology 12, 625153.10.3389/fpsyg.2021.625153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narasimhan, Bhuvana. 2003. Motion events and the lexicon: A case study of Hindi. Lingua 113(2), 123160.10.1016/S0024-3841(02)00068-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pajusalu, Renate, Neeme Kahusk, Heili Orav, Veismann, Ann, Vider, Kadri & Õim, Haldur. 2013. The encoding of motion events in Estonian. In van der Zee, Emile & Vulchanova, Mila (eds.), Motion Encoding in Language and Space (Explorations in Language and Space 6), 4466. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pasanen, Päivi & Pakkala-Weckström, Mari. 2008. The Finnish way to travel: Verbs of motion in Finnish frog story narratives. In Garant, Mikel, Helin, Irmeli & Yli-Jokipii, Hilkka (eds.) Kieli ja globalisaatio = Language and Globalization (AFinLAn Vuosikirja 66), 311331. Jyväskylä: Suomen soveltavan kielitieteen yhdistyksen julkaisuja.Google Scholar
Perlman, Marcus, Clark, Nathaniel & Falck, Marlene Johansson. 2015. Iconic prosody in story reading. Cognitive Science 39(6), 13481368.10.1111/cogs.12190CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plungian, Vladimir & Rakhilina, Ekaterina. 2013. Time and speed: Where do speed adjectives come from? Russian Linguistics 37(3), 347359.10.1007/s11185-013-9117-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, Friedemann. 2013. How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(9), 458470.10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. 2020. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 24 August 2021).Google Scholar
Rätsep, Huno. 1978. Eesti keele lihtlausete tüübid [Types of simple sentences in Estonian] (Eesti NSV TA Emakeele Seltsi Toimetised 12). Tallinn: Valgus.Google Scholar
Schäfer, Martin. 2020. From quick to quick-to-INFINITIVAL: On what is lexeme specific across paradigmatic and syntagmatic distributions. English Language and Linguistics 25(2), 347377.10.1017/S1360674320000167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shintel, Hadas & Nusbaum, Howard C.. 2008. Moving to the speed of sound: Context modulation of the effect of acoustic properties of speech. Cognitive Science 32(6), 10631074.10.1080/03640210801897831CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shintel, Hadas, Nusbaum, Howard C. & Okrent, Arika. 2006. Analog acoustic expression in speech communication. Journal of Memory and Language 55(2), 167177.10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1996. Two ways to travel: Verbs of motion in English and Spanish. In Shibatani, Masayoshi & Thompson, Sandra A. (eds.), Grammatical Constructions: Their Form and Meaning, 195220. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 2004. The many ways to search for a frog: Linguistic typology and the expression of motion events. In Strömqvist, Sven & Verhoeven, Ludo (eds.), Relating Events in Narrative, vol. 2: Typological and Contextual Perspectives, 219257. Mahwah, NJ & London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 2006. What makes manner of motion salient? Explorations in linguistic typology, discourse, and cognition. In Hickmann, Maya & Robert, Stéphane (eds.), Space in Languages: Linguistic Systems and Cognitive Categories (Typological Studies in Language 66), 5981. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.66.05sloCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I., Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Iraide, Kopecka, Anetta & Majid, Asifa. 2014. Manners of human gait: A crosslinguistic event-naming study. Cognitive Linguistics 25(4), 701741.10.1515/cog-2014-0061CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Speed, Laura J. & Vigliocco, Gabriella. 2014. Eye movements reveal the dynamic simulation of speed in language. Cognitive Science 38(2), 367382.10.1111/cogs.12096CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Speed, Laura J., Vinson, David P. & Vigliocco, Gabriella. 2019. Representing meaning. In Dąbrowska, Ewa & Divjak, Dagmar (eds.), Cognitive Linguistics: Foundations of Language, 221244. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110626476-010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stosic, Dejan. 2019. Manner as a cluster concept: What does lexical coding of manner of motion tell us about manner? In Aurnague, Michel & Stosic, Dejan (eds.), The Semantics of Dynamic Space in French: Descriptive, Experimental and Formal Studies on Motion Expression (Human Cognitive Processing 66), 141177. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Stosic, Dejan. 2020. Defining the concept of manner: An attempt to order chaos. Testi e linguaggi 14, 127150.Google Scholar
Strömqvist, Sven & Verhoeven, Ludo. 2004. Relating Events in Narrative, vol. 2: Typological and Contextual Perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.10.4324/9781410609694CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 1972. Semantic Structures in English and Atsugewi. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 1975. Semantics and Syntax of Motion. Syntax and Semantics 4, 181238.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen, Timothy (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description, vol. 3: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon, 1st edn, 57149. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 1988. Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science 12(1), 49100.10.1207/s15516709cog1201_2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 2000a. Toward a Cognitive Semantics, vol. I: Concept Structuring Systems. Cambridge, MA & London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 2000b. Toward a Cognitive Semantics, vol. II: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge, MA & London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 2007. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen, Timothy (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description, vol. 3: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon, 2nd edn, 66169. Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511618437.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taremaa, Piia. 2017. Attention Meets Language: A Corpus Study on the Expression of Motion in Estonian (Dissertationes Linguisticae Universitas Tartuensis 29). Tartu: University of Tartu Press.Google Scholar
Taremaa, Piia & Kopecka, Anetta. 2022. Manner of motion in Estonian: A descriptive account of speed. Studies in Language. Published online 16 March 2022. https://doi.org/10.1075/sl.21038.tar.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taremaa, Piia & Kopecka, Anetta. 2023. Speed and space: Semantic asymmetries in motion descriptions in Estonian. Cognitive Linguistics 34(1), in press.10.1515/cog-2021-0132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tauli, Valter. 1973. Standard Estonian Grammar, vol. I: Phonology, Morphology, Word-Formation (Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis, Studia Uralica et Altaica Upsaliensia 8). Uppsala: Almqvist & Wiksell.Google Scholar
Tauli, Valter. 1983. Standard Estonian Grammar, vol. II: Syntax (Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis, Studia Uralica et Altaica Upsaliensia 14). Uppsala: Borgströms Tryckeri AB.Google Scholar
Tesnière, Lucien. 1959. Éléments de syntaxe structurale [Elements of structural syntax]. Paris: Klincksieck.Google Scholar
Tuuri, Emilia. 2021. Concerning variation in encoding spatial motion: Evidence from Finnish. Nordic Journal of Linguistics. Published online 11 October 2021. https://doi.org/ 10.1017/S0332586521000202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veismann, Ann & Sahkai, Heete. 2016. Ühendverbidest läbi prosoodia prisma [Particle verbs and prosody]. Eesti Rakenduslingvistika Ühingu aastaraamat = Estonian Papers in Applied Linguistics 12, 269285.10.5128/ERYa12.16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viht, Annika & Habicht, Külli. 2019. Eesti keele sõnamuutmine [Estonian morphology] (Eesti Keele Varamu 4). Tartu: Tartu Ülikooli Kirjastus.Google Scholar
Vuillermet, Marine & Kopecka, Anetta. 2019. Trajectoire: A methodological tool for eliciting path of motion. In Lahaussois, Aimée & Vuillermet, Marine (eds.), Methodological Tools for Linguistic Description and Typology, special issue of Language, Documentation and Conservation 16, 340353.Google Scholar
Wickham, Hadley, Winston Chang, Lionel Henry, Thomas Lin Pedersen, Kohske Takahashi, Claus Wilke, Kara Woo, Yutani, Hiroaki & Dunnington, Dewey. 2021. ggplot2: Create elegant data visualisations using the grammar of graphics. R package version 3.3.4. https://cran.r-project.org/package=ggplot2 (accessed 12 October 2022).Google Scholar
Wickham, Hadley, François, Romain, Henry, Lionel & Müller, Kirill. 2020. dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation. R package version 1.0.6. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr (accessed 12 October 2022).Google Scholar
Zlatev, Jordan & Yangklang, Peerapat. 2004. A third way to travel. In Strömqvist, Sven & Verhoeven, Ludo (eds.), Relating Events in Narrative, vol. 2: Typological and Contextual Perspectives, 159190. Mahwah, NJ & London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A. 2009. Mental simulation in language comprehension and social cognition. European Journal of Social Psychology 39(7), 11421150.10.1002/ejsp.661CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Variables describing the whole data of the narrations

Figure 1

Table 2. Motion variables of general type

Figure 2

Table 3. Verb-related variables coded in motion clauses

Figure 3

Table 4. Clause-related variables of space and manner in motion clauses

Figure 4

Figure 1. Panel A: length of the narrations produced by the participants in three conditions measured in minutes. Panel B: length of the narrations in the number of clauses in total. Panel C: the number of motion clauses. The horizontal lines indicate median values. The diamond figures stand for mean values.

Figure 5

Figure 2. Speech rate of the narrators across the conditions. The horizontal lines indicate median values. The diamond figures stand for mean values.

Figure 6

Table 5. The five most frequent bare verbs across the three conditions (absolute frequencies)

Figure 7

Table 6. The five most frequent particle verbs across the three conditions (absolute frequencies)

Figure 8

Table 7. The frequencies of the types and tokens of motion verbs (without their optional particles) used by the narrators

Figure 9

Figure 3. The distribution of verbs across three conditions: manner verbs (= ‘manV’), path+manner verbs (= ‘path+manV’), path verbs (= ‘pathV’), neutral verbs (= ‘neutrV’), and verbs of ambiguous semantics (= ‘unclear’).

Figure 10

Figure 4. The characteristics of all motion clauses across three conditions in terms of (i) the frequencies of minus-ground (= ‘minusGr’) and plus-ground clauses (= ‘plusGr’; panel A) and (ii) the expression of spatial categories (panel B).

Figure 11

Figure 5. The presence (= ‘yes’) or absence (= ‘no’) of manner modifiers across three conditions.

Supplementary material: PDF

Taremaa et al. supplementary material

Taremaa et al. supplementary material

Download Taremaa et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 203.6 KB