Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T16:10:14.392Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social processes as the missing link: cross-sectionally testing a conceptual model on social mediators of early psychopathological development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 October 2024

Robin Achterhof*
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium KU Leuven Child & Youth Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Erasmus School of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Department of Psychology, Education & Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Mandeville Building Room T15-10, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Olivia J. Kirtley
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium KU Leuven Child & Youth Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Ginette Lafit
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium KU Leuven Child & Youth Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Research Group on Quantitative Psychology and Individual Differences, Faculty of Psychology, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Anu P. Hiekkaranta
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Noëmi Hagemann
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Flemish Scientific Society for Youth Health Care (VWVJ), Leuven, Belgium
Karlijn S. F. M. Hermans
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Strategy and Academic Affairs, Administration and Central Services, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands
Aleksandra Lecei
Affiliation:
Center for Clinical Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Bart Boets
Affiliation:
Center for Developmental Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Cécile Henquet
Affiliation:
Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, the Netherlands
Maude Schneider
Affiliation:
Clinical Psychology Unit for Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Rob Sips
Affiliation:
Erasmus School of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Department of Psychology, Education & Child Studies, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Mandeville Building Room T15-10, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Thomas Vaessen
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium Center for eHealth and Well-being Research, Department of Psychology, Health, and Technology, University of Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands
Ruud van Winkel
Affiliation:
Center for Clinical Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Wolfgang Viechtbauer
Affiliation:
Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, the Netherlands
Ulrich Reininghaus
Affiliation:
Center for eHealth and Well-being Research, Department of Psychology, Health, and Technology, University of Twente, Enschede, The Netherlands Department of Public Mental Health, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany ESRC Centre for Society and Mental Health and Centre for Epidemiology and Public Health, Health Service and Population Research Department, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Inez Myin-Germeys
Affiliation:
Center for Contextual Psychiatry, Research Group Psychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium KU Leuven Child & Youth Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
*
Corresponding author: Robin Achterhof; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Research suggests that most mental health conditions have their onset in the critically social period of adolescence. Yet, we lack understanding of the potential social processes underlying early psychopathological development. We propose a conceptual model where daily-life social interactions and social skills form an intermediate link between known risk and protective factors (adverse childhood experiences, bullying, social support, maladaptive parenting) and psychopathology in adolescents – that is explored using cross-sectional data.

Methods

N = 1913 Flemish adolescent participants (Mean age = 13.8; 63% girls) were assessed as part of the SIGMA study, a large-scale, accelerated longitudinal study of adolescent mental health and development. Self-report questionnaires (on risk/protective factors, social skills, and psychopathology) were completed during class time; daily-life social interactions were measured during a subsequent six-day experience-sampling period.

Results

Registered uncorrected multilevel linear regression results revealed significant associations between all risk/protective factors and psychopathology, between all risk/protective factors and social processes, and between all social processes and psychopathology. Social processes (social skills, quantity/quality of daily social interactions) were uniquely predicted by each risk/protective factor and were uniquely associated with both general and specific types of psychopathology. For older participants, some relationships between social processes and psychopathology were stronger.

Conclusions

Unique associations between risk/protective factors and psychopathology signify the distinct relevance of these factors for youth mental health, whereas the broad associations with social processes support these processes as broad correlates. Results align with the idea of a social pathway toward early psychopathology, although follow-up longitudinal research is required to verify any mediation effect.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

The foundations of adult mental health are laid early on, as the majority of all mental health conditions have their onset during adolescence (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005; Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui and Swendsen2010; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo, Salazar de Pablo and Fusar-Poli2021). Estimates indicate that 22% of adolescents present with severe and distressing mental health disorders, and that this prevalence is higher among girls than boys (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui and Swendsen2010). Mental health problems in adolescence are associated with numerous negative clinical, functional, and sociodemographic outcomes (Asselmann, Wittchen, Lieb, & Beesdo-Baum, Reference Asselmann, Wittchen, Lieb and Beesdo-Baum2018), including future suicide attempts (Miché et al., Reference Miché, Hofer, Voss, Meyer, Gloster, Beesdo-Baum and Lieb2018). Furthermore, the younger a person is when they first develop a mental health disorder, the more years they have to live with this, and the higher the chances of developing comorbid disorders later on (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Ambler, Danese, Elliott, Hariri and Moffitt2020). Adolescence thus presents a unique window of opportunity for prevention and early intervention programs (Colizzi, Lasalvia, & Ruggeri, Reference Colizzi, Lasalvia and Ruggeri2020).

The development of mental health problems has been associated with a wide array of transdiagnostic risk and protective factors that, as such, predict a range of different psychopathologies (e.g. depression, anxiety). Many identified transdiagnostic factors have a social component, including low social support (Dalgard, Bjork, & Tambs, Reference Dalgard, Bjork and Tambs1995), exposure to bullying (Brendgen, Reference Brendgen2018), family problems (Davies & Sturge-Apple, Reference Davies, Sturge-Apple, Lewis and Rudolph2014), exposure to adverse childhood experiences or trauma (McKay et al., Reference McKay, Cannon, Chambers, Conroy, Coughlan, Dodd and Clarke2021), and growing up in an urban environment (Polanczyk et al., Reference Polanczyk, Moffitt, Arseneault, Cannon, Ambler, Keefe and Caspi2010). In adolescence in particular, social risk and protective factors seem to play a large role in predicting a range of mental health outcomes (Klasen et al., Reference Klasen, Otto, Kriston, Patalay, Schlack and Ravens-Sieberer2015; Scardera et al., Reference Scardera, Perret, Ouellet-Morin, Gariépy, Juster, Boivin and Geoffroy2020). Furthermore, what we know relatively little about, is how these risk/protective factors may produce adaptive v. maladaptive development. To aid prevention and early intervention efforts, we need to learn more about the potential underlying processes through which these risk and protective factors are associated with mental health outcomes.

In the current paper, we propose that these underlying processes are also fundamentally social in nature. Risk and protective factors may shape how adolescents interact with others (i.e. they affect social processes), which, in turn, drives the dynamic process of psychopathological development. Following leading theories of developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti, Toth, Luthar, Burack, Cicchetti and Weisz1997), we consider that psychopathology arises in conjunction with altered dynamic interactions between individuals and their (social) context. As such, the simplest depiction of this idea is represented in the mediation model of Fig. 1, where social processes mediate the relationship between risk/protective factors and psychopathology, but also, where psychopathology impacts those same social processes. Although formally testing this mediation requires longitudinal data (Kline, Reference Kline2015), more detailed insight into these cross-sectional associations can provide a solid empirical basis for further investigations.

Figure 1. Implied mediation model underlying the reciprocal associations between social processes and psychopathology, and the effects of risk/protective factors.

The social nature of psychopathological development is already supported, for example, by findings of diminished interpersonal skills across different types of mental health issues (Segrin, Reference Segrin2019). At the same time, much research is hampered by a lack of ecological validity – or the real-world representativeness and generalizability of these social drivers of development. Traditional measures of interpersonal skills may not suffice for comprehensively capturing the social processes that link risk/protective factors to psychopathology (Hermans et al., Reference Hermans, Achterhof, Myin-Germeys, Kasanova, Kirtley, Schneider, Lewandowski and Moustafa2019). If we aim to better understand how different social processes are related to early psychopathological development, we need to take a more direct look at the proximal social processes relating to daily-life social interaction. Previous research drawing on the experience sampling method (ESM; Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, Reference Csikszentmihalyi and Larson1987; Myin-Germeys et al., Reference Myin-Germeys, Kasanova, Vaessen, Vachon, Kirtley, Viechtbauer and Reininghaus2018) has already identified associations between both the quantity and quality of daily social interactions, and different types of adolescent psychopathology (Achterhof et al., Reference Achterhof, Kirtley, Schneider, Lafit, Hagemann, Hermans and Myin-Germeys2021). Yet, it remains unclear to what extent these alterations in different social processes link to key risk and protective factors.

Moreover, as adolescence is a critical period of both changes in one's relationships and in one's mental health (Blakemore & Mills, Reference Blakemore and Mills2014; Orben, Tomova, & Blakemore Reference Orben, Tomova and Blakemore2020), we can also expect age-related differences in the contribution of different social processes to the development of psychopathology. A fundamental task of adolescence is to gain a better understanding of oneself in relation to others (Grusec & Davidov, Reference Grusec and Davidov2010; Smetana, Robinson, & Rote, Reference Smetana, Robinson, Rote, Grusec and Hastings2015), which is accompanied by an increasing social network outside of the family (Wrzus, Hänel, Wagner, & Neyer, Reference Wrzus, Hänel, Wagner and Neyer2013). To do achieve this task, sufficient interpersonal skills and qualitative social interactions may become more important as adolescents age. In addition, as boys and girls tend to differ both in terms of mental health (Campbell, Bann, & Patalay, Reference Campbell, Bann and Patalay2021) and social development (Smetana, Campione-Barr, & Metzger, Reference Smetana, Campione-Barr and Metzger2006), we can expect gender differences in the role of social processes to psychopathological development. In particular, recent evidence suggests that social interaction quality is more important for adaptive development in girls than boys (Barzeva, Richards, Veenstra, Meeus, & Oldehinkel, Reference Barzeva, Richards, Veenstra, Meeus and Oldehinkel2022).

The SIGMA study has been set up specifically to investigate both risk and protective factors as well as naturalistic social processes in a general population adolescent sample in Flanders, Belgium (Kirtley et al., Reference Kirtley, Achterhof, Hagemann, Hermans, Hiekkaranta, Lecei and Myin-Germeys2021). Social processes were assessed both using a self-report questionnaire on social skills, as well as a structured diary technique, the Experience Sampling Method (ESM), providing a unique opportunity to assess in daily life both the quantity of social interactions (how often adolescents were in interaction with others) as well as the quality of these social interactions (how adolescents experienced the social interactions; Achterhof et al., Reference Achterhof, Kirtley, Schneider, Hagemann, Hermans, Hiekkaranta and Myin-Germeys2022a). By including different age cohorts of adolescents from the outset of this longitudinal study (i.e. an accelerated longitudinal design; Galbraith, Bowden, & Mander, Reference Galbraith, Bowden and Mander2017), we are able to test potential developmental effects with data from the first wave. Although the proposed model suggests reciprocal effects between social processes and psychopathology (see Fig. 1), we acknowledge that the data used in this study does not allow for formally testing such effects (Maxwell & Cole, Reference Maxwell and Cole2007). Risk and protective factors were selected based on their previous robust associations with mental health outcomes (e.g. Brendgen, Reference Brendgen2018; Davies & Sturge-Apple, Reference Davies, Sturge-Apple, Lewis and Rudolph2014; McKay et al., Reference McKay, Cannon, Chambers, Conroy, Coughlan, Dodd and Clarke2021). They mostly refer to past events and experiences; however, as these questionnaires have not been completed at a preceding time point, we also acknowledge that we are unable to formally test the directionality of their effects. Instead, our current aim is to implicitly test the plausibility of this model by assessing all individual effects contained within it.

As such, the current study uses the data of the first wave of the SIGMA study to investigate (1) the prevalence of mental health problems in this large general population sample as well as the role of age and gender, (2) the associations between known risk and protective factors (adverse childhood experiences, bullying, parental psychological control, social support) and mental health problems (general, depression, anxiety, psychoticism), (3) the associations between known risk and protective factors and social processes (interpersonal skills, the quantity and quality of daily-life social interactions), (4) the associations between social processes and mental health problems. Both corrected and uncorrected associations will be tested throughout. Finally, it will be investigated whether the associations as outlined in 1–4, change with increasing age. As such, this study serves as a test case of different transdiagnostic social pathways toward the development of early mental health problems.

Methods

Recruitment and sample

The SIGMA study recruited N = 1913 adolescents in 22 schools across Flanders, Belgium (for detailed info, see Kirtley et al., Reference Kirtley, Achterhof, Hagemann, Hermans, Hiekkaranta, Lecei and Myin-Germeys2021), between January 2018 and June 2019. Adolescents were eligible to participate if they were in the 1st, 3rd, or 5th grade of secondary school. Participants and their parents were asked to provide informed assent and consent, respectively, for study participation. The study was approved by the Leuven Medical Ethical Committee (number S 61395).

Procedure

Participants were tested during a 100-min in-class session, in groups of 10–25 students. Participants filled in a battery of questionnaires individually on tablets, using the REDCap application (Harris et al., Reference Harris, Taylor, Thielke, Payne, Gonzalez and Conde2009), with the researchers present. Afterwards, participants received a study phone and were instructed for the ESM part of the study. Every day for the next six days, participants were prompted ten times per day to complete the ESM questionnaires on a research mobile phone with only the ESM application ‘MobileQ’ pre-installed (Meers, Dejonckheere, Kalokerinos, Rummens, & Kuppens, Reference Meers, Dejonckheere, Kalokerinos, Rummens and Kuppens2020). Questionnaire prompts were randomly distributed within each of ten 90-min blocks per day between 7.30 AM and 10.30 PM, with at least 15 min between consecutive prompts. Participants had 90 s to respond to each prompt, and 15 min to complete each ESM questionnaire once opened. As compensation for full study participation, adolescents received a €10 euro voucher for an online store.

Measures

Psychopathology symptoms

Psychopathology was assessed with the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis, Reference Derogatis1993), consisting of 53 items across nine subscales rated on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). The Global Severity Index (GSI) is the average response to all items. Reliability of the GSI was good, as assessed with both Cronbach's alpha (α = 0.96) and McDonald's Omega (Revelle & Condon, Reference Revelle and Condon2019; Ω = 0.97). The subscales Depression (α = 0.88; Ω = 0.90), Anxiety (α = 0.78; Ω = 0.87) and Psychoticism (α = 0.73; Ω = 0.75) consisted of the mean scores on those subscales.

Risk and protective factors

Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) were assessed with the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire, revision 2 (JVQ-R2; Hamby, Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, Reference Hamby, Finkelhor, Ormrod and Turner2004). With 34 yes/no items, four types of traumatic events were assessed: property crime, physical assault, sexual assault, and general maltreatment. Nine items of the ‘conventional crime module’ (e.g. relating to firearms) were considered of limited relevance for this sample and were removed for all first-grade students. All analyses were performed with a total score without this module, but a set of sensitivity analyses was performed with the full JVQ-R2 score as well (see online Supplementary Material S1). ACE was defined as the total number of adverse events reported by the participant (reliability of total score: α = 0.86; Ω = 0.87).

Bullying was assessed with one item inquiring about prevalence of bullying (‘Has another kid or teenager ever physically hurt you? (e.g. burned, bruised, cut, punched, kicked)) with answer options ‘Never’, ‘Rarely’, ‘Sometimes’, ‘Regularly’ and ‘Often’. An additional item inquired about the severity of the bullying (‘How bad was the bullying?), with answer options ‘Not, ‘A little’, ‘Moderately, and ‘Severely’. The product of both items was taken as the overall bullying score.

Parental psychological control was assessed for each parent by taking the mean of the eight items of the Psychological Control Scale – Youth Self-Report (Barber, Reference Barber1996), all ranging from ‘1. Not at all’ to ‘5. Very much’ (e.g. ‘My mother/father is always trying to change how I feel or think about things’, α = 0.84; Ω = 0.87).

Social support was assessed with the Dutch Social Support List (Van Sonderen, Reference Van Sonderen2012), consisting of 12 items worded with ‘Does it ever happen you that people…’ and divided into three subscales: everyday support (e.g. ‘Show you that they are fond of you?’), support in case of problems (e.g. ‘Comfort you?’), and appreciation (e.g. ‘Emphasize your strong points?’). Items were rated on a ‘1. Rarely or never’ to ‘4. Very often’ scale. A total mean score was calculated, with higher scores indicating more social support (α = 0.85; Ω = 0.87).

Social processes

Interpersonal skills were assessed with the Dutch self-report ‘Questionnaire Psychosocial Skills’ (VPV; (Scholte & Van der Ploeg, Reference Scholte and Van der Ploeg2013). The VPV consists of the two subscales ‘Interpersonal Skills’ and ‘Intrapersonal Skills’ – only the former subscale was used. This subscale, in turn, consists of two subscales on ‘Relational Skills’ (e.g. ‘I seek out to connect with peers that I like’) and ‘Affective Skills’ (e.g. ‘I recognize in others how they feel or think’), each consisting of nine items rated on a scale ranging from ‘1. Completely disagree’ to ‘5. Completely agree’. We used the full ‘Interpersonal Skills’ subscale, which showed good reliability (α = 0.83; Ω = 0.86).

Experience sampling methodology

To obtain the moment-level indicators of quantity/quality of daily social interactions, we draw on the social interaction data obtained throughout our six-day ESM period. The full ESM questionnaire is included in online Supplementary Material S2, and is also available in the online ESM Item Repository (www.esmitemrepository.com; Kirtley et al., Reference Kirtley, Hiekkaranta, Kunkels, Eisele, Verhoeven, Nierop and Myin-Germeys2020).

Quantity of Social Interactions. A dichotomous moment-level ‘Social Interaction’ score was constructed by taking the ESM prompts where participants indicated being both in the company of others (as indicated by any company type as a response to the item ‘Who am I with?’) and interacting with them (as indicated by at least a ‘2’ on the item ‘We are doing something together’). As the person-mean level of social interactions is predicted in each multilevel logistic regression model, we label this variable ‘Quantity of Social Interactions’.

Quality of Social Interactions. If participants indicated company in the ESM questionnaire, three qualitative items followed (‘I feel comfortable in this company’, ‘I feel valued in this company’, ‘I feel like I belong’), all rated from ‘1. Not at all’ to ‘7. Very much’. A mean ‘Quality of Social Interactions’ score was computed by taking the moment-level mean of these three items. Note that, strictly speaking, these items assess the experienced quality of social interaction at each time point – only as assessed by the individual participant. Within-person reliability for this variable was ω within = 0.86; between-person reliability was ω between = 0.92.

Statistical analysis

Sample size and statistical power

Prior to data collection, a simulation-based sample size calculation was conducted for the relatively complex research question on affective reactivity to the environment, which resulted in an estimated power of 0.94 for a total n = 2.001 (see Kirtley et al., Reference Kirtley, Achterhof, Hagemann, Hermans, Hiekkaranta, Lecei and Myin-Germeys2021), for more details). For the relatively simpler linear regression analyses included in this study, a sensitivity power calculation was performed in G × Power, to calculate the minimal effect size that we are able to find. This demonstrated that a sample size of 1913 would be able to detect at least a small (f2 = 0.01) effect size with 90% power, α = 0.05 and 6 predictors (4 tested + 2 covariates) in a multiple linear regression analysis.

Missing data handling

For the missing data on all (non-ESM) questionnaires, we imputed data at the item-level, based on the information of all other variables, using a multiple imputation model by chained equations (MICE), with the ‘mice’-package v. 3.14.0 (van Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2011) in R. All estimates were obtained by pooling the results (using Rubin's rules; Rubin, Reference Rubin1987) of each performed analysis on the 20 imputed datasets.

Analysis strategy

First, basic descriptive statistics were computed. The investigation of age and gender effects was performed by including these variables as covariates within the following models.

Second, associations between risk/protective factors and mental health outcomes were estimated in linear regression models estimating the effects of each of four risk/protective factors on each of four psychopathology outcomes. Corrected models included all risk/protective factors simultaneously and one psychopathology outcome; uncorrected models included one risk/protective factor per model.

Third, the associations between all risk/protective factors and interpersonal functioning (the first of three social processes) were estimated similarly: predicting interpersonal functioning in corrected and uncorrected linear regression models with the risk/protective factors as predictors. As the quantity and quality of social interaction variables are moment- (rather than person-)level variables, they necessitated a multilevel approach. Logistic multilevel regression models were estimated to assess the effect of each risk/protective factor on the probability of engaging in a social interaction at any given time point. Linear multilevel regression models were estimated to assess the effect of each risk/protective factor on the mean social interaction quality at any given time point.

Fourth, we needed to consider that, in these multilevel models, it is impossible to predict a Level 2-outcome (i.e. psychopathology) from a Level-1 predictor (i.e. quantity/quality of social interaction) (Preacher, Zyphur, & Zhang, Reference Preacher, Zyphur and Zhang2010). As we were interested in cross-sectional associations rather than any temporal (let alone causal) ordering of effects, we considered it appropriate to estimate the effect of each psychopathology variable on the quantity/quality of social interactions – rather than vice versa. We estimated the effects of each psychopathology variable on the quantity of social interactions in logistic multilevel regression models, and on the quality of social interactions in linear multilevel regression models. We also estimated the effect of psychopathology on interpersonal functioning in a set of linear regression models.

Finally, to assess differential effects according to age, interactions between age and each relevant predictor were added to all uncorrected models described above.

Open science practices

All hypotheses and the full analysis plan for the study were post-registered – a type of pre-registration occurring after data collection, but before data access or analysis (Benning, Bachrach, Smith, Freeman, & Wright, Reference Benning, Bachrach, Smith, Freeman and Wright2019) – on the Open Science Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/jhav7/?view_only=74cf9b5ba7924e35ba7d7bcd02dbd812). The limited deviations from the registration are listed in online Supplementary Material S3. For additional transparency, all used R code and output is made available online (osf.io/6h7be/).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics of all included variables are listed in Table 1 (all reflecting the imputed data; see online Supplementary Material S4 for descriptive statistics of raw data). N = 1913 adolescents (n = 1214 girls) participated in the study. Seven participants indicated their gender as other than male or female; gender information was missing for four participants. Three age cohorts were included, from three different years of Flemish secondary school: 1st year students (~ 12 years old; n = 1048), 3rd year students (~14 years old; n = 424), and 5th year students (~16 years old; n = 441). Compliance to the ESM protocol was relatively low, with, on average, 40% (s.d. = 22.8) of daily questionnaires answered by participants.

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of full sample, pooled across the 20 multiple imputed datasets

Correlations between all included continuous variables are presented in Table 2. Correlations between all psychopathology variables are generally quite high, ranging from 0.67 to 0.82 for the correlations between depression, anxiety, and psychoticism.

Table 2. Pearson correlations between all included (person-level) variable, pooled across the 20 multiple imputed datasets

Note: PPC, Parental Psychological Control; ACE: Adverse Childhood Experiences; GSI, General Severity Index (total psychopathology score).

a The quantity and quality of social interaction variables represent person-level aggregates representing person means.

*p < 0.01; **p < 0.001.

Age, gender and psychopathology

As age and gender were included as covariates in the prediction of outcomes of psychopathology, we can see in Table 3 how – for general psychopathology, depression, anxiety, and psychoticism – girls reported significantly more symptoms than boys. Also, compared to younger participants, older participants reported significantly more depressive symptoms, but there was no significant relationship between age and general psychopathology, psychoticism or anxiety, after correcting for multiple testing.

Table 3. Associations between risk/protective factors and psychopathology

PPC, parental psychological control; GSI, General Severity Index (general psychopathology); ACE, Adverse Childhood Experiences.

Corrected models include all variables in that column simultaneously as predictors; uncorrected models include age, sex, and each risk/protective factor separately as predictor.

Since each column with uncorrected associations represents four separate models with different values for intercept, age, and sex, the cells representing those values are empty in this table (see full model output online for coefficients, osf.io/6h7be/).

Note: p values in bold indicate a significant effect following Holm's multiple comparison correction with initial α = 0.05; p values in italics indicate a significant effect after α = 0.05.

Risk/protective factors and psychopathology

All four risk and protective factors were significantly associated with psychopathology in four separate models, both in the model of general psychopathology as well as in the models of anxiety, depression, and psychoticism (Table 3, uncorrected models). Higher levels of ACE, bullying and psychological parental control as well as lower levels of social support were associated with higher levels of psychopathology. When all predictors were entered into one model, associations remained significant (Table 3, corrected models), except for the association between social support and anxiety symptoms.

Risk/protective factors and social processes

All four risk and protective factors were significant in the models predicting the three social processes (Table 4, uncorrected models). So, higher levels of ACE, bullying, psychological parental control and lower levels of social support were associated with fewer interpersonal skills, and a lower quantity and quality of social interactions. When predictors were entered simultaneously, their unique contributions remained significant (Table 4, corrected models), except for the associations between bullying and interpersonal skills, and between bullying and quantity of social interactions.

Table 4. Associations between risk/protective factors and social processes

PPC, parental psychological control; SI, social interactions; ACE, Adverse Childhood Experiences.

Corrected models include all variables in that column simultaneously as predictors; uncorrected models include age, sex, and each risk/protective factor separately as predictor.

Note: p values in bold indicate a significant effect following Holm's multiple comparison correction with initial α = 0.05; p values in italics indicate a significant effect after α = 0.05.

Since each column with uncorrected associations represents four separate models with different values for intercept, age, and sex, those values are not included in this table (see full model output online for coefficients, osf.io/6h7be/).

Social processes and psychopathology

The four measures of psychopathology were all significantly associated with the three types of social processes (Table 5, uncorrected models). Higher levels of general psychopathology, anxiety, depression, and psychoticism were all associated with fewer interpersonal skills, and a lower quantity and quality of social interactions. When depression, anxiety, and psychoticism symptoms were entered simultaneously in the model, we only observed significant, unique associations between depressive symptoms and each of the three social processes (Table 5, corrected models).

Table 5. Associations between psychopathology and social processes

SI, social interactions; GSI, General Severity Index (general psychopathology score).

Corrected models include all variables (except for GSI) in that column simultaneously as predictors; uncorrected models include age, sex, and each type of psychopathology separately as predictor.

Note: p values in bold indicate a significant effect following Holm's multiple comparison correction with initial α = 0.05; p values in italics indicate a significant effect after α = 0.05.

Since each column with uncorrected associations represents four separate models with different values for intercept, age, and sex, those values are not included in this table (see full model output online for coefficients, osf.io/6h7be/).

Age-related differences in effects

All differences in effects across age groups are represented by the interaction effects in online Supplementary Tables S1–S12 in the online Supplementary Material S5. Most of these interaction effects are not statistically significant, indicating that most investigated associations did not differ as a function of age. However, there are some statistically significant interaction effects (following multiple comparisons correction): The negative relationship between general psychopathology/depression/psychoticism and interpersonal skills is stronger for those in the 3rd or 5th grade, compared to those in the 1st grade (online Supplementary Tables S9/S10/S12; online Supplementary Material S5); the negative relationship between anxiety and interpersonal skills is stronger for those in the 5th grade than for those in the 1st grade (online Supplementary Table S11; online Supplementary Material S5); the negative relationship between general psychopathology/depression/psychoticism and the quality of daily-life social interactions is stronger for those in the 5th grade, compared to for those in the 1st grade (online Supplementary Tables S9/S10/S12; online Supplementary Material S5). Following the registered analysis plan on age-related effects, sensitivity analyses were conducted that excluded people whose age strongly differed from their classmates (i.e. at least two years younger or older than the mean age in their grade). These analyses showing no substantive difference with the main results (online Supplementary Material S1, osf.io/6h7be/).

Discussion

The main aim of this study was to investigate the associations between known risk and protective factors, social processes, and general psychopathology in a general population sample of Flemish adolescents. We found significant associations between all types of variables: The social processes that we investigated were all meaningfully related to both risk/protective factors, and to different types of psychopathology. Although the conceptual model of a social mediation toward psychopathology still needs to be confirmed with longitudinal data (Fig. 1), these findings support the broad link between (day-to-day) social processes in youth mental health.

The reported results on the moderate prevalence of mental health symptoms are in line with previous research (Merikangas et al., Reference Merikangas, He, Burstein, Swanson, Avenevoli, Cui and Swendsen2010). Also, older adolescents reported more depressive symptoms than younger adolescents, and girls reported more symptoms (of every type) than boys. These findings are in line with other epidemiological work showing more depressive/anxiety psychopathology problems for girls than for boys (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Bann and Patalay2021; Zahn-Waxler, Shirtcliff, & Marceau, Reference Zahn-Waxler, Shirtcliff and Marceau2008), and increased psychopathology as adolescents age (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Amminger, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Lee and Üstün2007; Solmi et al., Reference Solmi, Radua, Olivola, Croce, Soardo, Salazar de Pablo and Fusar-Poli2021).

As hypothesized, we consistently found the risk factors parental psychological control, bullying, and adverse childhood experiences to be associated with higher psychopathology levels, whereas more social support was associated with less psychopathology. This is in line with previous meta-analytic evidence highlighting how higher levels of parental psychological control (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017), childhood trauma (McKay et al., Reference McKay, Cannon, Chambers, Conroy, Coughlan, Dodd and Clarke2021), and bullying (Moore et al., Reference Moore, Norman, Suetani, Thomas, Sly and Scott2017), and lower levels of social support (Scardera et al., Reference Scardera, Perret, Ouellet-Morin, Gariépy, Juster, Boivin and Geoffroy2020), are all related to more mental health problems in adolescents and/or young adults.

However, the main added value of the proposed model and its tested associations, is that we also found unique relationships between these risk/protective factors and different social processes – both in terms of interpersonal skills and differential quantity/quality of daily social interactions. More adverse childhood experiences and psychological control were associated with fewer interpersonal skills and a lower quantity and quality of daily-life social interaction, while social support showed reverse associations. Also, we found higher levels of different types of psychopathology to be associated with fewer interpersonal skills and a lower quantity and quality of social interactions. This demonstrates that each of these factors, that we know to be related to mental health, and mental health itself, can be uniquely linked to how young people interact with each other.

Still, with these significant findings, we need to acknowledge that we cannot speak to the processes that underlie the reported associations. Given previous longitudinal evidence of the predictive effects of good social relations on mental health (Milner, Krnjacki, & LaMontagne, Reference Milner, Krnjacki and LaMontagne2016), and the temporal precedence of most investigated risk/protective factors, we consider it likely that social processes (at least, to some extent) mediate the relationship between those risk/protective factors and psychopathology (cf. Fig. 1). However, given the cross-sectional and observational nature of the presented data, the veracity of the proposed mediation model remains to be tested.

It is also likely, for example, that latent psychopathology levels form the main driver of decreased interpersonal skills or lower levels of quantity/quality of social interactions (cf. the traditional conceptualization of social functioning as an outcome of mental health problems; e.g. Burns & Patrick, Reference Burns and Patrick2007). Learning more about the ordering of these effects is essential to better understand whether alterations in social processes are drivers, manifestations, or correlations of psychopathology. To inform on this temporal ordering of these effects (a necessary condition for determining the causality of effects; Shrout, Reference Shrout2011), longitudinal follow-up of these data is needed.

In addition, following leading theories of developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti, Toth, Luthar, Burack, Cicchetti and Weisz1997), the underlying processes may also be inherently transactional in nature. Transactional models of development emphasize how maladaptation (here, psychopathology) can be produced by a dynamic, reciprocal interplay between the individual and (social) context (Sameroff & MacKenzie, Reference Sameroff and MacKenzie2003). This dynamic unfolding toward a range of mental health outcomes can be kick-started by a range of risk factors such as those investigated in this study. Transactional models of developmental psychopathology imply that it is in immediate feedback loops between individuals themselves and their proximal (social) environments that mental health develops. For future work, it would be valuable to test such models more directly, by assessing those transactions between individual and social context (e.g. with ESM), and ascertaining to what extent they predict longer-term development. Although all data collected here were cross-sectional in nature, the simultaneous inclusion of different age cohorts allows for a preliminary investigation of developmental differences. Notably, we observed strengthened associations between social processes and different types of psychopathology for older (as compared to younger) participants. The stronger relationship between (a lack of) interpersonal skills and psychopathology may reflect the developmental goal of adolescents to increasingly seek out others to connect with (Wrzus et al., Reference Wrzus, Hänel, Wagner and Neyer2013). As adolescents age, they generally engage in more social interactions with peers. Those with a lack of interpersonal skills may have lower-quality social interactions, which, in turn, may be driving the rise of psychopathology symptoms. This hypothesis is also supported by the strengthened association between the quality of social interactions and psychopathology symptoms for the oldest cohort in the current study.

This increased relevance of social factors – particularly in daily life – as adolescents age also provides a potential fruitful avenue for prevention and early intervention efforts. The key imperative to strengthen youth mental health efforts (McGorry, Purcell, Hickie, & Jorm, Reference McGorry, Purcell, Hickie and Jorm2007) can be improved by focusing on those areas that are both meaningfully related to mental health and that are amenable to change. Although this study is unable to speak to the causal role of social processes in psychopathology development, previous evidence does strongly suggest the predictive adaptive effects of better social relations (Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith and Layton2010; Milner et al., Reference Milner, Krnjacki and LaMontagne2016). What requires further investigation, however, is the predictive effect of day-to-day social interaction (quality) in later mental health. If micro-level social processes (as investigated here) are indeed evidenced to be early warning signals if later psychopathological development, real-time interventions aimed at young people's daily social lives (such as Moderated Online Social Therapy+; Rice et al., Reference Rice, Gleeson, Leicester, Bendall, D'Alfonso, Gilbertson and Alvarez-Jimenez2018) may prove to be particularly effective. The moderate overlap between the different social processes investigated in this study suggests that they may each have their own unique value in this endeavor. What also needs to be considered in such developments, is the unique experience and potential benefit of online in addition to face-to-face interactions (Achterhof et al., Reference Achterhof, Kirtley, Schneider, Hagemann, Hermans, Hiekkaranta and Myin-Germeys2022b) – and to what extent online social interactions are able to fulfil young people's social needs.

The results of this study should, however, be viewed in light of its limitations. First, since the data were cross-sectional, follow-up waves are needed to test the direction of effects. Second, due to the time constraint when collecting data, there is a relatively high level of missingness within the data – a limitation that was partially tackled by employing a state-of-the-art multiple imputation technique to handle missing data (Chhabra, Vashisht, & Ranjan, Reference Chhabra, Vashisht and Ranjan2017). Within the ESM protocol, as well, participants in this study were less compliant than in similar ESM studies in adolescents (40% v. average of 74%, van Roekel, Keijsers, & Chung, Reference van Roekel, Keijsers and Chung2019), potentially leading to biased results. Despite this low compliance, the relatively high number of daily prompts (10) still gives us a large amount of data to draw on, while the short response rate to each questionnaire (90 s) and the prompting in naturalistic school settings are unique features ensuring high ecological validity of the data. Third, our bullying measure only consisted of two items on physical bullying, thereby not capturing the full scope of bullying (excluding e.g. emotional, verbal, and cyberbullying).

Two additional strengths should be noted. This study is one of the largest experience-sampling datasets in existence, thereby providing novel insights into the relationships between adolescents' day-to-day social lives and their mental health. In addition, the adherence to open-science practices (registration of the analyses pre-data analysis, the online publication of code and materials) strengthens the robustness, replicability, and reproducibility of the reported findings.

Conclusion

In this study, we confirmed that, in adolescents from the general population, social processes (social skills, quantity, and quality of everyday social interactions) are associated with risk and protective factors for psychopathology, and with depression, anxiety, and psychotic symptoms. Although further longitudinal work is required to verify the directionality of these associations, these results highlight the transdiagnostic relevance of social processes for different types of early psychopathology – perhaps even more so for late adolescence. A focus on everyday social interactions may aid in a better understanding of how known risk/protective factors can lead to different types of psychopathology in young people. Moreover, given the importance and targetable nature of social processes, an increased social focus will prove valuable in boosting prevention and intervention efforts.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001594

Funding statement

This study is supported by funding from Research Foundation Flanders, with an FWO Odysseus grant (Myin-Germeys, FWO G 08416N) supporting RA, APH, NH, KSFMH, and IMG. OJK is supported by a Senior Postdoctoral Fellowship from Research Foundation Flanders (FWO 1257821N). During preparation of this article, OJK was also supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from a Research Foundation Flanders Odysseus grant to IMG (FWO GOF8416N). MS is supported by a personal grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant number: PZ00P1_174206). RvW is supported by an FWO Senior Clinical Fellowship (FWO 1803616N) and by the Funds Julie Renson, Queen Fabiola and King Baudoin Foundation.

Competing interests

None.

References

Achterhof, R., Kirtley, O. J., Schneider, M., Lafit, G., Hagemann, N., Hermans, K. S. F. M., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2021). Daily-life social experiences as a potential mediator of the relationship between parenting and psychopathology in adolescence. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 1279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.697127CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achterhof, R., Kirtley, O. J., Schneider, M., Hagemann, N., Hermans, K. S. F. M., Hiekkaranta, A. P., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2022a). General psychopathology and its social correlates in the daily lives of youth. Journal of Affective Disorders, 309, 428436. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.04.147CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Achterhof, R., Kirtley, O. J., Schneider, M., Hagemann, N., Hermans, K. S. F. M., Hiekkaranta, A. P., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2022b). Adolescents’ real-time social and affective experiences of online and face-to-face interactions. Computers in Human Behavior, 129, 107159. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asselmann, E., Wittchen, H.-U., Lieb, R., & Beesdo-Baum, K. (2018). Sociodemographic, clinical, and functional long-term outcomes in adolescents and young adults with mental disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 137(1), 617. doi: 10.1111/acps.12792CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67(6), 32963319. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01915.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barzeva, S. A., Richards, J. S., Veenstra, R., Meeus, W. H. J., & Oldehinkel, A. J. (2022). Quality over quantity: A transactional model of social withdrawal and friendship development in late adolescence. Social Development, 31(1), 126146. doi: 10.1111/sode.12530CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benning, S. D., Bachrach, R. L., Smith, E. A., Freeman, A. J., & Wright, A. G. C. (2019). The registration continuum in clinical science: A guide toward transparent practices. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128(6), 528540. doi: 10.1037/abn0000451CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 187207. doi: 10.1146/psych.2013.65.issue-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brendgen, M. (2018). Peer victimization and adjustment in young adulthood: Introduction to the special section. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(1), 59. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0347-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, T., & Patrick, D. (2007). Social functioning as an outcome measure in schizophrenia studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 116(6), 403418. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01108.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, O. L. K., Bann, D., & Patalay, P. (2021). The gender gap in adolescent mental health: A cross-national investigation of 566829 adolescents across 73 countries. SSM – Population Health, 13, 100742. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2021.100742CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Ambler, A., Danese, A., Elliott, M. L., Hariri, A., … Moffitt, T. E. (2020). Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study. JAMA Network Open, 3(4), e203221. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chhabra, G., Vashisht, V., & Ranjan, J. (2017). A comparison of multiple imputation methods for data with missing values. Indian Journal of Science and Technology, 10(19), 17. doi: 10.17485/ijst/2017/v10i19/110646CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1997). Transactional ecological systems in developmental psychopathology. In Luthar, S., Burack, J. A., Cicchetti, D., & Weisz, R. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder (pp. 317349). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Colizzi, M., Lasalvia, A., & Ruggeri, M. (2020). Prevention and early intervention in youth mental health: Is it time for a multidisciplinary and trans-diagnostic model for care? International Journal of Mental Health Systems, 14(1), 23. doi: 10.1186/s13033-020-00356-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(9), 526536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalgard, O. S., Bjork, S., & Tambs, K. (1995). Social support, negative life events and mental health. British Journal of Psychiatry, 166(JAN.), 2934. doi: 10.1192/bjp.166.1.29CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, P. T., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2014). Family context in the development of psychopathology. In Lewis, M. & Rudolph, D. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 143161). Boston, MA: Springer US. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-9608-3_8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derogatis, L. R. (1993). BSI: Brief symptom inventory. Minneapolis: National Computer Systems.Google Scholar
Galbraith, S., Bowden, J., & Mander, A. (2017). Accelerated longitudinal designs: An overview of modelling, power, costs and handling missing data. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 26(1), 374398. doi: 10.1177/0962280214547150CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grusec, J. E., & Davidov, M. (2010). Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: A domain-specific approach. Child Development, 81(3), 687709. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01426.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamby, S. L., Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., & Turner, H. (2004). The Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire (JVQ): Unh.Edu, (March).Google Scholar
Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J. G. (2009). Research electronic data capture (REDCap)-A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 42(2), 377381. doi: 10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermans, K., Achterhof, R., Myin-Germeys, I., Kasanova, Z., Kirtley, O. J., & Schneider, M. (2019). Improving ecological validity in research on social cognition. In Lewandowski, K. E. & Moustafa, A. A. (Eds.), Social cognition in psychosis (pp. 249268). London, England: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-815315-4.00010-0Google Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLOS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Üstün, T. B. (2007). Age of onset of mental disorders: A review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 20(4), 359364. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32816ebc8cCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirtley, O. J., Achterhof, R., Hagemann, N., Hermans, K. S. F. M., Hiekkaranta, A. P., Lecei, A., … Myin-Germeys, I. (2021). Initial cohort characteristics and protocol for SIGMA: An accelerated longitudinal study of environmental factors, inter- and intrapersonal processes, and mental health in adolescence. PsyArXiv. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/jp2fkGoogle Scholar
Kirtley, O. J., Hiekkaranta, A. P., Kunkels, Y. K., Eisele, G., Verhoeven, D., Nierop, M. V., & Myin-Germeys, I. (2020). The Experience Sampling Method (ESM) Item Repository. doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/KG376CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klasen, F., Otto, C., Kriston, L., Patalay, P., Schlack, R., & Ravens-Sieberer, U., & The BELLA study group. (2015). Risk and protective factors for the development of depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: Results of the longitudinal BELLA study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24(6), 695703. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0637-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kline, R. B. (2015). The mediation myth. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 37(4), 202213. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2015.1049349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maxwell, S. E., & Cole, D. A. (2007). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation. Psychological Methods, 12(1), 2344. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.1.23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGorry, P. D., Purcell, R., Hickie, I. B., & Jorm, A. F. (2007). Investing in youth mental health is a best buy. Medical Journal of Australia, 187(S7), S5S7. 10.5694/j.1326-5377.2007.tb01326.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKay, M. T., Cannon, M., Chambers, D., Conroy, R. M., Coughlan, H., Dodd, P., … Clarke, M. C. (2021). Childhood trauma and adult mental disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 143(3), 189205. doi: 10.1111/acps.13268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meers, K., Dejonckheere, E., Kalokerinos, E. K., Rummens, K., & Kuppens, P. (2020). Mobileq: A free user-friendly application for collecting experience sampling data. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 15101515. doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01330-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., … Swendsen, J. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication-adolescent supplement (NCS-A). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 980989. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miché, M., Hofer, P. D., Voss, C., Meyer, A. H., Gloster, A. T., Beesdo-Baum, K., & Lieb, R. (2018). Mental disorders and the risk for the subsequent first suicide attempt: Results of a community study on adolescents and young adults. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(7), 839848. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1060-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, A., Krnjacki, L., & LaMontagne, A. D. (2016). Age and gender differences in the influence of social support on mental health: A longitudinal fixed-effects analysis using 13 annual waves of the HILDA cohort. Public Health, 140, 172178. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2016.06.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, S. E., Norman, R. E., Suetani, S., Thomas, H. J., Sly, P. D., & Scott, J. G. (2017). Consequences of bullying victimization in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. World Journal of Psychiatry, 7(1), 60. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v7.i1.60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myin-Germeys, I., Kasanova, Z., Vaessen, T., Vachon, H., Kirtley, O. J., Viechtbauer, W., & Reininghaus, U. (2018). Experience sampling methodology in mental health research: New insights and technical developments. World Psychiatry, 17(2), 123132. doi: 10.1002/wps.20513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A., Tomova, L., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2020). The effects of social deprivation on adolescent development and mental health. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 4(8), 634640. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30186-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinquart, M. (2017). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: An updated meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 873932. doi: 10.1037/dev0000295CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polanczyk, G., Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Cannon, M., Ambler, A., Keefe, R. S. E., … Caspi, A. (2010). Etiological and clinical features of childhood psychotic symptoms: Results from a birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(4), 328338. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation. Psychological Methods, 15(3), 209233. doi: 10.1037/a0020141.suppCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W., & Condon, D. (2019). Reliability from alpha to omega: A tutorial. Psychological Assessment, 31(12), 13951411. doi: 10.1037/pas0000754CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, S., Gleeson, J., Leicester, S., Bendall, S., D'Alfonso, S., Gilbertson, T., … Alvarez-Jimenez, M. (2018). Implementation of the enhanced moderated online social therapy (MOST+) model within a national youth E-mental health service (eheadspace): Protocol for a single group pilot study for help-seeking young people. JMIR Research Protocols, 7(2), e48. doi: 10.2196/resprot.8813CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sameroff, A. J., & MacKenzie, M. J. (2003). Research strategies for capturing transactional models of development: The limits of the possible. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 613640. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000312CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scardera, S., Perret, L. C., Ouellet-Morin, I., Gariépy, G., Juster, R.-P., Boivin, M., … Geoffroy, M.-C. (2020). Association of social support during adolescence with depression, anxiety, and suicidal ideation in young adults. JAMA Network Open, 3(12), e2027491. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.27491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholte, E. M., & Van der Ploeg, J. D. (2013). Vragenlijst psychosociale vaardigheden (VPV). Handleiding. Houten: Bohn Stafleu van Loghum.Google Scholar
Segrin, C. (2019). Indirect effects of social skills on health through stress and loneliness. Health Communication, 34(1), 118124. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2017.1384434CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shrout, P. E. (2011). Commentary: Mediation analysis, causal process, and cross-sectional data. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46(5), 852860. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2011.606718CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Metzger, A. (2006). Adolescent development in interpersonal and societal contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 57(1), 255284. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G., Robinson, J., & Rote, W. M. (2015). Socialization in adolescence. In Grusec, J. E. & Hastings, P. D. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 6084). New York: Guilford Press. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2015-05080-003Google Scholar
Solmi, M., Radua, J., Olivola, M., Croce, E., Soardo, L., Salazar de Pablo, G., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2021). Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: Large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Molecular Psychiatry, 17, 22. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01161-7Google Scholar
van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). Mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3), 167. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03Google Scholar
van Roekel, E., Keijsers, L., & Chung, J. M. (2019). A review of current ambulatory assessment studies in adolescent samples and practical recommendations. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(3), 560577. doi: 10.1111/jora.12471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Sonderen, E. (2012). Het meten van sociale steun met de Sociale steun lijst -interacties (SSL-I) en Sociale steun lijst -discrepanties (SSL-D): Een handleiding (2nd ed.). Groningen, the Netherlands: UMCG / Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, Research Institute SHARE.Google Scholar
Wrzus, C., Hänel, M., Wagner, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2013). Social network changes and life events across the life span: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 5380. doi: 10.1037/a0028601CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn-Waxler, C., Shirtcliff, E. A., & Marceau, K. (2008). Disorders of childhood and adolescence: Gender and psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 275303. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Implied mediation model underlying the reciprocal associations between social processes and psychopathology, and the effects of risk/protective factors.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of full sample, pooled across the 20 multiple imputed datasets

Figure 2

Table 2. Pearson correlations between all included (person-level) variable, pooled across the 20 multiple imputed datasets

Figure 3

Table 3. Associations between risk/protective factors and psychopathology

Figure 4

Table 4. Associations between risk/protective factors and social processes

Figure 5

Table 5. Associations between psychopathology and social processes

Supplementary material: File

Achterhof et al. supplementary material

Achterhof et al. supplementary material
Download Achterhof et al. supplementary material(File)
File 77.8 KB