Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T20:53:11.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modeling and debiasing resource saving judgments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Ola Svenson
Affiliation:
Decision Research, Eugene, Oregon 97 401 USA
Nichel Gonzalez
Affiliation:
Risk Analysis, Social and Decision Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden
Gabriella Eriksson
Affiliation:
Risk Analysis, Social and Decision Research Unit, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, S-106 91 Stockholm, Sweden Swedish National Road and Transport Institute, Linköping, Sweden
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Svenson (2011) showed that choices of one of two alternative productivity increases to save production resources (e.g., man-months) were biased. Judgments of resource savings following a speed increase from a low production speed line were underestimated and following an increase of a high production speed line overestimated. The objective formula for computing savings includes differences between inverse speeds and this is intuitively very problematic for most people. The purpose of the present studies was to explore ways of ameliorating or eliminating the bias. Study 1 was a control study asking participants to increase the production speed of one production line to save the same amount of production resources (man-months) as was saved by a speed increase in a reference line. The increases judged to match the reference alternatives showed the same bias as in the earlier research on choices. In Study 2 the same task and problems were used as in Study 1, but the participants were asked first to judge the resource saving of the reference alternative in a pair of alternatives before they proceeded to the matching task. This weakened the average bias only slightly. In Study 3, the participants were asked to judge the resources saved from each of two successive increases of the same single production line (other than those of the matching task) before they continued to the matching problems. In this way a participant could realize that a second production speed increase from a higher speed (e.g., from 40 to 60 items /man-month) gives less resource savings than the same speed increase from a first lower speed (e.g., from 20 to 40 items/man-month. Following this, the judgments of the same problems as in the other studies improved and the bias decreased significantly but it did not disappear. To be able to make optimal decisions about productivity increases, people need information about the bias and/or reformulations of the problems.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2014] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

Assume that you are a manager who runs a company with two production lines, A and B each of which should produce 1200 units of a product. Line A produces 30 and line B 70 units per man-hour. To save man-hours and money you have an opportunity to invest in one of these lines. The increase in production speed you get for the same investment is for line A from 30 to 40, and for B from 70 to 110 units produced per man-hour. Where would you allocate your investment? In an earlier study Svenson (2011) had participants solve such problems with an instruction including the following:

We want you to give spontaneous judgments concerning which of two productivity improvements will give most time-saving, line A or line B. The two lines produce the same quantities of the same product but at different lines. Your task is to choose the alternative, which after a productivity improvement will give most time-saving in man-hours compared to the situation before the production improvement.

Most people intuitively think that line B saves most man-hours. Not only the differences between the speed increases (10 units/man-hour faster for A and 40 for B) speaks for B but also the ratios (40/30 and 110/70) favor B. In fact, A saves more man-hours than B. We use this example to illustrate a correct algebraic solution of the problem. Assume that 1200 units are produced. Then, it takes line A 1200/30 (=40) man-hours to produce that quantity and 1200/40 (= 30) man-hours after the efficiency increase. Line B started with 1200/70 (=17.14) man-hours and after the improvement 1200/110 (=10.91) man-hours. Thus, the saving was 10 man-hours for line A and 6.23 man-hours for line B.

In general, unaided judgments of time and resources saved by increasing speed are prone to the time-saving bias (Svenson, 1970, 2008, 2011), which is illustrated by this example. The bias means that, when speed is increased from a low speed, judgments of savings are underestimated and when speed is increased from a high speed, they are overestimated.

Brown and Gobeli defined productivity as “measured as a ratio of the unit’s outputs to inputs for a given period of time” (Reference Brown and GobeliBrown & Gobeli, 1992, p. 325) and, even, if there are many formal measurement systems, unaided intuitive first impression judgments of productivity and productivity changes are important for preliminary and later final decisions (Reference SvensonSvenson, 2003). In the present contribution, we study judgments of resource savings after an increase in efficiency or productivity as a prototype for judgments of time/resource savings in general and how they can be debiased. Such judgments have been found to be systematically biased in other areas (Reference Eriksson, Svenson and ErikssonEriksson et al., 2013; Reference Fuller, Gormley, Stradling, Broughton, Kinnear, O’Dolan and HanniganFuller et al., 2009; Reference Larrick and SollLarrick & Soll, 2008; Reference Peer and GamlielPeer & Gamliel, 2012; Svenson, 1970, 2008) and the purpose of the present contribution is to validate the results from earlier production choice experiments (Reference SvensonSvenson, 2011) in studies of judgments and to find ways of avoiding the time-saving bias.

The objective mathematical function relating increase of production speed to resources saved (Reference SvensonSvenson, 2011) is the same also in other areas, such as, time saved by driving faster and fuel saved by using a more fuel efficient car (more miles per gallon) (Reference Larrick and SollLarrick & Soll, 2008; Svenson, 1970, 2008). Therefore, we assumed that human judges will be prone to make the same kind of judgment biases in all of these contexts and that a learning technique that is successful in debiasing judgments in one context will prove efficient also in other contexts.

The early study of the time-saving bias and how it could be avoided, Svenson (1971) used direct quick feedback of correct answers without explanation or time for insight or explicit reasoning. His participants were given training sessions of 2.5 hours per day during a week. The problems in a training session consisted of 1.5 hours of training with feedback in terms of a correct solution to each of a series of time-saving problems. Tests of learning were conducted before and after each learning session giving a total of 10 test phases. This way of learning to avoid the bias was extremely slow. It could be seen as based on exemplar learning (problem-response associations) coupled with quick non-conscious implicit similarity judgments giving the responses. In the present contribution, this “similar to exemplars strategy” will be classified as System 1 learning and it will be contrasted with System 2 learning that uses conscious mental processes. This simple two system approach classification of the processes leading to a judgment has been used by several researchers in the past (Epstein, 1994, Reference Chaiken and TropeChaiken & Trope, 1999). Experiential System 1 processes are seen as holistic, associationistic, fast and sometimes affective and the system is not open to conscious awareness (Reference EpsteinEpstein, 1994), while operations in System 2 use logical connections and conscious reasons to reach a judgment (Reference Slovic, Finucane, Peters and MacGregorSlovic, Finucane, Peters, & MacGregor, 2002; Reference KahnemanKahneman, 2003). Even though the System 1 and System 2 dichotomy is imprecise and applies only approximately in the present context, we will refer to it when we contrast different ways of debiasing time/resource-saving judgments.Footnote 1 Because the earlier study by Svenson (1971) with an exemplar-based System 1 approach was not so successful, we selected an approach to learning that requires less learning time and invites deliberate conscious controlled reasoning and increased insights into how comparisons of resource saving options can be made without systematic biases. Studies of such processes motivate the use of verbal protocols, that can reflect conscious System 2 processes and complement regular analyses of choices or numerical responses.

When a judge encounters a numerical judgment problem, there seems to be a hierarchy of rules in at least partly controlled cognition with a linear rule attempted first, as emphasized in an early paper by Reference Brehmer, Hagafors and JohanssonBrehmer, Hagafors and Johansson (1980) and later reported by several other authors (e.g., Reference Dutt and GonzalezDutt, & Gonzalez, 2013). If the linear rule is found unsatisfactory, curvilinear rules are attempted with more or less success (Reference Ebersbach, Lehner, Resing and WilkeningEbersbach Lehner, Resing & Wilkening, 2008; Brehmer 1971, Wagenaar, 1975, Cronin et al., 2009). If there is more than one information variable, additive relationships are implemented first (Reference BrehmerBrehmer 1971) and, if found unsuccessful, judgments of ratios and proportions may be attempted (Reference Resnick and SingerResnick & Singer, 1993). In the present study we focused on the use of linear and proportional rules that were used in solutions of resource saving problems.

The earlier study by Svenson (2011) showed that the time/resource saving bias distorted choices between productivity increase options in line with the time-saving bias (Svenson, 1970, 2008). In the present contribution, we assumed that this choice bias would also appear in judgments of resource savings, that it is reflected in verbal reports (System 2) and numerical answers (both System 1 and System 2) and that it could be counteracted in learning sessions inviting System 2 learning processes. Because the resource saving bias found for choices was replicated in the matching judgment task of the first study, we continued with two more studies with new participants drawn from the same population as the first set of participants. In these two additional studies, we investigated how the bias could be neutralized or eliminated by two different instructions and reformulations of the problems.

1.1 Model

In general terms, the man-hour gain, G from an improvement of a production line A or B is

(1)

where U is the number of units to be produced, P 1 and P 2 are the production speeds in units/man-hour before and after an improvement for a production line. Here, participants are asked to match two different productivity increases, for production lines A and B, so that they save equal amounts of resources, by choosing the increased speed of B that is needed to achieve this end. Here, P A1 is the initial production speed of A and P A2 the increased speed of A, and likewise for P B1 and PB2, which is in bold font to indicate that it is to be judged by the participant.

Based on earlier research (Reference SvensonSvenson, 2008), participants are assumed to follow the heuristic described by Equation (2),

(2)

This means that when the ratio between the speed increase and the higher speed is the same for two alternatives they are judged to save equal amounts of resources. Equation (2) can be reformulated as 1 − P A1/P A2 = 1 − P B1/PB2, or P B1/PB2 = P A1/P A2, and we call this the ratio rule.

The correct equation is.

(3)

Note, that the difference between Equations (2) and (3) lies in the fact that people are unable to adjust for both P A1 and P B1 in the denominators. The mathematics leading from Equation (1) to equation (3) can be found in the appendix.

Reference Peer and GamlielPeer and Gamliel (2013) found, in their studies, that the denominators in Equation (2), in another context, were P A1 and P B1 instead of P A2 and P B2. We do not know yet when the initial or increased speed is used in the denominator or what characteristics of people or problems that favor the use of one or the other factor. However, in the present study with matching tasks it does not matter which speed is used in the denominator of Equation (2). We hypothesized that, in the context of productivity increase judgments, participants would also use Equation (2) when asked to make two productivity increase gains equal. We used units/man-month instead of units/man-hour in the present studies as the productivity measure because it fits our judgment problems better than man-hour and the above equations apply to any production speed measure.

In the first study, we tested the hypothesis predicting that the time-saving bias should be replicated in the resource saving judgments following a productivity increase. We also wanted to identify specific judgment rules that participants used. In the second study, we tested the hypothesis that explicit intuitive judgments of man-months saved in a comparison alternative should improve matching judgments of resource savings and decrease the resource saving bias. In the third study, we asked participants to give two resource saving judgments concerning a single production line, improved in two successive steps. We tested the hypothesis that such judgments improve the participants’ judgments and decrease the resource saving bias.

2 General method

2.1 Participants

The participants were recruited from the same population of university students, psychology students at Stockholm university, for each of the three studies. In Study 1, 29 participants took part in the experiment (mean age 25.0) of whom 21 were female. Study 2, included 31 participants (mean age 25.6 years) of whom 24 were female and in Study 3 there were 31 participants (mean age 27.1) of whom 21 were female.Footnote 2 The participants received either a movie ticket voucher or a psychology course credit for their participation. No participant was in more than one study.

2.2 Procedure

The participants completed the questionnaires with the problems in a classroom and were informed that they could take the time they wanted to make their intuitive judgments and solve the problems. The participants used about 15 to 25 minutes to complete the judgments in each study. External decision aids like pens and calculators were not allowed in any of the studies. The order of the problems was counter balanced between participants in each study. The problems, materials and results will be presented separately below for each of the studies.

3 Study 1: Matching two resource improvement options

Based on earlier studies of choice behavior and the time-saving bias (Svenson, 2008, 2010), we predicted that matching judgments should be greater than objectively required when a lower speed was increased to match an increase of a greater speed. When a higher speed was increased to match a change in lower speeds we predicted that the judgments would be too small according to the time-saving bias.

3.1 Material and problems

The questionnaire consisted of 10 problems. Each problem consisted of two production lines, each of which produced 1000 units of a product. The first production line (Line A) had undergone an improvement that saved a certain amount of man-months. For this line, the participant was given the production speed before and after improvement. For the second production line (Line B), the participant was given only the production speed before the improvement, and an empty space for the participant’s judgment. The participant’s task was to judge the increased production rate for line B, which after the improvement would save the same amount of man-months as for Line A when 1000 units were produced (number of units are not important as long as they are not too few as shown in appendix). The following is an example from the questionnaire.

Line A: before improvement 40 units/man-month, after improvement 60 units/man-month,

Line B: before improvement 80 units/man-month, after improvement _______units/man-months.

The participants wrote their judgments in the blank space. At the end of the instruction the participant was informed that the number of units that were produced has no effect on the solutions of the problems. On the last page of the questionnaire, the participants were asked to describe their thoughts while making their judgments.

Table 1 shows the problems and that there were 6 problems (cases, 2,4,6,7,8,9) in which the matching speed was to be increased from a lower speed and 4 problems (cases 1,3,5,10) in which the increase was from a higher speed. As stated above, the time-saving bias predicts that the former cases should give overestimations of correct production speed and the latter underestimations of the speeds needed to match the comparison increase.

Table 1: Average judged production speeds, difference rule predictions, ratio rule predictions, correct solutions and proportional deviations from correct solutions in Study 1 and Study 2. Standard deviations in parentheses.

Note: * = p<.05

** = p<.01

*** = p<.001

one sample t-test for difference between judgment averages and objectively correct answers. Standard deviations in parentheses. Bold digits indicate significant improvements from Study 1 to Study 2, two-tailed t-tests independent samples, p<0.01 (case 6) and p<0.05 (case 10).

3.2 Results

3.2.1 Qualitative measures

No participant was excluded because of judgment outliers or other deviant judgments. The descriptions of how the participants solved the problems were transcribed and then coded independently by two coders. After an initial screening of the solution strategies, the verbal protocols were coded in one and only one of the following categories, (1) ratio rule, (2) difference rule or (3) other rule. The ratio rule was described by Equation (2) and the difference rule by the following equation with the judged increased matching speed in bold.

(4)

The inter coder agreement between the two coders was 23/29 (79.31%) for the participants’ descriptions of how they solved the problems with Kappa = .681, p<.001. The final codings were determined jointly by the two coders and they found that 12 participants reported a ratio rule as their main rule, while 10 of the remaining participants stated that they used a difference rule. The remaining 7 participants did not indicate any of these rules and were classified in the “other rule” category.

3.2.2 Quantitative measures

First, the mean values of the judgments for each problem were compared with the objectively correct values with two-tailed one-sample t-tests. The mean judgments were significantly different from the objectively correct speeds for all problems (Table 1). As predicted, when the initial production speed was lower for line A than for line B, the judged matching increase of B’s production speed was too low. When the initial production speed was higher for line A than for line B, the judged matching production speed was too high. The results show underestimations of the effects on savings in man-months of increasing production rates from 40 units/man-month or less. When the initial productivity was 70 units/man-month or higher, the effects of increases on man-month gains were overestimated. This corroborated earlier findings of time-saving studies (Reference SvensonSvenson, 2008).

The next to the last column in Table 1 shows the average and median proportional deviations from the correct value for each case in Study 1, (judgment - correct value) /correct value. On average the deviations were about +50% when Line B started at a lower speed and about −50% when Line B started at a higher speed. We used regression analysis to find out how well the difference and ratio rules could predict the judgments. The resource saving values predicted from the difference and ratio rules were the two independent variables for each case and the average judgment across participants was the dependent variable, J. The fitted function was J = 0.38·diff + 0.54·ratio + 7.89 (F(2,7) = 5699.5, p<0.001). This shows that both rules were reasonable predictors of the judgments on the average group level and that the ratio rule seemed to give the overall better predictions.

3.2.3 Quantitative and qualitative analysis

We have verbal reports about the rule used by different participants. If these reports are valid, it should be possible to validate the verbal reports in numerical analyses of the judgments (Reference Schulte-Mecklenbeck and RanyardSchulte-Mecklenbeck et al., 2011). Assuming that the 12 participants who reported that they used the ratio rule also did so, this means that if this subgroup is analyzed separately the ratio rule should predict their judgments to a greater extent than when all participants were analyzed. The corresponding holds for the 10 participants who reported using the difference rule.

First, we analyzed the data from the 12 participants in the ratio group. We computed the average for each case and used regression analysis and partial correlations in the same way as for the averages across all participants in the previous section. The regression function J = −0.04·diff + 0.99·ratio + 3.81 (F=(2,7) = 7493.0, p<0.001). This analysis was repeated with the 10 participants in the difference group. The resulting regression function was J = 0.83·diff + 0.10·ratio + 4.59 (F(2,7)= 798.1, p < 0.001). These results verified the validity of the verbal reports.

We wanted to go deeper into the analysis on the individual level and computed partial correlations between judgments and the ratio rule predictions with the difference variable controlled for each participant. Correspondingly, we computed partial correlations between judgments and the difference variable with the ratio variable controlled. The results with the classifications of the verbal reports are shown in Table A in the Appendix. Of the 12 participants who reported the ratio rule all 12 gave judgments that were explained by that rule (p=0.01) and 2 more participants’ judgments clearly showed that they also used the ratio rule without having reported this. The judgments indicated significant use of the difference rule for 7 (p=0.01) of the 10 participants who reported that they used the difference rule. The partial correlations with at least a 0.01 significance were used to group the participants into a ratio and a difference group. The average absolute difference, |judgment−correct value| in the ratio group was 73.96 (3.35) and in the difference group 85.82 (19.94). This group difference was significant (t = 2.21, p<0.05, two-tailed), illustrating that the ratio rule gives judgments that are closer to correct than the difference rule.

In conclusion, the resource-saving bias is a kind of time-saving biases found in earlier studies (Svenson, 2008, 2011) and it was replicated in the present studies as a matching bias. The systematic matching bias could be derived from at least two different cognitively controlled judgment rules, the difference rule and the ratio rule (we may assume largely System 2 processes) each of which was favored by one subgroup of participants. Both rules give biased estimates of resource savings.

In the following studies, we tested two ways of counteracting or eliminating the time or resource-saving bias in productivity increase judgments. Because most participants in Study 1 seemed to use controlled System 2 strategies, we used debiasing techniques based on reframing of problems enabling a participant her- or himself to gain insight into her or his own biases, enabling a change of rule for improvement.

One reason for the bias may be that people do not acknowledge how much resources are saved by the already known productivity increase in line A before they proceed to a comparison and this will be focused in the next study. The participants in this study were all recruited from the same pool of students at Stockholm University as in Study 1 and none had participated in Study 1.

4 Study 2: Matching preceded by judgments of resource savings

In this study, we encouraged the participants to estimate the man-months saved from the already implemented improvement of the first line A, before they judged the speed increase needed in B to match the resource saving of A.

4.1 Material and problems

We used the same 10 problems as in Study. Again, 1000 units were produced by each production line. The production speeds before and after an improvement were given for A and the participants judged the increased speed of B that would match the resource saving of A.

In this attempt to improve judgment performance, we asked the participants to estimate the man-months saved from the already known improvement of A before they judged the speed of the second line. Thus, the instruction to the participants was quite similar to the instruction given in Study 1 but, it also included the following information.

Your task is twofold: (1) First, estimate how many man-months line A will save from the improvement (from the low production speed to the highest speed). (2) Estimate in units/man-month the increased production speed of line B that would save the same amount of man-months as the improvement of line A.

The instruction explained the problems and the concept of man-month before the problem set was presented to the participants. When solving a problem a participant was not allowed to use external aids and told that the experimenter wanted her or his own unaided judgments.

4.2 Results

We excluded three participants who did not solve 90% or more of the problems, leaving judgments from 31 participants for analysis. Three outliers further than 4 SD from the mean judgment of a problem were treated as missing data. Table 1 shows that the average judgments deviated from correct values in the same directions as in Study 1, but the average judgment was not significantly different from the correct value for problem 4. We compared each problem in Study 1 and Study 2 and found that 2 judgments averages of the 10 problems improved significantly (two tailed t-tests, p=0.05). Hence, making a participant aware of the normatively logical first step of computing the saving for an existing alternative before giving a matching judgment for its alternative improved what may be called System 2 performance somewhat.

As in Study 1, we computed partial correlations for each participant between her or his judgments and the ratio and difference rule predictions and the average absolute deviation of the judgments from the correct values. The results (Appendix Table B) showed a decreased frequency of the difference rule. The ratio rule was significant at the 0.01 level for 12 of the 31 participants.

The resource-saving bias indicates that people do not understand the importance of the production speed from which an increase takes place. The purpose of Study 3 was to attempt to further improve intuitive judgments by having participants judge resource savings of the same production line after each of two steps of production speed increase (from P 1 to P 2 and from P 2 to P 3). In such problems, there are two production speed increases starting from two different speeds, just as in the matching task, but with one production line instead of two different lines. With this re-framing of the problem, a judge may become aware of the different effects of increases from a low and a high production speed.

5 Study 3: Matching session preceded by a preparatory session of successive resource saving judgments

Study 3 was designed to have participants themselves improve their judgment strategies in a way different from Study 2. We gave participants an initial set of problems in which it becomes obvious to a problem solver that neither the difference nor the ratio rule give reasonable solutions. The problems concerned two successive improvements of production speeds at the same plant and illustrate that the ratio rule gives solutions that are wrong. The difference rule is less complex than the ratio rule because the ratio rule includes the difference in the numerator. The difference rule gives poorer judgments than the ratio rule, and we designed the learning problems so that they should also illustrate that the difference rule is incorrect. We presented a separate and different set of resource saving problems in a separate session before the participants matched the 10 resource saving problems used in Studies 1 and 2.

Specifically, we predicted that, if the participants made judgments of only one production line with two successive production speed increases, they should become aware of the fact that the same proportional speed or difference improvement from a lower original production speed saves more resources than the same proportional or difference improvement from higher speeds. This is an important insight if one wants to counteract the production or time-saving bias. In this way, both the difference and ratio rules can be challenged by the participants themselves when they judge successive improvements. To exemplify, if an industry increases production speed from 20 units/man-hour to 40 units/man-hour it would save 50% of the resources. If the industry then increases production speed further to 80 units/man-hour, it would save 50% of the resources from the production efficiency already attained after the prior increase (and this is much less than the saving followed by the first speed increase). Hence, the two 50% speed increases do not give the same resource savings as predicted by the ratio rule.

5.1 Material and problems

The first set of problems presented a production line for 1000 units that had just been improved from one production rate (e.g., 30 units/man-month) to another (e.g., 40 units/man-month). The participants were asked to judge the resources saved in terms of worker man-months from this already implemented efficiency improvement. After having made a judgment of the man-months saved by the already implemented speed increase to 40 units/man-month, the participant was informed that the present speed was insufficient and that a second efficiency improvement was planned for the same line to reach an even higher production speed (e.g., 60 units/man-month). How many man-months would this plan save compared to the current production speed? The problems included the following speed triplets with speeds used in the previous studies: 40,60,80; 30,70,110; 40,50,80; 40,80,90; 30,40,70; 30,70,130; 40,80,120; 40,80,100; 40,80,150: 40,70,80. The participants were recruited in the same way as in the earlier studies and no participant had taken part in the previous studies. The participants judged the saving of one problem and turned to the next problem. After having completed this learning task, the participants solved the same problems as in Study 2.

5.2 Results

All participants solved more than 90% of the problems. None was excluded. Five outliers further than 4 SD from the mean judgment of a problem were eliminated from the data. The average judgments in Table 2 illustrate the time-saving bias with all averages differing from the correct values in the direction predicted by the time-saving bias. We performed two tailed t-tests (p=0.001) to compare Study 1 and Study 3 case by case. The results showed that the average judgments in Study 3 were significantly better than the averages in Study 1 (bold digits in Table 2) except for Case 4. The proportional deviations from correct values illustrate that the participants made improvements compared to the performance in the other studies.

Table 2: Average judged production speeds, difference rule predictions, ratio rule predictions, correct solutions and average and median proportional deviations from correct solutions in Study 1 and Study 3.

Note: * = p<.05

** = p<.01

*** = p<.001

two sample t-test differences from correct values. Standard deviations in parentheses. Bold digits indicate significant improvements from Study 1 to Study 3, two-tailed t-tests independent samples, p < 0.01 for all cases except p< 0.05 for case 8, and n.s. for case 4.

The time-saving bias predicts overestimation of production resources saved from low production speeds and underestimations from high speeds. Therefore, we split the problems in two sets of problems, increases from low and high speeds respectively and illustrated the data separately in these sets. Figure 1 gives the median proportional deviations for increases from low speeds in the top bars showing that the underestimation of the effect of a speed increases from a low speed and that the time-saving bias approaches zero and decreases over studies. Correspondingly, the effects of speed increases from high speeds are overestimated, judged speed changes will be too small as shown by the lower bars in Figure 1. However, the time-saving bias was not completely eliminated.

Figure 1: Median proportional deviations from correct answers in Study 1, 2 and 3. The upper bars describe median proportional deviations for the problems where the slower reference speed of the reference alternative A was greater than the slower speed of B (to be matched). Correspondingly, the lower bars describe median proportional deviations of problems with the lower reference speed in A slower than the lower speed of B.

As in Study 1 we computed partial correlations for each participant. The results (Table B in appendix) showed a decreased frequency of the difference rule in comparison with Study 1 and only 2 partial correlations of 31 correlations were significant (p<0.01). The ratio rule was significant (p=0.01) for 17 of the 31 participants. However, the ratio rule and the correct rule are highly correlated (for this set of cases r=0.89, p=0.001) and therefore we compared the absolute deviations from correct values as well and they were significantly smaller than those of Study 2. A one-way ANOVA of the mean average absolute deviations from correct values for the 3 studies (means 80.4, 79.3 and 53.9 for Study 1, 2 and 3) were significantly different, F(2:86) = 7.97, p<0.001. Post hoc Scheffe t-tests showed significance between Study 3 and Study 2 (p<0.01) and between Study 1 and Study 3 (p<0.01) and no significance between the first two studies.

6 Discussion

The studies illustrated that the time/resource-saving bias found in choices between efficiency improvements with the purpose of maximizing resource savings also applies to judgments. In search of the mental processes behind the resource saving bias, we found that verbal protocols identified two System 2 rules, the difference and the ratio rule, both of which give biased judgments of resource savings. Analyses of verbal protocols should be supported by analyses of other kinds of data (Reference Ranyard and SvensonRanyard & Svenson, 2011) and therefore, the verbal protocol results were validated in regression and correlation analyses based on numerical responses. The time/resource saving bias persisted and decreased only slightly when participants were asked to judge the resources saved for a reference alternative before solving a matching problem. But the bias could be weakened significantly by having the participants make judgments of the effects of successive production speed increases of one single production site before comparing alternatives in a matching session, although the systematic error was not eliminated completely.

It is interesting to compare these approaches to that of Reference Peer and GamlielPeer and Gamliel (2013), who re-framed the problem and instead of speed (mph) and gave information about how long it would take to complete a given distance (min/mile). This kind of information was also used by Eriksson et al. (2013) in a driving simulator study. Both studies show that, even if mph or km/h are more natural ways of expressing speed than the inverses, the latter drastically decreases or eliminates the time saving bias.

When small groups of participants are compared, as they are here, uncontrolled group differences can influence the results. However, the time-saving bias is a very strong effect, and the statistical power in these studies is high. Also, the results were strong with participants randomly assigned to Study 2 or 3 and tested simultaneously. All of this makes between-studies uncontrolled effects unlikely.

We used two measures of judgmental accuracy in the studies, average absolute deviation from correct values (Appendix Tables A, B and C) and proportional differences (Tables 1 and 2, Figure 1). From an applied perspective, a saved man-month is the always the same and therefore average absolute deviation from correct value is a proper measure. From a psychological perspective, we know since the days of Fechner that judgmental precision is related to magnitude, so we also used the proportional difference from correct value as a measure. These measures gave slightly different views on the judgments for Study 2. Proportional differences suggested that performance was slightly better than did the absolute difference measure.

In conclusion, part of the bias depends on the application of inadequate judgment rules, such as the difference and ratio rules. Interestingly, the participants themselves could improve their strategies and judgments after having solved only a small number of related problems. This result supports the suggestion from Study 1 that the production speed saving bias is more of a System 2 than a System 1 phenomenon, in contrasts to other heuristics like the representativeness and affect heuristics, which are seen as primarily in System 1 (Reference Tversky and KahnemanTversky & Kahneman, 1974; Reference Slovic, Finucane, Peters and MacGregorSlovic, Finucane, Peters, & MacGregor, 2007).

From an applied perspective, productivity and increase of productivity of any activity is of general and increasing social concern. Even if there are many formal systems to measure productivity (Reference Brown and GobeliBrown & Gobeli, 1992), unaided calculations of the effects of productivity changes are important for final decisions (Reference SvensonSvenson, 2003). Reformulations of the problems can improve those calculations to the benefit of those of us who are not experts in indicators of productivity. In this context it may be worth pointing out that stopping of a less productive system and investment of all resources in a more productive system is not always the best decision strategy if one wants to maximize savings of available resources. When a problem involves the effects of increases in speed, decision makers should be warned about the time /resource saving bias and informed about possibilities to reformulate a problem before they make their decisions.

Appendix

The man-hour gain, called G from an improvement of a production line is

(5)

where U is the number of units to be produced, P 1 and P 2 are the production speeds in units/man hour for before and after the change.

Thus, when changes in production speeds are equal for line A and line B, the following equality applies with P A1 as initial production speed of A and P A2 the increased speed of A and P B1 as initial production speed of B and P B2 the increased speed of B:

(6)

Equation (6) applies only if U > 0 and U/P B1 > U/P A1U/P A2 and U/P A1 > U/P B1U/P B2. The latter conditions apply because if the man-hour gain in one line is greater than the total man-hours needed to produce the same number of units before improvement in the other line, then there are not enough man-hours available to be saved when matching the two alternatives. Division by U gives

(7)

Note that the total number of units produced is of no importance to the choice between options as long as the conditions specified above apply.

Assume that you want to calculate the increased productivity, P B2, needed to give line B the same man-hour gain as the production improvement of line A. The answer can be derived from equation (7):

(8)

The formula seems complex for exact unaided judgments but if the problems are presented as in the Svenson (2011) problem scenario described earlier, they become easy to apprehend and seemingly not difficult to judge. When participants are asked to make two different productivity increases equal by choosing the increased speed needed to achieve this end, it is possible to rewrite Equation (8) in the following way:

(9)

In the studies of time-savings when driving, the participant’s judgments followed Equation (10) with production speed corresponding to driving speed (Reference SvensonSvenson, 2008). Note, that the difference between equation (9) and (10) lies in the fact that people were unable to adjust for both P A1 and P B1 in the denominators.

(10)

Table A: Study 1: Codings of verbal protocols and partial correlations for ratio and difference rule predictions. When the ratio rule applies so well that there is no variance left for the other rule no partial correlation can be computed. This was indicated by - in the table. When a verbal protocol could not be coded this was also marked. Last column shows average absolute deviations from correct rule predictions.

The symbols ***, ** and * indicate 0.001, 0.01 and 0.05 significance levels and # incomplete data set (judgments missing or outliers for 3 problems).

Table B: Study 2: Partial correlations for predictions from ratio and difference rules. The last column gives average absolute deviations from correct values.

The symbols ***, ** and * indicate 0.001, 0.01 and 0.05 significance levels.

Table C: Study 3: Partial correlations for predictions from ratio and difference rules. The last column gives average absolute deviations from correct values.

The symbols ***, ** and * indicate 0.001, 0.01 and 0.05 significance levels and # incomplete data set (judgments missing or outliers for 3 problems).

Footnotes

The Swedish Research Council supported this research.

The symbols ***, ** and * indicate 0.001, 0.01 and 0.05 significance levels and # incomplete data set (judgments missing or outliers for 3 problems).

The symbols ***, ** and * indicate 0.001, 0.01 and 0.05 significance levels.

The symbols ***, ** and * indicate 0.001, 0.01 and 0.05 significance levels and # incomplete data set (judgments missing or outliers for 3 problems).

1 The dichotomy has been criticized (Reference Keren and SchulKeren & Schul, 2009; Reference Kruglanski and GigerenzerKruglanski & Gigerenzer, 2011) on different grounds, e.g., the difficulty of finding criteria for a clear separation of the two systems. According to Evans (2008, p. 255), a number of authors have suggested that there may be two architecturally (and evolutionarily) distinct cognitive systems underlying dual-process models. “However, it emerges that (a) there are multiple kinds of implicit processes described by different theorists and (b) not all of the proposed attributes of the two kinds of processing can be sensibly mapped on to two systems as currently conceived. It is suggested that while some dual-process theories are concerned with parallel competing processes involving explicit and implicit knowledge systems, others are concerned with the influence of preconscious processes that contextualize and shape deliberative reasoning and decision-making.”

2 The time-saving bias was investigated earlier in driving contexts and was found to be quite strong. To illustrate, Svenson (2009, Table 2) investigated matching a speed increase from 60 to 110 km/h with a speed increase from 30 km/h, which gave an average judgment of 66 km/h (SD=30.6). The correct speed is 40 km/h which gives an effect size of 0.85 SD. This means that for p = 0.05, 0.01, and 0.001 a minimum of 17, 24 and 32 participants would be needed to detect the phenomenon with a power of 0.90 at these significance levels. In Study 1 we investigated 29 participants and found significance for all problems (9 problems with p<0.001 and 1 problem with p<0.01). However, in the present investigation we were interested not only in detecting the phenomenon but also in detecting differences in the strength of the phenomenon in Studies 2 and 3. We used the average absolute proportional difference between judgments and correct values and the standard deviations for all problems as shown in Table 1. The average absolute proportional difference was 0.51 and we decided that we wanted to detect at least an improvement down to 0.35 deviation from the correct value (0.00 assuming no random error). With 29 participants in Study 1 the power to detect this change between Study 1 and Study 2 and 3 would be 0.874 with 30 participants in each of the other studies assuming constant standard deviations and alpha = 0.05.

References

Brehmer, B. (1971). Subjects’ ability to use functional rules. Psychonomic Science, 24(6), 259260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B., Hagafors, R., & Johansson, R. (1980). Cognitive skills in judgment: Subjects’ ability to use information about weights, function forms, and organizing principles. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 26(3), 373385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, W. B. & Gobeli, D. (1992). Observations on the measurement of R & D productivity: A case condition. IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management, 39, 325331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cronin, M. A., Gonzalez, C., & Sterman, J. D. (2009). Why don’t well-educated adultsunderstand accumulation? A challenge to researchers, educators, and citizens. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(1), 116130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutt, V., & Gonzalez, C. (2013). Reducing the linear perception of nonlinearity: Use of a physical representation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(1), 5167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebersbach, M., Lehner, M., Resing, W., & Wilkening, F. (2008). Forecasting exponential growth and exponential decline: Similarities and differences. Acta psychologica, 127(2), 247257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksson, G., Svenson, O. & Eriksson, L. (2013). The time-saving bias: Judgements, cognitionand perception. Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 492497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American psychologist, 49(8), 709724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, R., Gormley, M., Stradling, S., Broughton, P., Kinnear, N., O’Dolan, C., & Hannigan, B.(2009). Impact of speed change on estimated journey time: Failure of drivers to appreciate relevance of initial speed. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 41(1), 1014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. The American economic review, 93(5), 14491475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (2009). Two Is Not Always Better Than One A Critical Evaluation of Two-System Theories. Perspectives on psychological science, 4(6), 533550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W., & Gigerenzer, G. (2011). Intuitive and deliberate judgments are based on common principles. Psychological review, 118(1), 97109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larrick, R. P. & Soll, J. B.(2008). The MPG illusion. Science, 320(5883), 1993–1594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peer, E., & Gamliel, E. (2012). Estimating time-savings: The use of the proportion and percentage heuristics and the role of need for cognition. Acta Psychologica, 141, 352359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peer, E., & Gamliel, E. (2013). Pace yourself: Improving time-saving judgments when increasing activity speed. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(2), 106115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ranyard, R., & Svenson, O. (2011). Verbal data and decision process analysis. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kuehlberger, & R. Ranyard (Eds.) A Handbook of process tracingmethods for decision research, (pp. 115137). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Resnick, L. B. & Singer, J. A. (1993). Protoquantitative origins of ratio reasoning. In T. P. Carpenter, E. Fennema & T. A. Romberg (Eds.) Rational numbers: An integration of research. Studies in mathematical thinking and learning., (pp. 107130). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M. Kuehlberger, A. & Ranyard, R. (Eds.) (2011). A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for behavioral economics. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 31(4), 329342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2007). The affect heuristic. European Journal of Operational Research, 177(3), 13331352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svenson, O. (1970). A functional measurement approach to intuitive estimation as exemplified by estimated time-savings. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 204210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svenson, O. (1971). Changing the structure of intuitive estimates of time-savings. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 12, 131134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svenson, O. (2003). Values, affect and processes in human decision making: a differentiation and consolidation theory perspective. In S. L. Schneider & J. Shanteau (Eds.) Emerging Perspectives on Judgment and Decision research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, (pp. 287326).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svenson, O. (2008). Decisions among time-saving options: When intuition is strong and wrong. Acta Psychologica, 127, 501509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svenson, O. (2011). Biased decisions concerning productivity increase options. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32, 440445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 11241131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagenaar, W. A., (1975). Misperception of exponential growth. Perception & Psychophysics, 18(6), 416422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1: Average judged production speeds, difference rule predictions, ratio rule predictions, correct solutions and proportional deviations from correct solutions in Study 1 and Study 2. Standard deviations in parentheses.

Figure 1

Table 2: Average judged production speeds, difference rule predictions, ratio rule predictions, correct solutions and average and median proportional deviations from correct solutions in Study 1 and Study 3.

Figure 2

Figure 1: Median proportional deviations from correct answers in Study 1, 2 and 3. The upper bars describe median proportional deviations for the problems where the slower reference speed of the reference alternative A was greater than the slower speed of B (to be matched). Correspondingly, the lower bars describe median proportional deviations of problems with the lower reference speed in A slower than the lower speed of B.

Figure 3

Table A: Study 1: Codings of verbal protocols and partial correlations for ratio and difference rule predictions. When the ratio rule applies so well that there is no variance left for the other rule no partial correlation can be computed. This was indicated by - in the table. When a verbal protocol could not be coded this was also marked. Last column shows average absolute deviations from correct rule predictions.

Figure 4

Table B: Study 2: Partial correlations for predictions from ratio and difference rules. The last column gives average absolute deviations from correct values.

Figure 5

Table C: Study 3: Partial correlations for predictions from ratio and difference rules. The last column gives average absolute deviations from correct values.

Supplementary material: File

Svenson et al. supplementary material

Svenson et al. supplementary material
Download Svenson et al. supplementary material(File)
File 13.2 KB