Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:42:00.344Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A voxel- and source-based morphometry analysis of grey matter volume differences in very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 August 2023

Lies Van Assche
Affiliation:
Geriatric Psychiatry, University Psychiatric Center KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Neuropsychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, Leuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Akihiro Takamiya*
Affiliation:
Neuropsychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, Leuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
Jan Van den Stock
Affiliation:
Geriatric Psychiatry, University Psychiatric Center KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Neuropsychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, Leuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Luc Van de Ven
Affiliation:
Geriatric Psychiatry, University Psychiatric Center KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Patrick Luyten
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Research Department of Clinical Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Louise Emsell
Affiliation:
Geriatric Psychiatry, University Psychiatric Center KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Neuropsychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, Leuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Translational MRI, Department of Imaging and Pathology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Mathieu Vandenbulcke
Affiliation:
Geriatric Psychiatry, University Psychiatric Center KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Neuropsychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, Leuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
*
Corresponding author: Akihiro Takamiya; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis (VLOSLP) is associated with significant burden. Its clinical importance is increasing as the global population of older adults rises, yet owing to limited research in this population, the neurobiological underpinnings of VLOSP remain insufficiently clarified. Here we address this knowledge gap using novel morphometry techniques to investigate grey matter volume (GMV) differences between VLOSLP and healthy older adults, and their correlations with neuropsychological scores.

Methods

In this cross-sectional study, we investigated whole-brain GMV differences between 35 individuals with VLOSLP (mean age 76.7, 26 female) and 36 healthy controls (mean age 75.7, 27 female) using whole-brain voxel-based morphometry (VBM) and supplementary source-based morphometry (SBM) on high resolution 3D T1-weighted MRI images. Additionally, we investigated relationships between GMV differences and cognitive function assessed with an extensive neuropsychological battery.

Results

VBM showed lower GMV in the thalamus, left inferior frontal gyrus and left insula in patients with VLOSLP compared to healthy controls. SBM revealed lower thalamo-temporal GMV in patients with VLOSLP. Processing speed, selective attention, mental flexibility, working memory, verbal memory, semantic fluency and confrontation naming were impaired in patients with VLOSLP. Correlations between thalamic volumes and memory function were significant within the group of individuals with VLOSLP, whereas no significant associations remained in the healthy controls.

Conclusions

Lower GMV in the thalamus and fronto-temporal regions may be part of the underlying neurobiology of VLOSLP, with lower thalamic GMV contributing to memory impairment in the disorder.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Although schizophrenia generally surfaces in adolescence, several studies report on individuals who first experienced psychosis very late in life, in the absence of a mood disorder or a neurological illness (Sharma, Debsikdar, Naphade, & Shetty, Reference Sharma, Debsikdar, Naphade and Shetty2014). Individuals with an onset of psychotic symptoms after 60 years are referred to as very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis (VLOSLP) according to an international expert consensus (Howard, Rabins, Seeman, Jeste, & The International Late-Onset Schizophrenia Group, Reference Howard, Rabins, Seeman and Jeste2000). Compared with early-onset schizophrenia (EOS), VLOSLP is mainly characterised by positive psychotic symptoms, whereas affective blunting and disorganisation are usually absent. The community prevalence of VLOSLP is only 0.1% to 0.5% (Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Dewey, Scott, Gilmore, Larkin, Cleave and McKibbin1998). However, there is a linear trend in the relationship between age and onset of non-organic and non-affective psychosis after the age of 60, exhibiting an 11% increase in VLOSLP with each 5 year increase in age (van Os, Howard, Takei, & Murray, Reference van Os, Howard, Takei and Murray1995). As the older age groups are the fastest growing section of the world population, healthcare may thus be increasingly confronted with a first onset of psychosis in elderly patients. Further elucidation of the neurobiological mechanisms of VLOSLP, also leading to debilitating symptoms such as cognitive and functional impairment, is urgently needed.

There is only limited research on neurobiological changes specifically in individuals with VLOSLP. Most research was conducted in samples of individuals with late onset schizophrenia (>40 years; LOS) or a mixed group of individuals with LOS and VLOSLP (Van Assche, Morrens, Luyten, Van de Ven, & Vandenbulcke, Reference Van Assche, Morrens, Luyten, Van de Ven and Vandenbulcke2017). These studies have reported an increased ventricle-to-brain ratio and larger third ventricles (Corey-Bloom, Jernigan, Archibald, Harris, & Jeste, Reference Corey-Bloom, Jernigan, Archibald, Harris and Jeste1995; Lesser et al., Reference Lesser, Miller, Swartz, Boone, Mehringer and Mena1993; Rabins, Pearlson, Jayaram, Steele, & Tune, Reference Rabins, Pearlson, Jayaram, Steele and Tune1987), decreased volumes in the amygdala, entorhinal cortex, hippocampus and anterior superior temporal gyrus (Barta et al., Reference Barta, Powers, Aylward, Chase, Harris, Rabins and Pearlson1997; Casanova, Reference Casanova2010; Sachdev, Brodaty, Cheang, & Cathcart, Reference Sachdev, Brodaty, Cheang and Cathcart2000), subcortical volume as well as temporal lobe volume reductions (Howard, Förstl, Almeida, Burns, & Levy, Reference Howard, Förstl, Almeida, Burns and Levy1992a; Howard, Förstl, Naguib, Burns, & Levy, Reference Howard, Förstl, Naguib, Burns and Levy1992b; Rabins, Aylward, Holroyd, & Pearlson, Reference Rabins, Aylward, Holroyd and Pearlson2000), greater thalamic volumes (Corey-Bloom et al., Reference Corey-Bloom, Jernigan, Archibald, Harris and Jeste1995), and cerebellar atrophy (Barak, Aizenberg, Mirecki, Mazeh, & Achiron, Reference Barak, Aizenberg, Mirecki, Mazeh and Achiron2002).

Consistent with findings in EOS, a marked cognitive impairment is one of the central aspects of late onset psychosis. Patients with LOS or VLOSLP show deficits in processing speed, attention, executive function, language and memory (Van Assche et al., Reference Van Assche, Morrens, Luyten, Van de Ven and Vandenbulcke2017). Several aspects of processing speed are affected, such as cognitive speed, psychomotor and complex visuo-perceptual speed (Henderson et al., Reference Henderson, Korten, Levings, Jorm, Christensen, Jacomb and Rodgers1998; Jeste et al., Reference Jeste, Harris, Krull, Kuck, McAdams and Heaton1995; Naguib & Levy, Reference Naguib and Levy1987; Vahia et al., Reference Vahia, Palmer, Depp, Fellows, Golshan, Kraemer and Jeste2010). Attention, and specifically vigilance, appeared reduced in VLOSLP (Hanssen et al., Reference Hanssen, van der Werf, Verkaaik, Arts, Myin-Germeys, van Os and Kohler2015). In the domain of executive function, working memory is deficient, as well as fluency, cognitive flexibility, shifting, planning, abstraction and logical reasoning (Almeida et al., Reference Almeida, Howard, Levy, David, Morris and Sahakian1995a, Reference Almeida, Howard, Levy, David, Morris and Sahakian1995b; Girard et al., Reference Girard, Simard, Noiseux, Laplante, Dugas, Rousseau and Bernier2011; Östling, Johansson, & Skoog, Reference Östling, Johansson and Skoog2004). Although the evidence for memory impairment in VLOSLP is not entirely consistent, many studies point to deficient encoding as well as consolidation skills (Almeida et al., Reference Almeida, Howard, Levy, David, Morris and Sahakian1995b; Brichant-Petitjean et al., Reference Brichant-Petitjean, Legauffre, Ramoz, Ades, Gorwood and Dubertret2013). There has been limited research into language function in LOS or VLOSLP, which points to a deficit in semantic processing (Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Paulsen, McAdams, Kuck, Zisook, Braff and Jeste1994; Jeste et al., Reference Jeste, Harris, Krull, Kuck, McAdams and Heaton1995). Impairments in the different neuropsychological domains in patients with LOS or VLOSLP are usually only mildly progressive in nature.

In the absence of a definitive understanding of the neurobiological underpinning of VLOSLP and those which lead to cognitive deficits, there are currently no biomarkers available to aid diagnosis and indicate the most suitable treatment. Neuroimaging studies have been conducted to identify differences between LOS/VLOSLP and normal ageing. However, previous studies have not focused solely on patients with VLOSLP, but rather a mixed group of LOS and VLOSLP. Additionally, most studies investigated pre-defined brain regions (Van Assche et al., Reference Van Assche, Morrens, Luyten, Van de Ven and Vandenbulcke2017). To address the knowledge gap, we conducted a data-driven, voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis, which offers the advantage of an unbiased evaluation of the whole-brain in patients with VLOSLP to identify grey matter (GM) abnormalities for the first time. In addition to this mass univariate approach, a data-driven, multivariate extension of VBM, i.e. source-based morphometry (SBM), may provide complementary information on the neurobiology of psychiatric disorders as neuronal network disorders. This technique can identify spatially distinct regions which show similar patterns of GM abnormalities, and which may thus reflect alterations in a common structural network. Additionally, we aimed to detect neuropsychological deficits and link these to the volumetric differences in an attempt to clarify the neurobiological mechanisms of VLOSLP and its debilitating cognitive symptoms.

Methods

Participants

A group of 36 individuals with VLOSLP who were consecutively admitted to the old age psychiatry ward participated in the current study. Individuals with VLOSLP fulfilled the consensus criteria proposed by the International Late-Onset Schizophrenia Group with first onset of psychosis after the age of 60 and no evidence of neurologic or major affective disorder (Howard et al., Reference Howard, Rabins, Seeman and Jeste2000). Other somatic or ophthalmologic conditions that might explain the onset of psychosis in late life had also been excluded. Thirty-six healthy older adults were also recruited using flyers. Additional exclusion criteria for both groups were (comorbid) major psychiatric illness, and previous or current alcohol or drug dependence. The current study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Hospitals of Leuven and all participants signed an informed consent.

MRI acquisition and image processing

High-resolution T1-weighted images were acquired on a 3T Philips Achieva scanner with an 8-channel head coil. High-resolution 3D turbo field echo (3DTFE) T1-weighted images were acquired with parameters: TR = 9.6 ms, TE = 4.6 ms, flip angle = 8°, voxel-size = 0.98 × 0.98 × 1.2 mm3, 182 axial slices.

All T1-images were processed using the default pipeline of the Computational Anatomy Toolbox (CAT12.6, http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/), a toolbox for Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12, version 7771, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Prior to preprocessing, all data were visually checked and manually aligned to the origin of images with the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line. Preprocessing included bias-correction, segmentation into GM, WM and CSF, spatial normalisation using the Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponentiated Lie Algebra (DARTEL) algorithm, and modulation. Images were smoothed with an 8-mm full-width at half maximum Gaussian kernel (FWHM). Total intracranial volume (TIV) was calculated using CAT12.

Multivariate SBM analysis

SBM is a data-driven, multivariate extension of VBM utilising independent component analysis (ICA) to identify patterns across multiple covarying networks (Xu, Groth, Pearlson, Schretlen, & Calhoun, Reference Xu, Groth, Pearlson, Schretlen and Calhoun2009). Using individual pre-processed GM image, we performed an ICA. An Infomax algorithm implemented in the SBM module of the GIFT toolbox (http://mialab.mrn.org/software/gift) was used to perform ICA decompositions. We set the number of components to 30 in accordance with similar studies (Gupta et al., Reference Gupta, Calhoun, Rachakonda, Chen, Patel, Liu and Turner2015; Xu et al., Reference Xu, Groth, Pearlson, Schretlen and Calhoun2009), and we used the ICASSO algorithm (Himberg, Hyvärinen, & Esposito, et al., Reference Himberg, Hyvärinen and Esposito2004) to increase component reliability and consistency. Components with a quality index >0.9 indicating stable decomposition were used in subsequent analyses. Group comparisons were conducted by using ICA loading parameters. A multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was used with loading parameters as dependent variables, diagnosis as a factor, and age as a covariate. We set p < 0.05 as a statistically significant threshold. The following separate ANCOVAs including age as a covariate were conducted to identify which components differed between groups. Detailed description of methodology for SBM can be found in the supplementary materials and a previous paper (Xu et al., Reference Xu, Groth, Pearlson, Schretlen and Calhoun2009).

Neuropsychological assessments

The following instruments were administered by trained neuropsychologists in a standardised way according to published test manuals. A standardised version of the Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE) was used as a tool for the assessment of global cognitive abilities (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975).

The Stroop test (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935) and Digit Span forward and backward tests (Jones & Macken, Reference Jones and Macken2015; Miller, Reference Miller1956) were used to assess processing speed, attention and executive function. Stroop I was used to assess processing speed and Stroop Interference Factor (IF) was used to estimate selective attention, mental flexibility and inhibitory control (Van der Elst, Van Boxtel, Van Breukelen, & Jolles, Reference Van der Elst, Van Boxtel, Van Breukelen and Jolles2006). The Digit Span forward and backward are the most frequently used instruments to measure attention span, verbal storage and rehearsal systems. The maximum number of digits repeated in the same and in reverse order was used as estimates of attention/memory span and working memory respectively.

The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (Rey, Reference Rey1964) and animal verbal fluency (AVF) (Benton, Reference Benton1968) tasks were used to assess episodic and semantic memory respectively.

The Boston Naming Test was used to assess confrontation naming skills (Goodglass, Kaplan, & Weintraub, Reference Goodglass, Kaplan and Weintraub1983; Rabin, Paolillo, & Barr, Reference Rabin, Paolillo and Barr2016) because it is a well-established reliability and validity among different healthy and clinical populations (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, Reference Strauss, Sherman and Spreen2006). The number of items accurately named without offering semantic or phonetic cues constitutes the total score.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to analyse the clinical data. Distributions of all variables were inspected using histograms, q–q plots, and Shapiro–Wilk tests. Whole-brain analyses were conducted using SPM12, and other analyses were conducted by using SPSS v. 25.

To investigate any differences in grey matter volume (GMV) between patients with VLOSLP and healthy controls, whole-brain voxel-wise comparisons were performed using SPM12, with age and TIV as covariates. The statistical threshold for the voxel-wise whole-brain analyses was set at family-wise error corrected p < 0.05 determined by threshold-free cluster enhancement (Smith & Nichols, Reference Smith and Nichols2009). An absolute threshold masking of 0.1 and a mask defined by AAL3 as an explicit mask were applied. Mango (http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/mango.html) and R version 3.4.3 were used to visualise the results.

To investigate significant differences between groups of participants on neuropsychological measures, we applied a MANCOVA (Pillai's trace because of the relatively small sample sizes) model with groups (with and without psychosis) as independent variable, test scores as dependent variables, and age as a covariate. Partial correlations were used to explore associations between regional GMV differences based on the results in the whole-brain analysis and neuropsychological measures. Age and TIV were included as covariates. For these analyses, we also set p < 0.05 as the statistically significant threshold following Bonferroni correction.

Results

Population and clinical characteristics

There were no significant group differences in age (p = 0.47) and gender (p = 1.0) (Table 1). Individuals with VLOSLP showed statistically significantly lower MMSE scores compared to healthy controls (t(51) = 7.77, p < 0.001). Psychotic symptoms consisted mainly of paranoid delusions (89%), often combined with (multimodal) hallucinations. When comparing the neuropsychological functioning of both participant groups with age as a covariate, there was a significant effect of group on neuropsychological results (V = 0.53, F (9, 60) = 7.40, p < 0.001). Separate t tests for each variable revealed significant mean differences in all neuropsychological variables except the Digit Span forward and RAVLT recognition after applying Bonferroni correction. There were medium to large effect sizes for all significant differences (Table 2).

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of both participant groups

Note: HC, healthy controls; VLOSLP, Very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis; M, mean; s.d., standard deviation; MMSE, Mini Mental State Examination.

Table 2. Neuropsychological results of both participant groups

Note: HC, healthy controls; VLOSLP, Very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis; M, mean; s.d., standard deviation; Stroop IF, Stroop Interference Factor; DS, Digit Span; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; AVF, animal verbal fluency; BNT, Boston Naming Test. After applying Bonferroni correction differences between the two groups were considered significant at a p < 0.005 level; * = p < 0.005. ES = Effect size: Cohen's d was used to measure effect sizes of the group differences (Zakzanis, Kielar, Young, & Boulos, Reference Zakzanis, Kielar, Young and Boulos2001); CL effect size, common language effect size, the % chance that in randomly selected pairs of individuals the participant from one group would score higher than the participant from the second group.

Whole-brain GMV comparison

Data from one participant with VLOSLP was excluded from further analyses as visual inspection of the scan revealed an infarction. The whole-brain voxel-wise analysis identified significant lower GMV in the thalamus and left frontal regions, including the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and insula (Fig. 1). No brain regions were larger in patients with VLOSLP compared with healthy older adults. The additional VBM analysis with a more liberal but with a standard statistical threshold found a significant volume reduction in the right hippocampus, right inferior temporal gyrus, and right cerebellum in addition to the thalamus and left insula (online Supplementary Table S1 and Fig. S1).

Figure 1. Lower grey matter volume in VLOSLP. (a) VBM showed significant grey matter volume reductions in the thalamus (b), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and insula (c) in individuals with VLOSLP compared with healthy controls. Significance threshold was set at family wise error corrected p < 0.05 determined by threshold-free cluster enhancement. ‘−logp = 1.3’ is equivalent to p = 0.05, and ‘−logp = 3’ is equivalent to p = 0.001.

In order to gain further insights into the GMV differences within the thalamus, we overlayed our results on the AAL3 brain atlas. We found that the lower GMV region was mainly located in the anteroventral/ventral anterior (AV/VA) nucleus, ventral lateral (VL) nucleus, and mediodorsal nucleus (MD) nucleus. To investigate which cortical regions could be affected by the lower thalamic GM region, we overlayed our results on the Oxford Thalamic Connectivity Atlas (Behrens et al., Reference Behrens, Johansen-Berg, Woolrich, Smith, Wheeler-Kingshott, Boulby and Matthews2003). We found that the lower thalamic GMV region detected in the whole-brain analysis mainly connects the prefrontal and temporal regions (Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Detailed exploration of grey matter volume reductions in thalamic nuclei in individuals with VLOSLP. The identified thalamic regions in the whole-brain analysis were located in the AV/VA nucleus, VL and MD nucleus according to the AAL3 brain atlas (a, b). These brain regions have structural connectivity with prefrontal and temporal regions according to the Oxford Thalamic Connectivity Atlas (c, d). AV, anteroventral (nucleus); IL, intralaminar (nucleus); MDN, mediodorsal nucleus; Pul, pulvinar nucleus; VA, ventral anterior (nucleus); VL, ventral lateral (nucleus); VPL, ventral posterolateral.

SBM analysis

SBM analysis identified eight stable components in our cohort (online Supplementary Fig. S2). There was a significant main effect of diagnosis on ICA loading parameters (F 8, 61 = 2.87, p = 0.009). Separate univariate ANCOVAs revealed that there was a main effect of diagnosis in the component 2 (thalamic and hippocampal component) (F 1, 68 = 15.2, p < 0.001) even after multiple comparisons correction.

Associations between regional GM differences and neuropsychological test scores

Partial correlations between neuropsychological performance and brain regions that showed lower GMV following the whole-brain VBM demonstrated significant relationships across groups between thalamus and Stroop IF, RAVLT sum, RAVLT delayed recall, RAVLT recognition and AVF after applying Bonferroni corrections. There were also three significant associations across groups between the IFG and insula and the Stroop IF and RAVLT sum and delayed recall. Within group significant associations existed between thalamus and RAVLT delayed recall in individuals with VLOSLP and no significant associations remained in the group of healthy controls (Table 3).

Table 3. Partial correlations (r values) with age and total intracranial volume as covariates between brain volumes and neuropsychological measures across and within groups

Notes: RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; AVF, animal verbal fluency; DS, Digit Span; BNT, Boston Naming Test; VOSP, Visual Object and Space Perception battery. * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001 before Bonferroni correction, bold print = significant after Bonferroni correction.

Discussion

Our comprehensive whole-brain VBM and SBM analysis demonstrated lower volumes in the thalamus and fronto-temporal regions, including left IFG, left insula, and hippocampus in individuals with VLOSLP. Furthermore, we found that lower thalamic GMV was associated with cognitive dysfunction in this group. Of note, we included only individuals with VLOSLP, in contrast to most previous studies, which included a mixed group of LOS and VLOSLP, thus, we provide more reliable evidence on the neurobiology of VLOSLP from a relatively homogenous sample of this diagnostic category.

Further investigation of thalamic volumes suggested specific reductions in the AV/VA nucleus, VL and MD nucleus. Volumetric changes in the thalamus and specifically the MD nucleus microstructure have already been described in first episode psychosis and in clinical high-risk adults (Cho et al., Reference Cho, Kwak, Hwang, Lee, Kim, Lee and Kwon2019). Moreover, thalamic abnormality has been found to subserve psychotic symptoms in any psychotic disorder regardless of age (Huang et al., Reference Huang, Rogers, Sheffield, Jalbrzikowski, Anticevic, Blackford and Woodward2020). In addition, we found lower GMV in the thalamic cluster included the areas that show structural connectivity with prefrontal and temporal regions. Prior research identified altered thalamo-cortical anatomical connectivity as a transdiagnostic feature of psychosis, already noticeable in the early stages of disease and it has also been associated with cognitive impairment (Sheffield et al., Reference Sheffield, Huang, Rogers, Giraldo-Chica, Landman, Blackford and Woodward2020).

We demonstrated impairments in patients with VLOSLP compared to healthy controls on measures of processing speed, selective attention/mental flexibility, working memory, semantic fluency, verbal memory and naming. In line with our results, a potentially less impaired recognition but clearly deficient immediate as well as delayed recall/retrieval in a verbal memory task was reported in a meta-analysis on cognition in EOS (Aleman, Hijman, de Haan, & Kahn, Reference Aleman, Hijman, de Haan and Kahn1999; Frangou, Hadjulis, & Vourdas, Reference Frangou, Hadjulis and Vourdas2007). This may point to memory deficits that are (partly) mediated by executive dysfunction, possibly related to reduced volumes in thalamic regions with prefrontal and temporal connections (Doughty & Done, Reference Doughty and Done2009). Such a combination of executive and memory dysfunction is also reminiscent of the two-factor model of delusions (Coltheart, Reference Coltheart2010), which states that the manifestation of a delusion requires the presence of memory impairment to prompt a delusional belief and coinciding executive deficits that interfere with processes of belief evaluation. Similarly, the onset of hallucinations has been linked to an interaction between problematic suppression of personal memories and impaired reality monitoring (Jellinger, Reference Jellinger2012).

Reduced volumes in the left IFG and insula were associated specifically with impairments in mental flexibility/response inhibition as well as verbal memory across groups. Prior studies have demonstrated that a fronto-temporal network supports episodic memory (Baker, Sanders, Maccotta, & Buckner, Reference Baker, Sanders, Maccotta and Buckner2001) and have also shown that the ventrolateral cortico-limbic pathway, including the IFG and insular cortex, play an important role in adapting behaviour in environmental conditions that are not always predictable, which is especially difficult in individuals with psychotic symptoms (Tops & Boksem, Reference Tops and Boksem2011). Research that looked specifically at the role of the left IFG and insula in inhibitory control, found that they were crucial even though neuroimaging studies thus far have focused more on the right IFG as a neurobiological correlate of inhibition and the left IFG has been implicated mainly in language function (Swick, Ashley, & Turken, Reference Swick, Ashley and Turken2008).

The large number of associations between the ‘relay’ structure in the brain, the thalamus, and neuropsychological results in the current study illustrates its pivotal role in memory, executive functioning as well as attention in general (Georgescu, Popa, & Zagrean, Reference Georgescu, Popa and Zagrean2020; Van der Werf et al., Reference Van der Werf, Scheltens, Lindeboom, Witter, Uylings and Jolles2003; Van der Werf, Witter, Uylings, & Jolles, Reference Van der Werf, Witter, Uylings and Jolles2000). Moreover, thalamic abnormalities have been associated with language, motor and executive functioning in individuals with EOS specifically (Andrews, Wang, Csernansky, Gado, & Barch, Reference Andrews, Wang, Csernansky, Gado and Barch2006; Coscia et al., Reference Coscia, Narr, Robinson, Hamilton, Sevy, Burdick and Szeszko2009; Crespo-Facorro et al., Reference Crespo-Facorro, Roiz-Santiáñez, Pelayo-Terán, Rodríguez-Sánchez, Pérez-Iglesias, González-Blanch and Vázquez-Barquero2007).

However, in the current study many of the relationships between brain volumes and neuropsychological measures were no longer significant within groups. The only significant association that remained within the group of individuals with VLOSLP was that between the thalamus and delayed recall in a verbal memory task. Verbal memory is certainly one of the more severely affected domains in schizophrenia (Frangou, Reference Frangou2010; Guimond, Chakravarty, Bergeron-Gagnon, Patel, & Lepage, Reference Guimond, Chakravarty, Bergeron-Gagnon, Patel and Lepage2015). Functional alterations to the IFG and thalamus – as detected using a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy during a verbal learning task – have been shown to affect verbal memory in individuals with schizophrenia, suggestive again of the importance of structural (dis)connections in cognitive impairments (Hagino et al., Reference Hagino, Suzuki, Mori, Nohara, Yamashita, Takahashi and Kurachi2002).

A limitation of the current study is small sample size. Collecting data from larger samples is challenging in VLOSLP as the condition is rare and individuals with paranoid symptoms are often hesitant to participate in research studies. Although larger than many previous studies, the sample size may affect statistical power, possibly leading to type II errors. Indeed, our statistical threshold in the whole brain analysis and the MANCOVA comparing neuropsychological results is a standard one, which may be very strict for our sample size and lead to an underestimation of volumetric brain differences or neuropsychological deficits in VLOSLP v. healthy older adults. To address this issue, we therefore report the results of another VBM analysis with a liberal statistical threshold and SBM analysis, which is sensitive to group differences in small sample sizes, as supplementary analyses. Also, the limited number of significant associations between lower GMV and neuropsychological scores within groups may point to a lack of statistical power. Nevertheless, existing knowledge on the neurobiology and neuropsychology of VLOSLP is very scarce, and typically involves small groups of individuals. Moreover, previous research almost always pooled data from both LOS and VLOSLP, whereas the neurobiological mechanisms may be different in both conditions. Therefore, our findings in a larger, more clinically homogeneous sample than previous studies are relevant to the field.

Conclusion

In the current study, we found lower GMV in the left IFG and insula as well as the thalamus in individuals with VLOSLP compared with healthy older adults. The IFG, insula and thalamic areas were associated with deficits in verbal memory and executive function. Moreover, lower GMV in the thalamic cluster included the areas that show structural connectivity with prefrontal and temporal regions. Future research investigating the integrity of such structural connections could help further elucidate the neurobiological underpinnings of VLOSLP, which may identify targets for (multimodal) treatments, involving pharmacological as well as non-pharmacological revalidation approaches, promoting self-sufficiency and quality of life in individuals with VLOSLP.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723002258.

Acknowledgements

No acknowledgements.

Financial support

This research was supported by the KU Leuven Sequoia Fund for Research on Ageing and Mental Health., KU Leuven grant C24/18/095, and Research Foundation Flanders (FWO) grant: G0C0319N. A.T. was supported financially by the fellowship of Astellas Foundation for Research on Metabolic Disorders.

Competing interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Footnotes

*

Shared first authorship

References

Aleman, A., Hijman, R., de Haan, E. H., & Kahn, R. S. (1999). Memory impairment in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(9), 13581366. doi:10.1176/ajp.156.9.1358.Google Scholar
Almeida, O. P., Howard, R., Levy, R., David, A. S., Morris, R. G., & Sahakian, B. J. (1995a). Clinical and cognitive diversity of psychotic states arising in late life (late paraphrenia). Psychological Medicine, 25(4), 699714.Google Scholar
Almeida, O. P., Howard, R., Levy, R., David, A. S., Morris, R. G., & Sahakian, B. J. (1995b). Cognitive features of psychotic states arising in late life (late paraphrenia). Psychological Medicine, 25(4), 685698.Google Scholar
Andrews, J., Wang, L., Csernansky, J. G., Gado, M. H., & Barch, D. M. (2006). Abnormalities of thalamic activation and cognition in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(3), 463469. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.463.Google Scholar
Baker, J. T., Sanders, A. L., Maccotta, L., & Buckner, R. L. (2001). Neural correlates of verbal memory encoding during semantic and structural processing tasks. Neuroreport, 12(6), 12511256. doi:10.1097/00001756-200105080-00039.Google Scholar
Barak, Y., Aizenberg, D., Mirecki, I., Mazeh, D., & Achiron, A. (2002). Very late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis: Clinical and imaging characteristics in comparison with elderly patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190(11), 733736. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000038167.15155.d6.Google Scholar
Barta, P. E., Powers, R. E., Aylward, E. H., Chase, G. A., Harris, G. J., Rabins, P. V., … Pearlson, G. D. (1997). Quantitative MRI volume changes in late onset schizophrenia and Alzheimer's disease compared to normal controls. Psychiatry Research, 68(2–3), 6575.Google Scholar
Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., Woolrich, M. W., Smith, S. M., Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., Boulby, P. A., … Matthews, P. M. (2003). Non-invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 750757. doi:10.1038/nn1075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A. L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6(1), 5360.Google Scholar
Brichant-Petitjean, C., Legauffre, C., Ramoz, N., Ades, J., Gorwood, P., & Dubertret, C. (2013). Memory deficits in late-onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 151(1–3), 8590. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casanova, M. F. (2010). The pathology of paraphrenia. Current Psychiatry Reports, 12(3), 196201. doi:10.1007/s11920-010-0108-8.Google Scholar
Cho, K. I. K., Kwak, Y. B., Hwang, W. J., Lee, J., Kim, M., Lee, T. Y., & Kwon, J. S. (2019). Microstructural changes in higher-order nuclei of the thalamus in patients with first-episode psychosis. Biological Psychiatry, 85(1), 7078. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.05.019.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M. (2010). The neuropsychology of delusions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 1626. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05496.x.Google Scholar
Copeland, J. R., Dewey, M. E., Scott, A., Gilmore, C., Larkin, B. A., Cleave, N., … McKibbin, P. E. (1998). Schizophrenia and delusional disorder in older age: Community prevalence, incidence, comorbidity, and outcome. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24(1), 153161. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033307.Google Scholar
Corey-Bloom, J., Jernigan, T., Archibald, S., Harris, M. J., & Jeste, D. V. (1995). Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in late-life schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(3), 447449. doi:10.1176/ajp.152.3.447.Google Scholar
Coscia, D., Narr, K., Robinson, D., Hamilton, L., Sevy, S., Burdick, K., … Szeszko, P. (2009). Volumetric and shape analysis of the thalamus in first-episode schizophrenia. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 12361245. doi:10.1002/hbm.20595.Google Scholar
Crespo-Facorro, B., Roiz-Santiáñez, R., Pelayo-Terán, J. M., Rodríguez-Sánchez, J. M., Pérez-Iglesias, R., González-Blanch, C., … Vázquez-Barquero, J. L. (2007). Reduced thalamic volume in first-episode non-affective psychosis: Correlations with clinical variables, symptomatology and cognitive functioning. NeuroImage, 35(4), 16131623. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.048.Google Scholar
Doughty, O. J., & Done, D. J. (2009). Is semantic memory impaired in schizophrenia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 91 studies. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14(6), 473509. doi:10.1080/13546800903073291.Google Scholar
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). ‘Mini-mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.Google Scholar
Frangou, S. (2010). Cognitive function in early onset schizophrenia: A selective review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3(79). doi:10.3389/neuro.09.079.2009.Google Scholar
Frangou, S., Hadjulis, M., & Vourdas, A. (2007). The Maudsley early onset schizophrenia study: Cognitive function over a 4-year follow-up period. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(1), 5259. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm124.Google Scholar
Georgescu, I. A., Popa, D., & Zagrean, L. (2020). The anatomical and functional heterogeneity of the mediodorsal thalamus. Brain Sciences, 10(9). doi:10.3390/brainsci10090624.Google Scholar
Girard, C., Simard, M., Noiseux, R., Laplante, L., Dugas, M., Rousseau, F., … Bernier, P. J. (2011). Late-onset-psychosis: Cognition. International Psychogeriatrics, 23(8), 13011316. doi:10.1017/s1041610211000238.Google Scholar
Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston naming test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.Google Scholar
Guimond, S., Chakravarty, M. M., Bergeron-Gagnon, L., Patel, R., & Lepage, M. (2015). Verbal memory impairments in schizophrenia associated with cortical thinning. NeuroImage, 11, 2029. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.12.010.Google Scholar
Gupta, C. N., Calhoun, V. D., Rachakonda, S., Chen, J., Patel, V., Liu, J., … Turner, J. A. (2015). Patterns of gray matter abnormalities in schizophrenia based on an international mega-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41, 11331142.Google Scholar
Hagino, H., Suzuki, M., Mori, K., Nohara, S., Yamashita, I., Takahashi, T., … Kurachi, M. (2002). Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of the inferior frontal gyrus and thalamus and its relationship to verbal learning task performance in patients with schizophrenia: A preliminary report. [10.1046/j.1440-1819.2002.01045.x]. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 56(5), 499507. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1819.2002.01045.x.Google Scholar
Hanssen, M., van der Werf, M., Verkaaik, M., Arts, B., Myin-Germeys, I., van Os, J., … Kohler, S. (2015). Comparative study of clinical and neuropsychological characteristics between early-, late and very-late-onset schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23(8), 852862. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2014.10.007.Google Scholar
Heaton, R., Paulsen, J. S., McAdams, L. A., Kuck, J., Zisook, S., Braff, D., … Jeste, D. V. (1994). Neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenics. Relationship to age, chronicity, and dementia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51(6), 469476. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950060033003.Google Scholar
Henderson, A. S., Korten, A. E., Levings, C., Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Jacomb, P. A., & Rodgers, B. (1998). Psychotic symptoms in the elderly: A prospective study in a population sample. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13(7), 484492.Google Scholar
Himberg, J., Hyvärinen, A., & Esposito, F. (2004). Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. NeuroImage, 22, 12141222.Google Scholar
Howard, R., Förstl, H., Almeida, O., Burns, A., & Levy, R. (1992a). Computer-assisted CT measurements in late paraphrenics with and without Schneiderian first-rank symptoms: A preliminary report. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 7(1), 3538. doi:10.1002/gps.930070106.Google Scholar
Howard, R., Förstl, H., Naguib, M., Burns, A., & Levy, R. (1992b). First-rank symptoms of Schneider in late paraphrenia. Cortical structural correlates. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 108109.Google Scholar
Howard, R., Rabins, P. V., Seeman, M. V., Jeste, D. V., & The International Late-Onset Schizophrenia Group. (2000). Late-onset schizophrenia and very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis: An international consensus. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(2), 172178. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.172.Google Scholar
Huang, A. S., Rogers, B. P., Sheffield, J. M., Jalbrzikowski, M. E., Anticevic, A., Blackford, J. U., … Woodward, N. D. (2020). Thalamic nuclei volumes in psychotic disorders and in youths with psychosis spectrum symptoms. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(12), 11591167. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.19101099.Google Scholar
Jellinger, K. A. (2012). Cerebral correlates of psychotic syndromes in neurodegenerative diseases. Journal of Cellular and Molecular Medicine, 16(5), 9951012. doi:10.1111/j.1582-4934.2011.01311.x.Google Scholar
Jeste, D. V., Harris, M. J., Krull, A., Kuck, J., McAdams, L. A., & Heaton, R. (1995). Clinical and neuropsychological characteristics of patients with late-onset schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(5), 722730. doi:10.1176/ajp.152.5.722.Google Scholar
Jones, G., & Macken, B. (2015). Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: Why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition, 144, 113. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.009.Google Scholar
Lesser, I. M., Miller, B. L., Swartz, J. R., Boone, K. B., Mehringer, C. M., & Mena, I. (1993). Brain imaging in late-life schizophrenia and related psychoses. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 19(4), 773782.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 8197. doi:10.1037/h0043158.Google Scholar
Naguib, M., & Levy, R. (1987). Late paraphrenia: Neuropsychological impairment and structural brain abnormalities on computed tomography. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 2(2), 8390. doi:10.1002/gps.930020205.Google Scholar
Östling, S., Johansson, B., & Skoog, I. (2004). Cognitive test performance in relation to psychotic symptoms and paranoid ideation in non-demented 85-year-olds. Psychological Medicine, 34(3), 443450. doi:10.1017/S0033291703001144.Google Scholar
Rabin, L. A., Paolillo, E., & Barr, W. B. (2016). Stability in test-usage practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada over a 10-year period: A follow-up survey of INS and NAN members. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31(3), 206230. doi:10.1093/arclin/acw007.Google Scholar
Rabins, P., Aylward, E., Holroyd, S., & Pearlson, G. (2000). MRI findings differentiate between late-onset schizophrenia and late-life mood disorder. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(10), 954960.Google Scholar
Rabins, P., Pearlson, G., Jayaram, G., Steele, C., & Tune, L. (1987). Increased ventricle-to-brain ratio in late-onset schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(9), 12161218. doi:10.1176/ajp.144.9.1216.Google Scholar
Rey, A. (1964). L'examen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Sachdev, P., Brodaty, H., Cheang, D., & Cathcart, S. (2000). Hippocampus and amygdala volumes in elderly schizophrenic patients as assessed by magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 54(1), 105112. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1819.2000.00644.x.Google Scholar
Sharma, E. R., Debsikdar, A. V., Naphade, N. M., & Shetty, J. V. (2014). Very late-onset schizophrenia like psychosis: Case series and future directions. Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine, 36(2), 208210. doi:10.4103/0253-7176.130999.Google Scholar
Sheffield, J. M., Huang, A. S., Rogers, B. P., Giraldo-Chica, M., Landman, B. A., Blackford, J. U., … Woodward, N. D. (2020). Thalamocortical anatomical connectivity in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(5), 10621071. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbaa022.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M., & Nichols, T. E. (2009). Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. NeuroImage, 44(1), 8398. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.061.Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643662.Google Scholar
Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, A. U. (2008). Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neuroscience, 9, 102. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-9-102.Google Scholar
Tops, M., & Boksem, M. A. S. (2011). A potential role of the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula in cognitive control, brain rhythms, and event-related potentials. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 330. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00330.Google Scholar
Vahia, I. V., Palmer, B. W., Depp, C., Fellows, I., Golshan, S., Kraemer, H. C., & Jeste, D. V. (2010). Is late-onset schizophrenia a subtype of schizophrenia? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122(5), 414426. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01552.x.Google Scholar
Van Assche, L., Morrens, M., Luyten, P., Van de Ven, L., & Vandenbulcke, M. (2017). The neuropsychology and neurobiology of late-onset schizophrenia and very-late-onset schizophrenia-like psychosis: A critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 604621. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.024.Google Scholar
Van der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P., Van Breukelen, G. J., & Jolles, J. (2006). The Stroop color-word test: Influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment, 13(1), 6279. doi:10.1177/1073191105283427.Google Scholar
Van der Werf, Y. D., Scheltens, P., Lindeboom, J., Witter, M. P., Uylings, H. B., & Jolles, J. (2003). Deficits of memory, executive functioning and attention following infarction in the thalamus: A study of 22 cases with localised lesions. Neuropsychologia, 41(10), 13301344. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00059-9.Google Scholar
Van der Werf, Y. D., Witter, M. P., Uylings, H. B., & Jolles, J. (2000). Neuropsychology of infarctions in the thalamus: A review. Neuropsychologia, 38(5), 613627. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00104-9.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Howard, R., Takei, N., & Murray, R. (1995). Increasing age is a risk factor for psychosis in the elderly. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 30(4), 161164.Google Scholar
Xu, L., Groth, K. M., Pearlson, G., Schretlen, D. J., & Calhoun, V. D. (2009). Source-based morphometry: The use of independent component analysis to identify gray matter differences with application to schizophrenia. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 711724.Google Scholar
Zakzanis, K. K., Kielar, A., Young, D. A., & Boulos, M. (2001). Neuropsychological differentiation of late onset schizophrenia and frontotemporal dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 6(1), 6377. doi:10.1080/13546800042000052.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of both participant groups

Figure 1

Table 2. Neuropsychological results of both participant groups

Figure 2

Figure 1. Lower grey matter volume in VLOSLP. (a) VBM showed significant grey matter volume reductions in the thalamus (b), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and insula (c) in individuals with VLOSLP compared with healthy controls. Significance threshold was set at family wise error corrected p < 0.05 determined by threshold-free cluster enhancement. ‘−logp = 1.3’ is equivalent to p = 0.05, and ‘−logp = 3’ is equivalent to p = 0.001.

Figure 3

Figure 2. Detailed exploration of grey matter volume reductions in thalamic nuclei in individuals with VLOSLP. The identified thalamic regions in the whole-brain analysis were located in the AV/VA nucleus, VL and MD nucleus according to the AAL3 brain atlas (a, b). These brain regions have structural connectivity with prefrontal and temporal regions according to the Oxford Thalamic Connectivity Atlas (c, d). AV, anteroventral (nucleus); IL, intralaminar (nucleus); MDN, mediodorsal nucleus; Pul, pulvinar nucleus; VA, ventral anterior (nucleus); VL, ventral lateral (nucleus); VPL, ventral posterolateral.

Figure 4

Table 3. Partial correlations (r values) with age and total intracranial volume as covariates between brain volumes and neuropsychological measures across and within groups

Supplementary material: File

S0033291723002258sup001.docx

Van Assche et al. supplementary material

Download S0033291723002258sup001.docx(File)
File 638.3 KB