Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T22:26:16.523Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spanish validation of General Decision-Making Style scale: Sex invariance, sex differences and relationships with personality and coping styles

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Adrián Alacreu-Crespo
Affiliation:
Department of Emergency Psychiatry and Post-Acute Care, CHU Montpellier, Montpellier, France; Neuropsychiatry, Epidemiological and Clinical Research, INSERM, University of Montpellier, Montpellier, France; Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
María C. Fuentes*
Affiliation:
Corresponding author. Department of Methodology of Behavioral Sciences, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
Diana Abad-Tortosa
Affiliation:
Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
Irene Cano-Lopez
Affiliation:
International University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain; Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
Esperanza González
Affiliation:
Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
Miguel Ángel Serrano
Affiliation:
Department of Psychobiology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain
*
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The General Decision-Making Styles (GDMS) scale measures five decision-making styles: rational, intuitive, dependent, avoidant and spontaneous. GDMS has been related to coping and some personality factors and sex-differences has been described. In spite of its usefulness, there is not a validated Spanish translation. The aim of this study is to translate to Spanish and provide psychometric evidence considering sex differences and the relationships between GDMS, personality and coping variables. Two samples were used for this study; the first sample composed by 300 participants who completed the GDMS and the Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI), and the second sample of 361 participants who completed the GDMS, the Ten Item Personality Trait Inventory and the brief COPE scales. Participants from second sample filled in GDMS a second time (137 participants) after eight weeks from the first data collection. Confirmatory factor analyses showed a five-factor composition of GDMS with equivalence across sex using invariance analyses. Moreover, GDMS showed acceptable internal consistency and temporal stability. Finally, rational and intuitive styles were related to healthier coping patterns and emotional stability, while dependent, avoidant and spontaneous styles were associated with unhealthy coping patterns and emotional instability.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2019] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Researchers have proved the utility of decision-making styles in the prediction of some important daily life decisions: choosing a career (Reference Gati, Landman, Davidovitch, Asulin-Peretz and GadassiGati, Landman, Davidovitch, Asulin-Peretz & Gadassi, 2010; Reference Singh and GreenhausSingh & Greenhaus, 2004), choose a major college (Reference Galotti, Ciner, Altenbaumer, Geerts, Rupp and WoulfeGalotti et al., 2006) or the satisfaction with a job (Reference Crossley and HighhouseCrossley & Highhouse, 2005). Decision-making styles have been defined as “the learned habitual response pattern exhibited by an individual when confronted with a decision situation. It is not a personality trait, but a habit-based propensity to react in a certain way in a specific decision context (p. 820).” (Reference Scott and BruceScott & Bruce, 1995). Thus, the definition of decision making style was built on the idea that each individual has an habitual pattern of interpreting and responding to decision-making tasks (Reference Driver and KerrDriver, 1979; Reference HarrenHarren, 1979). Decision making styles are related to cognitive styles because decisions depend on how people process environmental information (Reference Hunt, Krzystofiak, Meindl and YousryHunt, Krzystofiak, Meindl & Yousry, 1989).

Janis and Mann’s (1977) conflict model proposes that decision style depends on features of the situation (e.g., whether there is time pressure). However, Scott & Bruce (1995) described decision styles as learned habits where the key factor is the number of alternatives identified and the information gathered during a decision (Reference Driver, Brousseau and HunsakerDriver, Brousseau & Hunsaker, 1993). In this sense, according to Curry’s (1983) “onion theory of personality”, decision-making styles can be conceptualized as “surface” individual differences. Thus, individual differences are represented as the layers of an onion, being the more stable characteristics on the layers closest to the center of the onion (e.g., personality traits). On the other side, “surface” characteristics, although have some stability, are more malleable and adaptive to situations (Reference CurryCurry, 1983). Thus, the use of a particular decision-making style depends on both the situation and the “central” individual differences of people (Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2004).

Reference Scott and BruceScott and Bruce (1995) developed the General Decision Making Style questionnaire (GDMS) for evaluate decision making styles, an instrument of 25 items and five scales: rational style, characterized by logical approach to decisions by searching information and alternatives and a carefully thought out; intuitive style, where people make decisions depending on their hunches or feelings and the flow of the information; dependent style, in which people search advice and guidance from other people in their decision processes; avoidant style, characterized by procrastinating and avoiding decisions; and spontaneous style, characterized by making quick decisions without thinking twice. The first four scales (rational, intuitive, dependent and avoidant) were theorized by Reference Scott and BruceScott and Bruce (1995), but, during the evaluation of the instrument the fifth factor structure including the spontaneous style emerged. Past research has confirmed, using Exploratory factor analysis (EFA), Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and measurement invariance (MI), that GDMS has valid psychometric properties (see Table 1 for a summary of the adaptations/translations). In fact, GDMS shows similar construct validity with both varimax and oblimin rotation. However, because of the scale inter-correlations, and because of some “problematic” items that show cross-load between scales and low inter-item correlation, the oblimin rotation (which does not assume independence) is preferable. In spite of the definition of decision-making styles as learned habits or propensity to respond to a decision-making situation (Reference Scott and BruceScott & Bruce, 1995), only Spicer & Sandler-Smith (2005) have performed a four weeks test-retest reliability to test temporal stability, showing an acceptable temporal stability for all the scales. However, the low sample on retest (82 respondents) and the short period between test-retest suggest that further test-retest analyses are needed.

Table 1: Summary of the validation and adaptations for the General Decision Making Style questionnaire

Note: If some information were not in the cited articles we asked the authors in order to obtain the information; Problematic items = Items who cross-load in some scales or had low inter-items correlations.

The GDMS has been adapted to different languages: Swedish (Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2004), Italian (Reference Baiocco, Laghi and D’AlessioBaiocco, Laghi & D’Alessio, 2009; Reference Gambetti, Fabbri, Bensi and TonettiGambetti, Fabbri, Bensi & Tonetti, 2008), Dutch (Reference Curşeu and SchruijerCurşeu & Schruijer, 2012), Slovak (Reference Bavolar and OrosováBavolar & Orosová, 2015), French (Reference Girard, Reeve and BonaccioGirard, Reeve & Bonaccio, 2016) and a German adaptation for patients on clinical decision making (Reference Fischer, Soyez and GurtnerFischer, Soyez & Gurtner, 2015). A Spanish translation was also found (Reference del Campo, Pauser, Steiner and Vetscheradel Campo, Pauser, Steiner & Vetschera, 2016), but its psychometric properties had not been completely probed. Although, there are several scales to evaluate decision making stylesFootnote 1, GDMS is the most widely used scale in the literature, so a Spanish validation is needed.

1.1 Decision-making styles and coping styles, thinking styles and personality

Decision-making styles have been related to personality and cognitive variables. For example, coping styles for conflict management (e.g., avoid conflict, looking for social interactions…) were related to decision-making styles (Reference LooLoo, 2000). Also, decision-making styles are considered as factors of resilience/vulnerability to stress. Indeed, high scores in the avoidant style are related to higher levels of cortisol under a stressful task (Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2008). Regarding thinking styles, rational style is associated with high analytical and sequential thinking style while intuitive style is associated with holistic and intuitive thinking (Reference Gambetti, Fabbri, Bensi and TonettiGambetti et al., 2008). Past validations of GDMS related decision-making styles to trait variables, as mental health, self-esteem or locus of control. A summary of all these studies can be found on Table 1.

Furthermore, GDMS has been related to the Big Five personality traits (Reference Bavolar and Bačíková-SleškováBavolar & Bačíková-Slešková, 2018; Reference Dewberry, Juanchich and NarendranDewberry, Juanchich & Narendran, 2013; Reference Wood and HighhouseWood & Highhouse, 2014). Rational and intuitive styles were consistently associated with high openness, conscientiousness and less neuroticism. Moreover, intuitive style has been related to high agreeableness and extraversion. Dependent style was related to high agreeableness and neuroticism. Avoidant and spontaneous styles were associated with high neuroticism and low conscientiousness and agreeableness. But differently with extraversion, where avoidant style was negatively related to while spontaneous style was positively associated with extraversion.

1.2 Sex differences in decision-making styles

Different results have been found for sex differences. Some studies did not find any (Reference Baiocco, Laghi and D’AlessioBaiocco et al., 2009; Reference LooLoo, 2000). However, Delaney et al. (2015), using cluster analyses for creating different decision style profiles, found that women have lower predisposition to an affective/experiential profile whereas they had higher predisposition to a dependent style in comparison to men. The predisposition of women to dependent style compared to men was found using police investigators; this study also found that men employed the rational style more than women (Reference Salo and AllwoodSalo & Allwood, 2011). Although some literature shows sex differences, no study (that we know of) provided sex invariance evidence for GDMS, an important issue for ensuring that these sex-differences are not due to the absence of equivalence in the psychometric properties of GDMS between men and women.

2 Main study

Thus, our main purpose was to validate a Spanish adaptation of the GDMS. Additionally, we used this adaptation to examine correlations with sex, personality and coping patterns. In order to achieve these objectives, two samples were collected: the first sample composed by 300 participants who completed the GDMS and the Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI), and a second sample of 361 participants who completed the GDMS, the Ten item personality trait inventory and the brief COPE scales. The second sample filled in GDMS a second time (137 participants) after eight weeks in order study the temporal stability of the scale.

2.1 Method

2.1.1 Participants

First Sample:

300 (158 women) Spanish students from different faculties of the University of Valencia participated in this study (Mean age 21.84; SD = 2.45; range = 18–34 years). Participants were recruited using informative posters. Participation was voluntary and informant consent was obtained.

Second Sample:

The sample was composed of 361 (236 women) Spanish students from different faculties of the University of Valencia and the University Miguel Hernandez (Mean age 20.94; SD = 3.84; range = 18–53 years). Participants were recruited during their academic course and completed questionnaires during 15 minutes. Participation was voluntary and informant consent was obtained before participation. In other to take the test-retest reliability, the same tests were filled in after exactly two months from the first evaluation. From the original sample 137 students (37.9 %) completed the retest.

2.1.2 General decision-making style

We translated the GDMS (Reference Scott and BruceScott and Bruce, 1995) to Spanish from English version; subsequently a native English, translated the scale back into English. No special problems were detected in the back-translated version. Past research showed that one item from the spontaneous style as a “problematic” item in some of the validations (see Table 1), showing cross-load with the intuitive style consistently (“When making decisions, I do what seems natural at the moment”). Although, other items also showed cross-load problems, only this item was consistently problematic, and for that reason we decided to eliminate that item from our adaptation. Therefore, at the first step, our GDMS adaptation had 24 items, one item less than the original version (Reference Scott and BruceScott & Bruce, 1995). Four of the scales had 5 items and only the spontaneous one had 4 items rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranged from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”. The questionnaire heading was: “Listed below are statements describing how individuals go about making important decisions.” Spanish and English version of GDMS can be found in Appendix 2.

2.1.3 Instruments

First Sample: In the first sample, we use the Rational-Experiential Inventory to test convergent validity.

Rational-Experiential Inventory:

We used the 40-items Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI: Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999) in its Spanish version (Reference Peñarroja, Serrano, Gracia, Alacreu-Crespo, González and Martínez-TurPeñarroja et al., 2017). This scale measure rational or experiential thinking style and subdivide each scale in ability or engagement. The ability sub-scale reflects the belief in his/her abilities in using the rational or the experiential thinking. The engagement scale reflects preferences to engage in the rational or the experiential style. Cronbach’s alpha (α) from our sample was: rational engagement (α = .79), rational ability (α = .78), experiential engagement (α = .85) and experiential ability (α = .77).

Second Sample:

In the second sample we employed the Ten Item Personality Inventory and the Brief Cope to test the possible relationships with personality traits and healthy/unhealthy coping styles to stress.

Ten Item Personality Inventory:

We used the Spanish version of the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI: Romero, Villar, Gómez-Fraguela & López-Romero, 2012), which assess personality traits based on the five factor theory of personality (Reference Costa and McCraeCosta & McCrae, 1992). The scale had a total of 10 items consisting of a pair of descriptors and scored from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Each Big-Five dimension was represented by two items, Cronbach’s α from our sample was: E = Extraversion (α = .68), A = Agreeableness (α = .22), ES = Emotional Stability (α = .66), O = Openness (α = .48), C = Conscientiousness (α = .44). This version shows reasonable psychometric properties in terms of test-retest reliability and convergence with the biggest five factor scales (TIPI: Romero, Villar, Gómez-Fraguela & López-Romero, 2012).

Brief COPE:

A Spanish translation (Reference Morán, Landero and GonzálezMorán, Landero & González, 2010) of the brief COPE (Reference CarverCarver, 1997) was used to assess the habitual coping strategies. The scale had 28 items with four alternatives of response from 1 (I usually don’t do this at all) to 4 (I usually do this a lot) and it is divided in first order 14 sub-scales. Cronbach’s α from our sample was: Active coping (α = .54), Planning (α = .52), Emotional support (α = .72), Instrumental support (α = .71), Religion (α = .79), Positive reframing (α = .65), Acceptance (α = .43), Denial (α = .63), Humor (α = .81), Self-distraction (α = .62), Self-blame (α = .60), Behavioral disengagement (α = .66), Venting (α = .51) and Substance use (α = .91).

2.1.4 Statistical analysis

Confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) were performed using maximum likehood estimation with robust corrections (MLR) due to the ordinal nature of the data (Reference Finney, DiStefano, Hancock and MuellerFinney & DiStefano, 2006). Because of the lack of consensus in empirical research about the inter-correlation between GDMS scales, two models were tested: (1) an orthogonal 5 factor model (MO), assuming totally independence between scales; and (2) a correlated 5 factor model (MC), assuming inter-correlation between scales. To evaluate model fit we considered the Satorra-Bentler Scaled Chi-square (SB-χ 2) (Reference Satorra and BentlerSatorra & Bentler, 2001) and other robust indexes: the comparative fit index (CFI), where values > .95 implies good fit and values > .90 implies acceptable fit (Reference Marsh and HauMarsh & Hau, 1996); and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) (Reference Hu and BentlerHu & Bentler, 1999) with an confidence interval of 90%, where < .05 values implies good fit, values between .05 and .08 implies acceptable fit and values > .08 implies marginal or poor fit (Reference Browne and CudeckBrowne & Cudeck, 1992).

When the structure of CFA was settled, a model testing approach was employed using multi-sample CFA to examine the invariance from GDMS across sex (Men/Women). First, the five-factor structure was separately tested on each group separately (Models M0a and M0b). After the determination of good fit for each group, both models were integrated into a configural model in which the same factor structure for both groups was tested simultaneously, providing a baseline model (M1). Later, increasingly constrained models were applied to examine the equality of measurement: equal factor loadings across groups (M2), equal factor variances and covariances (M3), the intercepts of items to be the same across groups (M4) and, finally, the equality of error variances and covariances (M5). Taking into account that measurement invariance is required for group comparison, it is necessary only to obtain empirical evidence of factor loadings and intercepts in order to compare means of underlying factors across groups (Reference Millsap, Olivera-Aguilar and HoyleMillsap & Olivera-Aguilar, 2012; Reference Wang and WangWang & Wang, 2012), but adding the factor variances-covariances and the error variances-covariances restrictions can improve the hypothesis of equivalence across sex (Reference ByrneByrne, 2006). To test the invariance hypothesis, changes in the SB-χ 2 (Δ SB-χ 2) between unconstrained and constrained models were tested. However, given the well-known limitations of this statistical approach (Reference Cheung and RensvoldCheung & Rensvold, 2002), we also calculated the change in CFI (Δ CFI), where values less than or equal to .01 are indicative of measurement invariance. Moreover, the change in RMSEA (Δ RMSEA) was also considered. An increase of Δ RMSEA no more than .015 provides support to the most parsimonious model (Reference ChenChen, 2007). Finally, we performed a t test to check sex-differences in the GDMS scales.

The internal consistency from the subscales was measured using Cronbach’s α and composite reliability coefficients, providing information about this issue using both constrained and unconstrained method (Reference Peterson and KimPeterson & Kim, 2013). From the second sample, test-retest reliability was measured using intraclass correlations (ICC) using the two-way mixed effects model with absolute agreement (Reference Koo and LiKoo & Li, 2016). Student’s t or chi-square test were used to compare participants who complete the retest and participants who did not, for sex, age and decision making styles.

Finally, Pearson correlations were performed between the scales of GDMS with the REI scales in order to provide convergent validity, and with the TIPI and brief COPE scales to test the possible relationships with personality traits and healthy/unhealthy coping styles to stress.

All the analyses were performed using SPSS 20.0 and EQS 6.1 (Reference BentlerBentler, 2006).

2.2 Results

2.2.1 Factor structure, sex invariance and sex differences

We first checked the factor loadings of each item in their hypothesized scale. All items showed adequate values, ranging from .47 to .91, except two items from the intuitive scale (“I generally make decisions that feel right to me”, and “When I make a decision, it is more important for me to feel the decision is right than to have a rational reason for it”), which showed factor loadings < .30 (.18 and .26, respectively). Those items were eliminated from the following analysis.

CFA results indicated that the correlated 5-factor (MC) is the model with better fit (SB-χ 2 = 403.99, CFI = .962, RMSEA = .040; see Table 2) confirming the validity of the 5-factor model, assuming inter-correlation between GDMS scales (Figure 1).

Table 2: Confirmatory factor analyses and sex invariance models of GDMS

Note:

* p < .05; FL = Factor loadings, FVC = Factor variances-covariances, INT = Intercepts, EVC = Error variances-covariances.

Figure 1: Correlated confirmatory factor analysis.

Before multi-group analyses, the correlated 5-factor model was separately tested for women (M0a) and men (M0b). Results showed a good fit to the data in both groups, and an adequate fit for the configural model (M1). Results obtained from the comparison between the four nested models tested with the configural model showed that: factor loadings did not differ across sex (M2, Δ CFI < .01, Δ RMSEA < .015); the dispersion of factor scores across sex was the same and they followed equal relational patterns (M3, Δ CFI < .01, Δ RMSEA < .015); the item scores across sex have the same scalar measurement (M4, Δ CFI < .01, Δ RMSEA < .015); and, finally, the item residual variances and covariances are the same across sex (M5, Δ CFI < .01, Δ RMSEA < .015) (see Table 2)

From the five decision making styles, the dependent style showed significant sex-differences (t 641 = −2.09, p < .04, uncorrected for multiple tests), showing women higher scores in the dependent style in comparison to men (Table 3). Moreover, the spontaneous style also showed significant sex differences (t 641 = 3.22, p < .001), showing men higher scores than women.

Table 3: Descriptive statistics, test-retest reliability, internal consistency and inter-scales correlations

Note: SD = standard deviation; ICC = Intraclass correlation; CI = Confident interval 95 %; R=Rational; I=Intuitive; D=Dependent; A=Avoidant. ICC=Testt-retest; CR=composite reliability. All p-values of correlations are < .001 except .11, which is .05, and all correlations between .10 and −.10 which are >.05. 1 Analysis using second Sample.

2.2.2 Internal consistency, test-retest reliability and inter-scale correlations

Table 3 shows descriptive, Cronbach’s α and composite reliability as indicators of internal consistency, test-retest reliabilities, and the inter-scale correlations. Results regarding internal consistency showed adequate values, ranging from .72 to .91 for both Cronbach’s α and composite reliability. Test-retest reliability using ICC showed a great significant temporal stability for all the scales (range ICC = .77 to .86, p < .001). There were significantly fewer women in the group of people who completed the retest than in the group who not (χ2 = 3.91, p < .048). However, there were no differences in age or decision-making styles at first test between people who completed the retest and who did not (all p’s > .05; Appendix 1). Finally, correlations between the scales shows that rational style correlated negatively with avoidant and spontaneous styles, and positively with dependent style; also intuitive style correlated positively with spontaneous style, and avoidant style correlated positively with dependent and spontaneous styles.

2.2.3 Relations of GDMS with thinking styles

In this section we describe the relations with the total rational and experiential thinking styles, for more information about engagement and ability sub-scales see Table 4.

Table 4: Pearson correlations between GDMS scales with REI, TIPI and BriefCOPE scales

Note:

*** p < .001

** p < .01

* p < .05.

The rational decision making style was positive and moderately related to rational thinking style and negatively weakly associated with experiential thinking style. The intuitive decision making style was positively associated to experiential thinking style. The dependent style was only weakly negatively related to rational style. The avoidant style was negatively related to rational style too. And, finally, the spontaneous style was positively associated with experiential style.

2.2.4 Relationships of GDMS with personality factors

Pearson correlations for study the relations of decision-making styles with five factor personality and coping styles were described in Table 4.

The rational scale was positive, but moderately, associated with agreeableness, conscientiousness and positively associated with emotional stability. The intuitive scale was moderated and positively related to extraversion and positively to openness. The dependent style was negative associated with emotional stability. The avoidant style was negatively associated with all the five factors. Finally, the spontaneous style was positively related to extraversion and negatively to emotional stability, agreeableness and conscientiousness.

2.2.5 Relationships of GDMS with coping styles

Concerning coping styles, rational style from GDMS correlates positively with the coping active scale and the planning coping scale, also was positively but weakly related to the acceptance scale and negatively associated with humor, behavioral disengagement, venting and substance use coping styles. The intuitive scale correlated positively with active coping and positive reframing and negatively with self-blame and behavioral disengagement. Dependent style correlated strongly and positively with emotional support and instrumental support, and also weakly positively correlated with venting and negatively with humor. Avoidant scale strongly positively correlated to behavioral disengagement, and positively to denial, self-blame, substance use, self-distraction and religion; furthermore, avoidant GDMS style negatively correlated with active coping, planning and positive reframing. Finally, positive and significant correlations were found between spontaneous style and substance use, denial, behavioral disengagement, self-distraction, self-blame and humor; moreover, spontaneous style correlated negatively to planning.

2.3 Discussion

The principal aim of this research was to validate the Spanish translation of the GDMS and provide psychometric properties from this translation. For that, we aimed to confirm the 5-factor structure and provided invariance by sex evidence. Our results showed good construct validity for the correlated five factor structure of the Spanish adaptation of GDMS. This result appeared when two items of intuitive scale were eliminated leading the scale with a total of 22 items. Furthermore, the questionnaire structure is also invariant across sex, confirming the hypothesis of equivalence by sex. Moreover, the five scales showed acceptable internal consistency and test-retest reliability. Apart from this, other results showed that women scored higher in dependent style than men; this result may be related to studies that suggest that women use social support as a coping strategy (Reference TaylorTaylor, 2006; Reference Thoits and EckenrodeThoits, 1991). By contrast, men scored somewhat higher in spontaneous decision-making style. Finally, the correlations between GDMS subscales with personality the five factor model, coping styles and thinking styles, are consistent with past research.

We confirmed the 5-factor structure from the Spanish adaptation of GDMS showing that the model with better fit is the correlated model, which agrees with the previous research (Reference LooLoo, 2000). The scale shows the same five factor structure as the original scale (Reference Scott and BruceScott & Bruce, 1995), and the subsequent adaptations to other languages (Reference Baiocco, Laghi and D’AlessioBaiocco et al., 2009; Reference Bavolar and OrosováBavolar & Orosová, 2015; Reference Curşeu and SchruijerCurşeu & Schruijer, 2012; Reference Gambetti, Fabbri, Bensi and TonettiGambetti et al., 2008; Reference Girard, Reeve and BonaccioGirard et al., 2016; Reference LooLoo, 2000; Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2004). However, this adaptation has 22 items instead of 25 items from the original scale. As we explained in the methods section, one of the items from spontaneous scale (item S5), was removed from the adaptation prior to the analyses because previous research showed the item is generally “problematic”, showing cross-load systematically with the intuitive scale (Reference Baiocco, Laghi, D´alesio, Gurrieri and Di ChiacchioBaiocco, Laghi, D´alesio, Gurrieri & Di Chiacchio, 2007; Reference Bavolar and OrosováBavolar & Orosová, 2015; Reference Curşeu and SchruijerCurşeu & Schruijer, 2012; Reference del Campo, Pauser, Steiner and Vetscheradel Campo et al., 2016; Reference Gambetti, Fabbri, Bensi and TonettiGambetti et al., 2008; Reference Girard, Reeve and BonaccioGirard et al., 2016; Reference LooLoo, 2000; Reference Scott and BruceScott & Bruce, 1995; Reference Spicer and Sadler-SmithSpicer & Sadler-Smith, 2005). This is reasonable because the highly similarity between both scales. Other studies also showed that there is another “problematic” items but, only S5 appear consistently in almost all the previous research. Moreover, once the CFA was performed two more items were removed from the intuitive scale because they showed factor loadings below to .30, as literature recommends. The meaning of both items seems to be slightly different from the other three items of the intuitive scale. In this sense, the removed items are asking what the feeling is after the decision making while the remaining items are focused on the information (hunches) that influence decision-making.

Regarding invariance, to the best of our knowledge this is the first time that GDMS showed invariance for sex. A previous study showed invariance between two different languages English and French (Reference Girard, Reeve and BonaccioGirard et al., 2016), but our results provided both metric and scalar invariance evidence for sex. The scale surpasses the usual criteria for factor variance-covariance invariance and error variances-covariances invariance. Indeed, error variances-covariances is a really improbable and heavy restriction (Reference Meredith, Horn, Colins and SayerMeredith & Horn, 2001). Therefore, GDMS showed equivalence across sex surpassing both the basic and the more robust restrictions. These results imply that the sex-differences found in previous studies were not due to the scale. Thus, past research on sex-differences has reported that dependent style is more used by women than men, and rational style or a combination of spontaneous and intuitive style as more used by men than women (Reference Delaney, Strough, Parker and Bruine de BruinDelaney et al., 2015; Reference Salo and AllwoodSalo & Allwood, 2011). In this regard, our results for sex-differences provided more evidence for the dependent style and spontaneous style along the same line as previous research.

Despite the modifications in the sub-scales correction, results showed acceptable internal consistency for all scales. Moreover, results showed high test-retest reliability. In this regard, this adaptation seems to measure the decision-making styles in Spanish speaking populations. Moreover, we provided evidence about temporal stability with a sample of 137 participants in the retest (two months after the first measure), one month more than Spicer & Sandler-Smith (2005) study.

Regarding convergent validity, like the results from Gambetti et al. (2008) with the Style of Learning and Thinking test (SOLAT: Albaili, 1993), our results showed that rational decision making style correlates positively with rational thinking style and negatively with the experiential thinking style. And intuitive decision-making style showed the reverse direction in its relationships with thinking styles. Moreover, our results showed that higher scores in dependent and avoidant styles predicted lower scores in rational thinking style. In addition, scoring more on spontaneous style correlates with more experiential thinking style, possibly because of the strong relationship between intuitive and spontaneous decision-making style, and because spontaneous style is based on fast decisions based sometimes in hunches.

Having a good measure of how people usually decide, as decision making styles, is helpful for research and for psychological practice. Different studies have shown that decision making styles are good predictors of real life decision-making with long term consequences which could influence peoples’ life (Reference Galotti, Ciner, Altenbaumer, Geerts, Rupp and WoulfeGalotti et al., 2006; Reference Gati, Gadassi and Mashiah-CohenGati, Gadassi & Mashiah-Cohen, 2012; Reference Gati, Landman, Davidovitch, Asulin-Peretz and GadassiGati et al., 2010; Reference Singh and GreenhausSingh & Greenhaus, 2004). In fact, decision making styles seem to influence the way people perceive and cope with stressful situations (Reference Allwood and SaloAllwood & Salo, 2012; Reference Salo and AllwoodSalo & Allwood, 2011; Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2008), and are good predictors of general mental health (Reference Bavolar and OrosováBavolar & Orosová, 2015) and health-risk behaviors (Reference Bavolar and Bačíková-SleškováBavolar & Bačíková-Slešková, 2018). In this sense, our results suggest that the rational and intuitive styles as “the healthier styles”. To support this, both styles showed positive relationships with emotional stability, and active and healthier coping styles (e.g., active coping, positive reframing, or planning). This confirms the results from previous research: relating rational style to high self-efficacy and self-esteem (Reference Baiocco, Laghi, D´alesio, Gurrieri and Di ChiacchioBaiocco et al., 2007; Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2004) and low stress in public officials (Reference Allwood and SaloAllwood & Salo, 2012); showing intuitive style as a style associated with less regret after a medical decision and better mental health (Reference Bavolar and OrosováBavolar & Orosová, 2015); and showing both relationships with less neuroticism and high conscientiousness (Reference Bavolar and Bačíková-SleškováBavolar & Bačíková-Slešková, 2018; Reference Wood and HighhouseWood & Highhouse, 2014).

By contrast, dependent, avoidant and spontaneous styles were related to less emotional stability. In this sense, dependent and avoidant style have been associated with low self-esteem and self-efficacy (Reference Baiocco, Laghi, D´alesio, Gurrieri and Di ChiacchioBaiocco et al., 2007; Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2004), high levels of perceived stress and sleep disturbance (Reference Allwood and SaloAllwood & Salo, 2012; Reference Salo and AllwoodSalo & Allwood, 2011) and high neuroticism (Reference Bavolar and Bačíková-SleškováBavolar & Bačíková-Slešková, 2018; Reference Dewberry, Juanchich and NarendranDewberry et al., 2013; Reference Wood and HighhouseWood & Highhouse, 2014). It is probable that using more those decision-making styles would be worse for mental health. Concretely, avoidant and spontaneous styles showed associations with passive coping styles or maladjusted behaviors (drug use, denial, or self-blame), and less conscientiousness and agreeableness. In support of this hypothesis, avoidant style was related in previous research to worse mental health (Reference Bavolar and OrosováBavolar & Orosová, 2015) and higher levels of cortisol after a real-life stressful decision environment (Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2008).

Finally, it is important to highlight sex differences from dependent and spontaneous decision styles. The relationship between dependent style with emotional instability could be due to the prevalence in women of higher scores in neuroticism (Reference Weisberg, De Young and HirshWeisberg, De Young & Hirsh, 2011). Also, a dependent style would be based on an evolutionary characteristic for women to perform more “tend-and-befriend” behaviors under stressful situations (Reference TaylorTaylor, 2006). By contrast, the higher tendency of men to engage in spontaneous decision making could show men to be more impulsive and engage in risky decision making (Reference Barel, Shahrabani and TzischinskyBarel, Shahrabani & Tzischinsky, 2017; Reference Cano-López, Cano-López, Hidalgo and González-BonoCano-López, Cano-López, Hidalgo & González-Bono, 2017; Reference Lighthall, Sakaki, Vasunilashorn, Nga, Somayajula, Chen and MatherLighthall et al., 2012).

As a limitation of this research, participants from both studies were university students, which were necessary to generalize to other samples. This is a general problem for the validation of GDMS because almost all the preceding validations were based on student samples (see Table 1), except for two military samples, one sample of engineers and two of adolescents (Baiocco et al., 2009, 2007; Reference Scott and BruceScott & Bruce, 1995; Reference ThunholmThunholm, 2004). That issue is important because differences in decision styles have been found between samples of different age (Reference Delaney, Strough, Parker and Bruine de BruinDelaney et al., 2015). Another limitation is the low Cronbach’s α of some of the sub-scales of TIPI or brief COPE. This low internal consistency in the scales could be the reason for the lack of relationships between some Big five traits and decision making styles that have been seen in past research (Reference Bavolar and Bačíková-SleškováBavolar & Bačíková-Slešková, 2018; Reference Dewberry, Juanchich and NarendranDewberry et al., 2013; Reference Wood and HighhouseWood & Highhouse, 2014). We suggest that future research should obtain more variety of samples in age and context, to perform age invariance and to use longer scales to test personality and coping styles.

In conclusion, the Spanish adaptation of GDMS questionnaire has acceptable psychometric characteristics, and it is thus a useful instrument to measure decision making styles in Spanish speaking populations. Moreover, invariance by sex implies more validity to future research with GDMS. Finally, personality and coping styles relationships with GDMS provides more clues to the adequacy of each decision style in people’s lives.

Appendix

Appendix 1: Mean ± SD and p values of the comparisons between participants who complete retest and participants who not, in the second sample

Appendix

Appendix 2: English General Decision Making Style and Spanish translation

Note: Int. = Introduction to test

* Item eliminate after confirmatory factor analysis for low factor loadings (< .30).

Footnotes

We want to thank Mr. Francisco Molins Correa and Miss Yasmina El Arbi Catala for their help in recruiting and including participants in the data base. We thank Dr. Eva Leon Zarzeño, Dr. Raquel Costa Ferrer and Dr. Ismael Quintanilla Pardo for their availability in giving access to their courses. We thank Miss Fatmanur Sahin for English editing.

Note: Int. = Introduction to test

* Item eliminate after confirmatory factor analysis for low factor loadings (< .30).

1 These include: the Decision Making Style Inventory (DSI: Nygren, 2000) a 45 items inventory that evaluates analytical, intuitive and regret-avoidant factors; the Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire (Melbourne DMQ: Reference Mann, Burnett, Radford and FordMann, Burnett, Radford & Ford, 1997) a 22 items scale that evaluates vigilance, hypervigilance, buck-passing and procrastination styles; the Decision Styles Scale (DSS: Reference Hamilton, Shih and MohammedHamilton, Shih & Mohammed, 2016) a 10 items inventory to evaluate rational or intuitive decision making, and finally, the Decision Styles Questionnaire (DSQ: Reference Leykin and DerubeisLeykin & Derubeis, 2010) a 43 items scale that evaluates anxiety, avoidance, brooding, dependent, vigilant, intuition, and spontaneity styles. See also http://sjdm.org/dmidi.

References

Albaili, M. A. (1993). Inferred hemispheric thinking style, gender, and academic major among United Arab Emirates college students. Percept Mot Skills, 76(3 Pt 1), 971977. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8321616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allwood, C. M., & Salo, I. (2012). Decision-making styles and stress. International Journal of Stress Management, 19(1), 3447. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baiocco, R., Laghi, F., & D’Alessio, M. (2009). Decision-making style among adolescents: Relationship with sensation seeking and locus of control. Journal of Adolescence, 32(4), 963976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.08.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baiocco, R., Laghi, F., D´alesio, M., Gurrieri, G., & Di Chiacchio, C. (2007). La valutazione degli stili nella presa di decisione in adolescenza: quale utilizzo in ambito educativo? Psicologia Dell’educazione e Formazione, 9(1), 6798.Google Scholar
Barel, E., Shahrabani, S., & Tzischinsky, O. (2017). Sex hormone/cortisol ratios differentially modulate risk-taking in men and women. Evolutionary Psychology, 15(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704917697333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavolar, J., & Orosová, O. (2015). Decision-making styles and their associations with decision-making competencies and mental health. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(1), 115122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavolar, J., & Bačíková-Slešková, M. (2018). Do decision-making styles help explain health-risk behavior among university students in addition to personality factors? Studia Psychologica, 60 (2), 7183. https://doi.org/10.21909/sp.2018.02.753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (2006). EQS 6 structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software, Inc.Google Scholar
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological Methods & Research, 21(2), 230258. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124192021002005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203726532.Google Scholar
Cano-López, I., Cano-López, B., Hidalgo, V., & González-Bono, E. (2017). Effects of acute stress on decision making under ambiguous and risky conditions in healthy young men. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 19(2016), E59. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2016.57.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’ too long: Consider the brief cope. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4(1), 92100. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401\_6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(3), 464504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233255. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902\_5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual: revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO five-factor inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa FL Psychological Assessment Resources, 3, 101. https://doi.org/10.1037//1040-3590.4.1.5.Google Scholar
Crossley, C. D., & Highhouse, S. (2005). Relation of job search and choice process with subsequent satisfaction. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26(2), 255268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2004.04.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curry, L. (1983). An organization of learning styles theory and constructs. Educational Research Information Centre (ERIC), 228. Retrieved from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED235185.pdf.Google Scholar
Curşeu, P. L., & Schruijer, S. G. L. (2012). Decision styles and rationality: An analysis of the predictive validity of the general decision-making style inventory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 72(6), 10531062. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164412448066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
del Campo, C., Pauser, S., Steiner, E., & Vetschera, R. (2016). Decision making styles and the use of heuristics in decision making. Journal of Business Economics, 86(4), 389412. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11573-016-0811-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delaney, R., Strough, J. N., Parker, A. M., & Bruine de Bruin, W. (2015). Variations in decision-making profiles by age and gender: A cluster-analytic approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 1924. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.04.034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewberry, C., Juanchich, M., & Narendran, S. (2013). Decision-making competence in everyday life: The roles of general cognitive styles, decision-making styles and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(7), 783788. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.06.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Driver, M. J. (1979). Individual decision making and creativity. In Kerr, S. (Ed.), Organizational behavior (pp. 5991). Columbus, OH: Grid Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Driver, M. J., Brousseau, K. E., & Hunsaker, P. L. (1993). The dynamic decision maker. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.Google Scholar
Finney, S. J., & DiStefano, C. (2006). Nonnormal and categorical data in structural equation modeling. In In Hancock, G.R. & Mueller, R.O. (Eds.). Structural equation modeling: A second course (pp. 439492). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.Google Scholar
Fischer, S., Soyez, K., & Gurtner, S. (2015). Adapting Scott and Bruce’s general decision-making style inventory to patient decision making in provider choice. Medical Decision Making, 35(4), 525532. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989X15575518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galotti, K. M., Ciner, E., Altenbaumer, H. E., Geerts, H. J., Rupp, A., & Woulfe, J. (2006). Decision-making styles in a real-life decision: Choosing a college major. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(4), 629639. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.03.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gambetti, E., Fabbri, M., Bensi, L., & Tonetti, L. (2008). A contribution to the italian validation of the general decision-making style inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(4), 842852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gati, I., Gadassi, R., & Mashiah-Cohen, R. (2012). Career decision-making profiles vs. styles: Convergent and incremental validity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81(1), 216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2012.03.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gati, I., Landman, S., Davidovitch, S., Asulin-Peretz, L., & Gadassi, R. (2010). From career decision-making styles to career decision-making profiles: A multidimensional approach. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76(2), 277291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2009.11.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girard, A. J., Reeve, C. L., & Bonaccio, S. (2016). Assessing decision-making style in French-speaking populations: Translation and validation of the general decision-making style questionnaire. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée/European Review of Applied Psychology, 66(6), 325333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2016.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, K., Shih, S. I., & Mohammed, S. (2016). The development and validation of the rational and intuitive decision styles scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(5), 523535. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2015.1132426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harren, V. A. (1979). A model of career decision making for college students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14(2), 119133. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-8791(79)90065-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, R. G., Krzystofiak, F. J., Meindl, J. R., & Yousry, A. M. (1989). Cognitive style and decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44(3), 436453. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(89)90018-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janis, I., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making: A psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Koo, T. K., & Li, M. Y. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of Chiropractic Medicine, 15(2), 155163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leykin, Y., & Derubeis, R. J. (2010). Decision-making styles and depressive symptomatology: Development of the decision styles questionnaire. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(7), 506515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lighthall, N. R., Sakaki, M., Vasunilashorn, S., Nga, L., Somayajula, S., Chen, E. Y., … Mather, M. (2012). Gender differences in reward-related decision processing under stress. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 476484. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loo, R. (2000). A psychometric evaluation of the general decision-making style inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 29(5), 895905. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00241-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, L., Burnett, P., Radford, M., & Ford, S. (1997). The Melbourne decision making questionnaire: An instrument for measuring patterns for coping with decisional conflict. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10(1), 119.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K. T. (1996). Assessing goodness of fit: Is parsimony always desirable? Journal of Experimental Education, 64(4), 364390. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1996.10806604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meredith, W., & Horn, J. (2001). The role of factorial invariance in modeling growth and change. In Colins, M. L. & Sayer, A. G. (Eds.) New methods for the analysis of change. (pp. 203240). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10409-007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millsap, R. E., & Olivera-Aguilar, M. (2012). Investigating measurement invariance using confirmatory factor analysis. In Hoyle, R. (Ed.) Handbook of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Morán, C., Landero, R., & González, M. C. T. (2010). COPE-28: Un análisis psicométrico de la versión en Español del brief COPE. Universitas Psychologica, 9(2), 543552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nygren, T. E. (2000). Development of a measure of decision making styles to predict per-formance in a dynamic J/DM task. In 41st Psychonomic SocietyMeeting. New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 972987. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peñarroja, V., Serrano, M. A., Gracia, E., Alacreu-Crespo, A., González, P., & Martínez-Tur, V. (2017). Rational-experiential thinking style and rational intergroup cooperation: the moderating role of intergroup conflict / Estilos de pensamiento racional-experiencial y la cooperación intergrupal racional: el rol modulador del conflicto intergrupal. Revista de Psicologia Social, 32(1), 2351. https://doi.org/10.1080/02134748.2016.1248028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. A., & Kim, Y. (2013). On the relationship between coefficient alpha and composite reliability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(1), 194198. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero, E., Villar, P., Gómez-Fraguela, J. A., & López-Romero, L. (2012). Measuring personality traits with ultra-short scales: A study of the Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) in a Spanish sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(3), 289293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salo, I., & Allwood, C. M. (2011). Decision-making styles, stress and gender among investigators. Policing: An International Journal of Police Strategies & Management, 34(1), 97119. https://doi.org/10.1108/13639511111106632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66(4), 507514. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02296192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, S. G., & Bruce, R. a. (1995). Decision-making style: The development and assessment of a new measure. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55(5), 818831. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164495055005017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, R., & Greenhaus, J. H. (2004). The relation between career decision-making strategies and person-job fit: A study of job changers. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 64(1), 198221. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-8791(03)00034-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spicer, D., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2005). An examination of the general decision making style questionnaire in two UK samples. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 20(2), 137149. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940510579777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E. (2006). Tend and befriend: Biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 273277. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoits, P. A. (1991). Gender differences in coping with emotional distress. In Eckenrode, J. (Ed.) The Social Context of Coping (pp. 107138). New York, NY: Plenum Press https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-3740-7\_6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thunholm, P. (2004). Decision-making style: Habit, style or both? Personality and Individual Differences, 36(4), 931944. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00162-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thunholm, P. (2008). Decision-making styles and physiological correlates of negative stress: Is there a relation? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 213219. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00640.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, J., & Wang, X. (2012). Structural equation modeling: Applications using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: Applications Using Mplus. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118356258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, Y. J., De Young, C. G., & Hirsh, J. B. (2011). Gender differences in personality across the ten aspects of the Big Five. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(178). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, N. L., & Highhouse, S. (2014). Do self-reported decision styles relate with others’ impressions of decision quality? Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 224228. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.036.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1: Summary of the validation and adaptations for the General Decision Making Style questionnaire

Figure 1

Table 2: Confirmatory factor analyses and sex invariance models of GDMS

Figure 2

Figure 1: Correlated confirmatory factor analysis.

Figure 3

Table 3: Descriptive statistics, test-retest reliability, internal consistency and inter-scales correlations

Figure 4

Table 4: Pearson correlations between GDMS scales with REI, TIPI and BriefCOPE scales

Figure 5

* Appendix 2: English General Decision Making Style and Spanish translation

Supplementary material: File

Alacreu-Crespo et al. supplementary material

Alacreu-Crespo et al. supplementary material
Download Alacreu-Crespo et al. supplementary material(File)
File 470 KB