Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:31:05.270Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Authors' reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Nav Kapur
Affiliation:
Centre for Suicide Prevention, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, and Greater Manchester Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK
Leah Quinlivan
Affiliation:
Centre for Suicide Prevention, Manchester Academic Health Science Centre, University of Manchester, Manchester
Sarah Steeg
Affiliation:
Institute of Brain, Behaviour and Mental Health, Faculty of Medical and Human Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester
Rory O'Connor
Affiliation:
Institute of Health and Wellbeing, University of Glasgow, Glasgow
Keith Hawton
Affiliation:
Centre for Suicide Research, University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford
David Gunnell
Affiliation:
School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2017 

We thank Professor Fazel and Dr Wolf for their thoughtful letter and interest in our article. Reference Quinlivan, Cooper, Meehan, Longson, Potokar and Hulme1 Well, at least we agree on one thing: randomised controlled trials could help to clarify the role of risk assessment scales in the management of people who have harmed themselves. However, we do not think that future research should investigate scores ‘with or without additional clinician input’. Risk should not simply be a score or a colour on a traffic light system. In isolation, such ratings may be worse than useless, especially if they distract clinicians from engaging with their patients. An alternative design might be to investigate what risk scales add to ‘assessment as usual’.

Just to be clear, we are not suggesting that global clinician or patient judgement should be used in preference to rating scales to predict future self-harm. None of the measures is fit for this purpose in our view. Global risk assessments by clinicians have been found to perform poorly in previous studies. Reference Kapur, Cooper, Rodway, Kelly, Guthrie and Mackway-Jones2 With colleagues in Australia some of us are currently engaged in a systematic review of this very issue. We think that Fazel & Wolf's observation that there was less variability in the scores on the scales than in the clinician ratings is an interesting one. The problem was that in our study the scales actually performed, on average, a bit worse than the global measures. There may be circumstances when scales are useful, of course. For example, as an aide memoire for new staff or as measures of change.

How might we explain the fact that risk scales performed even less well than unvalidated single-item clinician and patient measures? The obvious explanation is that the risk scales themselves were not very good. But there are other possibilities too. For the clinician ratings, we acknowledged in our discussion that the centres all had a special interest in self-harm and that predictive performance might be different in other hospitals. We also mentioned that clinicians might subliminally have used items on the scales to derive their global assessments. Fazel & Wolf's idea that patients were also taking such factors into account as the assessment progressed is intriguing and certainly worth exploring in future studies. It could be that we gain useful new insights by asking our patients how they make their own judgements of risk.

There have been a number of large systematic reviews recently all pointing in the same direction. Reference Chan, Bhatti, Meader, Stockton, Evans and O'Connor3,Reference Quinlivan, Cooper, Davies, Hawton, Gunnell and Kapur4 In their editorial on our paper, Owens & Kelley highlighted the poor to worthless performance of all measures. Reference Owens and Kelley5 They also asked whether risk assessment scales were on their way out. Over to readers of the BJPsych

References

1 Quinlivan, L, Cooper, J, Meehan, D, Longson, D, Potokar, J, Hulme, T, et al. Predictive accuracy of risk scales following self-harm: multicentre, prospective cohort study. Br J Psychiatry 2017; 210: 429–36.Google Scholar
2 Kapur, N, Cooper, J, Rodway, C, Kelly, J, Guthrie, E, Mackway-Jones, K. Predicting the risk of repetition after self harm: cohort study. BMJ 2005; 330: 394–5.Google Scholar
3 Chan, MKY, Bhatti, H, Meader, N, Stockton, S, Evans, J, O'Connor, RC, et al. Predicting suicide following self-harm: systematic review of risk factors and risk scales. Br J Psychiatry 2016; 209: 277–83.Google Scholar
4 Quinlivan, L, Cooper, J, Davies, L, Hawton, K, Gunnell, D, Kapur, N. Which are the most useful scales for predicting repeat self-harm? A systematic review evaluating risk scales using measures of diagnostic accuracy. BMJ Open 2016; 6: e009297-e.Google Scholar
5 Owens, D, Kelley, R. Predictive properties of risk assessment instruments following self-harm. Br J Psychiatry 2017; 210: 384–6.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.