Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:43:05.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

THE MUTUAL INTELLIGIBILITY OF L2 SPEECH

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 February 2006

Murray J. Munro
Affiliation:
Simon Fraser University
Tracey M. Derwing
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
Susan L. Morton
Affiliation:
Simon Fraser University

Abstract

When understanding or evaluating foreign-accented speech, listeners are affected not only by properties of the speech itself but by their own linguistic backgrounds and their experience with different speech varieties. Given the latter influence, it is not known to what degree a diverse group of listeners might share a response to second language (L2) speech. In this study, listeners from native Cantonese, Japanese, Mandarin, and English backgrounds evaluated the same set of foreign-accented English utterances from native speakers of Cantonese, Japanese, Polish, and Spanish. Regardless of native language background, the listener groups showed moderate to high correlations on intelligibility scores and comprehensibility and accentedness ratings. Although some between-group differences emerged, the groups tended to agree on which of the 48 speakers were the easiest and most difficult to understand; between-group effect sizes were generally small. As in previous studies, the listeners did not consistently exhibit an intelligibility benefit for speech produced in their own accent. These findings support the view that properties of the speech itself are a potent factor in determining how L2 speech is perceived, even when the listeners are from diverse language backgrounds.This work was supported by two grants from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada to the first two authors. The authors thank J. E. Flege and three anonymous SSLA reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of the paper as well as the listeners and speakers for their willingness to participate in the study. The listening stimuli used here were also used in a paper published in SSLA in 1997.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson-Hsieh, J., Johnson, R., & Koehler, K. (1992). The relationship between native speaker judgments of nonnative pronunciation and deviance in segmentals, prosody, and syllable structure. Language Learning, 42, 529555.Google Scholar
Anderson-Hsieh, J. & Koehler, K. (1988). The effect of foreign accent and speaking rate on native speaker comprehension. Language Learning, 38, 561613.Google Scholar
Asher, J. & Garcia, R. (1969). The optimal age to learn a foreign language. Modern Language Journal, 53, 334341.Google Scholar
Bent, T. & Bradlow, A.R. (2003). The interlanguage speech intelligibility benefit. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114, 16001610.Google Scholar
Breitkreutz, J.A., Derwing, T.M., & Rossiter, M.J. (2001). Pronunciation teaching practices in Canada. TESL Canada Journal, 19, 5161.Google Scholar
Brennan, E. & Brennan, J. (1981). Measurements of accent and attitude toward Mexican-American speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10, 487501.Google Scholar
Brodkey, D. (1972). Dictation as a measure of mutual intelligibility: A pilot study. Language Learning, 22, 203220.Google Scholar
Burda, A., Scherz, J., Hageman, C., & Edwards, H. (2003). Age and understanding of speakers with Spanish or Taiwanese accents. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 1120.Google Scholar
Dávila, A., Bohara, A., & Saenz, R. (1993). Accent penalties and the earnings of Mexican Americans. Social Science Quarterly, 74, 902915.Google Scholar
Derwing, T.M. & Munro, M.J. (1997). Accent, intelligibility, and comprehensibility: Evidence from four L1s. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 116.Google Scholar
Derwing, T.M. & Munro, M.J. (2005). Second language accent and pronunciation teaching: A research-based approach. TESOL Quarterly, 39, 379397.Google Scholar
Derwing, T.M., Munro, M.J., & Carbonaro, M. (2000). Does popular speech recognition software work with ESL speech? TESOL Quarterly, 34, 592603.Google Scholar
Derwing, T.M., Rossiter, M.J., & Munro, M.J. (2002). Teaching native speakers to listen to foreign-accented speech. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 23, 245259.Google Scholar
Flege, J.E. (1988a). Factors affecting degree of perceived foreign accent in English sentences. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84, 7079.Google Scholar
Flege, J.E. (1988b). The production and perception of foreign language speech sounds. In H. Winitz (Ed.), Human communication and its disorders: A review—1988 (pp. 224401). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Flege, J.E. & Fletcher, K.L. (1992). Talker and listener effects on degree of perceived foreign accent. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91, 370389.Google Scholar
Flege, J.E., Munro, M.J., & Mackay, I.R.A. (1995). Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 31253134.Google Scholar
Gass, S. & Varonis, E. (1984). The effect of familiarity on the comprehensibility of nonnative speech. Language Learning, 34, 6589.Google Scholar
Hazan, V. & Markham, D. (2004). Acoustic-phonetic correlates of talker intelligibility for adults and children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116, 31083118.Google Scholar
Howell, D.C. (1992). Statistical methods for psychology (3rd ed.). Boston: PWS-Kent Publishing Co.
Kalin, R. & Rayko, D. (1978). Discrimination in evaluative judgments against foreign-accented job candidates. Psychological Reports, 43, 12031209.Google Scholar
Kent, R., Miolo, G., & Bloedel, S. (1994). The intelligibility of children's speech: A review of evaluation procedures. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 3, 8195.Google Scholar
Lippi-Green, R. (1997). English with an accent: Language, ideology, and discrimination in the United States. London: Routledge.
Mackay, I.R.A., Flege, J.E., & Imai, S., (in press). Evaluating the effects of chronological age and sentence duration on degree of perceived foreign accent. Applied Psycholinguistics.
Magen, H. (1998). The perception of foreign-accented speech. Journal of Phonetics, 26, 381400.Google Scholar
Major, R. (2001). Foreign accent: The ontogeny and phylogeny of second language phonology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Major, R., Fitzmaurice, S., Bunta, F., & Balasubramanian, C. (2002). The effects of nonnative accents on listening comprehension: Implications for ESL assessment. TESOL Quarterly, 36, 173190.Google Scholar
Munro, M.J. (2003). A primer on accent discrimination in the Canadian context. TESL Canada Journal, 20, 3851.Google Scholar
Munro, M.J. (2005). Foreign accent and speech intelligibility. Manuscript in preparation.
Munro, M.J. & Derwing, T.M. (1994). Evaluations of foreign accent in extemporaneous and read material. Language Testing, 3, 253266.Google Scholar
Munro, M.J. & Derwing, T.M. (1995). Processing time, accent, and comprehensibility in the perception of native and foreign-accented speech. Language and Speech, 38, 289306.Google Scholar
Munro, M.J. & Derwing, T.M. (1999). Foreign accent, comprehensibility, and intelligibility in the speech of second language learners. Language Learning, 49, 285310.Google Scholar
Munro, M.J. & Derwing, T.M. (2001). Modeling perceptions of the accentedness and comprehensibility of L2 speech: The role of speaking rate. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 451468.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, M. (1989). Intelligibility rating of L2 speech pre- and postintervention. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 515521.Google Scholar
Raisler, I. (1976). Differential response to the same message delivered by native and foreign speakers. Foreign Language Annals, 9, 256259.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. (1992). Nonlanguage factors affecting undergraduates' judgments of nonnative English-speaking teaching assistants. Research in Higher Education, 33, 511531.Google Scholar
Scovel, T. (1988). A time to speak: A psycholinguistic investigation into the critical period for human speech. New York: Harper and Row.
Shrout, P.E. & Fleiss, J.L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 2, 420428.Google Scholar
Smith, L. & Bisazza, J. (1982). The comprehensibility of three varieties of English for college students in seven countries. Language Learning, 32, 259269.Google Scholar
Smith, L. & Rafiqzad, K. (1979). English for cross-cultural communication: The question of intelligibility. TESOL Quarterly, 13, 371380.Google Scholar
Southwood, M. & Flege, J.E. (1999). Scaling foreign accent: Direct magnitude estimation versus interval scaling. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 13, 335349.Google Scholar
Thompson, I. (1991). Foreign accents revisited: The English pronunciation of Russian immigrants. Language Learning, 41, 177204.Google Scholar
Zielinski, B. (2004, May). Measurement of speech intelligibility: What are we actually measuring? Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Association for Applied Linguistics, Portland, OR.