Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T00:52:08.110Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Borderline personality disorder symptom networks across adolescent and adult clinical samples: examining symptom centrality and replicability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2022

Jessica R. Peters*
Affiliation:
Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, USA
Michael L. Crowe
Affiliation:
Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, USA VA Boston Healthcare System, Boston, USA
Theresa Morgan
Affiliation:
Butler Hospital, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, USA
Mark Zimmerman
Affiliation:
Rhode Island Hospital, Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, USA
Carla Sharp
Affiliation:
University of Houston, Houston, USA
Carlos M. Grilo
Affiliation:
Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, USA
Charles A. Sanislow
Affiliation:
Wesleyan University, Middletown, USA
M. Tracie Shea
Affiliation:
Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, USA
Mary C. Zanarini
Affiliation:
McLean Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA
Thomas H. McGlashan
Affiliation:
Yale School of Medicine, New Haven, USA
Leslie C. Morey
Affiliation:
Texas A&M University, College Station, USA
Andrew E. Skodol
Affiliation:
University of Arizona College of Medicine, Tucson, USA
Shirley Yen
Affiliation:
Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, USA Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center, Massachusetts Mental Health Center, Harvard Medical School, Boston, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Jessica R. Peters, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Numerous theories posit different core features to borderline personality disorder (BPD). Recent advances in network analysis provide a method of examining the relative centrality of BPD symptoms, as well as examine the replicability of findings across samples. Additionally, despite the increase in research supporting the validity of BPD in adolescents, clinicians are reluctant to diagnose BPD in adolescents. Establishing the replicability of the syndrome across adolescents and adults informs clinical practice and research. This study examined the stability of BPD symptom networks and centrality of symptoms across samples varying in age and clinical characteristics.

Methods

Cross-sectional analyses of BPD symptoms from semi-structured diagnostic interviews from the Collaborative Longitudinal Study of Personality Disorders (CLPS), the Methods to Improve Diagnostic Assessment and Service (MIDAS) study, and an adolescent clinical sample. Network attributes, including edge (partial association) strength and node (symptom) expected influence, were compared.

Results

The three networks were largely similar and strongly correlated. Affective instability and identity disturbance emerged as relatively central symptoms across the three samples, and relationship difficulties across adult networks. Differences in network attributes were more evident between networks varying both in age and in BPD symptom severity level.

Conclusions

Findings highlight the relative importance of affective, identity, and relationship symptoms, consistent with several leading theories of BPD. The network structure of BPD symptoms appears generally replicable across multiple large samples including adolescents and adults, providing further support for the validity of the diagnosis across these developmental phases.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe psychiatric disorder entailing intense, unstable affect; self-destructive, impulsive behavior; difficulties with interpersonal relationships; and unstable identity (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). A diagnosis of BPD can be met by the presence of any combination of at least five of the nine DSM diagnostic criteria, resulting in a highly heterogenous disorder with 256 possible combinations of symptoms. Numerous theories have arisen positing different core components of the disorder. Some highlight emotion dysregulation (Crowell, Beauchaine, & Linehan, Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Glenn & Klonsky, Reference Glenn and Klonsky2009; Linehan, Reference Linehan1993), including affective lability and components of impulsivity (Links, Heslegrave, & van Reekum, Reference Links, Heslegrave and van Reekum1999), as central. Other theories focus on difficulties in interpersonal relationships (Gunderson, Reference Gunderson2007) and understanding of the self (Kaufman & Meddaoui, Reference Kaufman and Meddaoui2021; Kerr, Finlayson-Short, McCutcheon, Beard, & Chanen, Reference Kerr, Finlayson-Short, McCutcheon, Beard and Chanen2015) as hallmarks of BPD. Factor analysis of the nine symptoms found a three-factor grouping into the categories of affective dysregulation, behavioral disinhibition, and disturbed relatedness (Sanislow, Grilo, & McGlashan, Reference Sanislow, Grilo and McGlashan2000; Sanislow et al., Reference Sanislow, Grilo, Morey, Bender, Skodol, Gunderson and McGlashan2002); however, these analytic approaches do not examine how these factors and symptoms within them interconnect within the disorder.

Network analysis has recently gained traction as a method to model psychopathology symptoms (Borsboom, Reference Borsboom2017). In contrast to latent approaches that model symptoms as indicators of an underlying, single source of variance (dimensions), the network approach models symptoms as a reinforcing network of biological factors, emotions, cognitions, and behaviors. Symptoms, modeled as network ‘nodes,’ are assumed to cohere due to causal relationships between them (modeled as ‘edges’), rather than due to a single underlying cause. This approach may hold particular relevance for disorders such as BPD whose symptoms span a wide range of affect, cognition, and behavior that may emerge at different developmental points, differentially influence each other, and thus vary in how central their role is within the syndrome (Fried & Cramer, Reference Fried and Cramer2017).

In addition to modeling the expected influence of symptoms within networks, network structures can be compared across samples, allowing researchers to examine the replicability of symptom networks and specific network characteristics (Costantini et al., Reference Costantini, Richetin, Preti, Casini, Epskamp and Perugini2019; Fried et al., Reference Fried, Eidhof, Palic, Costantini, Huisman-van Dijk, Bockting and Karstoft2018). This facilitates comparisons across meaningful sample differences, such as developmental stages. Although the DSM permits diagnosing BPD in adolescents with marked and persistent symptoms of the disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) and mounting evidence demonstrates the validity of the diagnosis in adolescents (Chanen, Sharp, & Hoffman, Reference Chanen, Sharp and Hoffman2017), adolescent BPD remains a controversial topic (Miller, Muehlenkamp, & Jacobson, Reference Miller, Muehlenkamp and Jacobson2008), with many clinicians and researchers misinformed on the topic (Griffiths, Reference Griffiths2011; Laurenssen, Hutsebaut, Feenstra, Van Busschbach, & Luyten, Reference Laurenssen, Hutsebaut, Feenstra, Van Busschbach and Luyten2013), and adolescent BPD is thus underdiagnosed, undertreated, and understudied (Sharp & Fonagy, Reference Sharp and Fonagy2015).

Limited work to date has applied network approaches to BPD symptoms, all in adults. In one study of a large, non-clinical student sample, as well as a small (N = 96) clinical sample, affective lability was the most central self-reported BPD symptom across indices (Richetin, Preti, Costantini, & De Panfilis, Reference Richetin, Preti, Costantini and De Panfilis2017); these findings were replicated in a study of a large sample (N = 5221) of psychiatric patients (Peckham et al., Reference Peckham, Jones, Snorrason, Wessman, Beard and Björgvinsson2020). Similarly, a study using a large sample of students and treatment-seeking community members modeled networks of self-reported BPD features (rather than diagnostic criteria), also finding a central role of emotion dysregulation, as well as interpersonal difficulties (Southward & Cheavens, Reference Southward and Cheavens2018). A limitation across all of the network studies is that none use clinical diagnostic interviews to assess the BPD criteria modeled in networks, and the only study in a large clinical sample used a screening measure meant to detect possible symptoms with higher sensitivity (i.e. the McLean Screening Instrument for BPD), v. measures with higher specificity to determine symptom presence.

Only one study examined potential age effects on network structure, finding a stronger link between anger and unstable relationships and a weaker link between emptiness and suicide in participants aged >46 years compared to participants ⩽46 years old (Peckham et al., Reference Peckham, Jones, Snorrason, Wessman, Beard and Björgvinsson2020). However, this study did not include adolescents and therefore cannot address issues of BPD network stability across this key developmental stage. In fact, no work published to date has examined BPD symptom networks in adolescent samples (clinical or otherwise) or compared networks across adolescent and adult samples. While prior work examined intercorrelations among BPD criteria in adolescents compared with adults (Becker et al., Reference Becker, Grilo, Morey, Walker, Edell and McGlashan1999), this study used small samples (Ns <50) and did not utilize methods like network analysis that can compare network features and centrality of specific nodes.

The goals of the present study were to estimate networks and centrality estimates of BPD symptoms, established through validated diagnostic interviews, and to estimate these networks across three large clinical samples of variable age and BPD symptom severity. We were interested in determining the degree of stability of network features across these different samples, as well as identifying divergent characteristics, with a particular focus on comparing the stability across adults and adolescents. We hypothesized that affective instability would be a central feature across all three networks, based on prior findings. We did not have a hypothesis about the exact degree of replicability of BPD network structure nor which features would diverge.

Methods

Participants

Data from three previously collected samples were analyzed for the present study. One sample is the baseline data from the Collaborative Longitudinal Study of Personality Disorders (CLPS), a longitudinal, multisite study examining the course of personality disorders, based out of four Northeastern US medical centers (Gunderson et al., Reference Gunderson, Shea, Skodol, McGlashan, Morey, Stout and Keller2000; Skodol et al., Reference Skodol, Gunderson, Shea, McGlashan, Morey, Sanislow and Stout2005). Participants (N = 733; 731 with complete BPD symptom data used) were recruited from clinics with advertisements targeting individuals with current or prior treatment history. Individuals meeting criteria for one of four personality disorders (BPD, schizotypal, obsessive-compulsive, and avoidant) or major depressive disorder with no more than 2 PD criteria for any PD (and less than 15 total) were eligible to participate. The majority of participants were female (64%) and White (67%), with a mean age of 32.50 years (s.d. = 8.11) and a range of 18–45 years.

A second adult sample was obtained via the Methods to Improve Diagnostic Assessment and Service (MIDAS) project, based out of Rhode Island Hospital (Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman and First2003). Participants (N = 3800) were recruited through a community-based, outpatient psychiatry practice affiliated with an academic medical center, where research is integrated as part of a standardized intake process (Zimmerman, Reference Zimmerman and First2003). The practice does not restrict services based on presenting issue or diagnosis, and therefore patients represent a range of psychiatric concerns. A subsample of 3651 with complete BPD symptom data were used in the present study. The majority of the patients were female (60%) and White (87%), with a mean age of 38.81 years (s.d. = 13.35) and a range of 16–85 years old.

An adolescent sample (N = 1021) was recruited in the Houston area from inpatient psychiatric hospital settings (n = 872; 85%), through consecutive admissions [see prior publications for further details (Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Williams, Ha, Baumgardner, Michonski, Seals and Fonagy2009)] and an additional healthy comparison group (n = 149; 15%) recruited through schools and community resources (Penner, McLaren, Leavitt, Akca, & Sharp, Reference Penner, McLaren, Leavitt, Akca and Sharp2019). The majority of the patients were female (64%) and White (62%), with a mean age of 15.10 years (s.d. = 1.43) and an age range of 12–18 years.

Measures

In the CLPS sample, BPD symptoms were assessed using the Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (DIPD-IV; Zanarini, Frankenburg, Sickel, & Yong, Reference Zanarini, Frankenburg, Sickel and Yong1996). The DIPD–IV is a semi-structured interview assessing each criterion of the DSM–IV PDs, which are rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not present; 1 = present but of uncertain clinical significance; 2 = present and clinically significant). In the CLPS sample, interrater reliability of the DIPD–IV (kappa) was 0.68 for BPD (Zanarini et al., Reference Zanarini, Skodol, Bender, Dolan, Sanislow, Schaefer and Gunderson2000).

In the MIDAS sample, BPD symptoms were assessed with the BPD module of the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, Reference Pfohl, Blum and Zimmerman1997). Symptoms are scored on a 4-point scale (0 = not present or limited to rare isolated occasions; 1 = subthreshold: some evidence of the trait but it is not sufficiently pervasive or severe to consider the criterion; 2 = full threshold: criterion is clearly present but may be better accounted for by an Axis I disorder; 3 = full threshold: criterion is clearly present but is not better accounted for by an Axis I disorder). Given that scores of 2 and 3 do not differentiate symptom severity and also reflect a distinction not assessed in the other measures used, these scores were collapsed into a single score (2) for the present study. Interrater reliability for BPD criteria was assessed in a subsample (n = 44), with a mean kappa of 0.71.

Adolescent BPD symptoms were assessed using the Childhood Interview for Borderline Personality Disorder (CI-BPD; Zanarini, Reference Zanarini2003). The CI-BPD is a semi-structured interview developed specifically for use with children and adolescents to assess BPD and has shown strong psychometric properties in adolescents (Michonski, Sharp, Steinberg, & Zanarini, Reference Michonski, Sharp, Steinberg and Zanarini2013; Sharp, Ha, Michonski, Venta, & Carbone, Reference Sharp, Ha, Michonski, Venta and Carbone2012). The interview was adapted from the BPD module of the DIPD-IV interview, described above. Symptoms are scored as absent (0), probably present (1), or definitely present (2). In the present sample, 10% of the interviews (n = 107) were coded by a second-rater for interrater reliability. Agreement on dichotomous BPD diagnosis between the two raters was substantial (κ = 0.74, p < 0.01).

Analyses

Ordinal BPD symptom data (ratings from 0 to 2) were used to estimate partial Spearman correlation symptom networks. All individual networks were estimated using the R (R Core Team, 2020) for statistical computing bootnet (Epskamp, Borsboom, & Fried, Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018) package. Networks were estimated individually for each sample using an Extended Bayesian Information Criterion (Chen & Chen, Reference Chen and Chen2008) (EBIC) graphical LASSO (Tibshirani, Reference Tibshirani1996) regularization. This regularization approach penalizes for complexity to minimize the estimation of spurious edges and return a more conservative network (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018). A threshold was also added to each network to increase reliability by excluding edges of limited clinical significance.Footnote 1 Nonparametric bootstrapping was used to estimate edge weight accuracy and centrality stability and the identified confidence intervals were used to test the strength/influence differences of edges and nodes within samples (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018).

Individual network nodes were compared across samples on several metrics. Our primary metric of centrality is expected influence, which performs comparably or better than the strength centrality metric (Robinaugh, Millner, & McNally, Reference Robinaugh, Millner and McNally2016). The expected influence of a node is the sum of the edge weights connecting it to other nodes. Additionally, we also estimated and present node predictability, defined as the percentage of variability in a given node that is associated with other nodes in the network (Haslbeck & Waldorp, Reference Haslbeck and Waldorp2018). The higher a predictability score, the more that node can be accounted for by other internal network nodes, relative to variability independent from other nodes.

To examine differences between the networks, the expected influence of both individual nodes (symptoms) and strength of edges (connections between symptoms) were compared across the samples in pairwise tests using the NetworkComparisonTest (NCT) (Fried et al., Reference Fried, Eidhof, Palic, Costantini, Huisman-van Dijk, Bockting and Karstoft2018; Van Borkulo et al., Reference Van Borkulo, Boschloo, Kossakowski, Tio, Schoevers, Borsboom and Waldorp2016), corrected for multiple comparisons using Benjamini and Hochberg False Discovery Rate (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995), consistent with prior network analyses of BPD (Peckham et al., Reference Peckham, Jones, Snorrason, Wessman, Beard and Björgvinsson2020). While the NCT package has only been validated for Pearson partial correlation networks, the Pearson and Spearman correlation matrices were comparable for each sample (CLPS r = 0.99; MIDAS r = 0.98; Adolescent r = 0.99). R code for all analyses is provided in online Supplemental Materials.

Results

Sample descriptives

Symptom endorsement means and standard deviations in each sample, as well as patterns of significant differences between samples, are reported in Table 1. Across symptoms, the CLPS sample generally had the highest levels of endorsement, followed by the adolescent sample, followed by the MIDAS sample, with only one symptom not significantly different between each adjacent sample. Within the CLPS sample, 46% met the criteria for BPD (⩾5 of 9 symptoms rated 2), with M = 3.41 (s.d. = 2.70) criteria endorsed across the full sample. Within MIDAS, 10% met criteria, M = 1.74 (s.d. = 1.97), and within the adolescent sample, 26.2% met criteria, M = 2.78 (s.d. = 2.41).

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of BPD symptoms in three samples

Note: Means with different superscripts are significantly different at p < 0.05. Symptoms were scored on the following three-point scales: CLPS (0 = not present, 1 = present but of uncertain clinical significance, 2 = present and clinically significant); MIDAS (0 = not present or limited to rare isolated occasions, 1 = subthreshold: some evidence of the trait but it is not sufficiently pervasive or severe to consider the criterion, 2 = full threshold: criterion is clearly present but may be better accounted for by an Axis I disorder or full threshold: criterion is clearly present and is not better accounted for by an Axis I disorder); Adolescent (0 = absent, 1 = probably present, 2 = definitely present).

Independent network estimation

Individually estimated symptom network models and node influence estimates are presented in Fig. 1a. Network node locations are based on an average network and held constant across the samples to allow for easier visual comparisons. In the models, edge thickness represents edge weight; however, relative node locations in network models should not be used to infer centrality, which is instead presented in Fig. 1b graph. See online Supplementary Materials for tables providing all edge, expected influence, and predictability values for each network along with bootstrapped confidence intervals. In the CLPS network, predictability ranged from 0.09 (feelings of emptiness) to 0.28 (relationship instability). In the MIDAS network, predictability ranged from 0.17 (efforts to avoid abandonment) to 0.39 (affective instability). In the adolescent network, predictability ranged from 0.19 (efforts to avoid abandonment) to 0.36 (affective instability). The robustness of each network was estimated using the correlation stability (CS) coefficient (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018).Footnote 2 Network edges (CLPS CS = 0.53; MIDAS CS = 0.86; Adolescent CS = 0.68) and expected influence (CLPS CS = 0.41; MIDAS CS = 0.83; Adolescent CS = 0.48) were reliable.

Fig. 1. Individually estimated regularized partial Spearman correlation networks (a) and standardized expected influence (b) across three samples. Network edge thickness represents the degree of association. The gray area in the rings around the network nodes depicts predictability (the variance of a given node explained by all connected nodes). Ang, intense anger; Aff, affective instability; Emp, feelings of emptiness; IDD, identify disturbance; Dis, stress-related paranoia or dissociation; Abn, efforts to avoid abandonment; SIB, suicidal or self-injurious; Imp, self-damaging impulsivity; Rel, unstable relationships. All edges are positive. Edges with a magnitude of less than 0.1 are not displayed.

Within each network, bootstrapped estimates were used to compare expected influence across nodes (see Fig. 2). Within the CLPS network, unstable relationships, affective instability, identity disturbance, and self-harming behaviors were all significantly greater in expected influence than at least one other node, and emptiness were significantly lower in expected influence than all four. In MIDAS, the nodes separated mostly into two groups, with affective instability, unstable relationships, identity disturbance, and intense anger all having significantly greater expected influence than most other nodes. In the Adolescent sample, affective instability was significantly stronger than most other symptoms, and stress-related paranoia or dissociation was stronger than four of the nine symptoms.

Fig. 2. Expected influence centrality difference tests based on nanoparametric bootstrapping within each of the three samples. Black cell indicates a significant difference between the associated centrality estimates. The diagonal of each plot displays the observed expected influence for each of the nodes. Ang, intense anger; Aff, affective instability; Emp, feelings of emptiness; IDD, identify disturbance; Dis, stress-related paranoia or dissociation; Abn, efforts to avoid abandonment; SIB, suicidal or self-injurious; Imp, self-damaging impulsivity; Rel, unstable relationships.

The overall networks demonstrated strong Spearman rank correlations between samples, with CLPS and MIDAS correlated at 0.78, CLPS and Adolescent at 0.55, and MIDAS and Adolescent at 0.46. Network comparison tests (see Table 2) demonstrated stronger global (across all nodes) expected influence within the CLPS network than the MIDAS network, and paranoia/dissociation having greater expected influence in the CLPS network and suicidal/self-injurious behaviors in MIDAS network. The CLPS and MIDAS networks had one difference in edge strength, with affective instability and anger more strongly associated in the MIDAS sample. The CLPS and adolescent networks had no significant differences in global expected influence or specific node expected influence differences and one difference in edge strength, with anger and impulsivity more strongly associated in the adolescent sample. The MIDAS and adolescent samples had the most significant differences, with stronger global expected influence within the adolescent network and with the dissociation/paranoia node having greater expected influence in adolescents. Seven (of 36) edges with strength differences. Affective instability demonstrated stronger associations with anger and relationships in MIDAS; other edge differences included a range of nodes and were stronger in the adolescent sample. Overall, these findings suggest that independently estimated network structures are largely similar across the samples, especially between the two adult samples (CLPS and MIDAS) and the two samples with higher levels of BPD pathology (CLPS and adolescent).

Table 2. Significant (with Benjamini–Hochberg corrections) network comparison results contrasting node strength and edge weights in each pair of BPD symptom networks

Discussion

Findings provide support for a largely replicable BPD network structure across three samples differing in age and levels of BPD pathology, including adolescent and adult samples. Networks were strongly correlated across the three samples, suggesting a largely uniform network structure for BPD. This is consistent with a prior finding examining BPD networks across age in adulthood that found few age-related changes (Peckham et al., Reference Peckham, Jones, Snorrason, Wessman, Beard and Björgvinsson2020). While the network structures were strongly correlated, the MIDAS sample demonstrated a less interconnected network overall, relative to the CLPS and Adolescent samples, which may reflect the MIDAS study tapping a population with more diverse forms of psychopathology v. a higher rate of individuals endorsing the full BPD syndrome in the CLPS sample, which specifically recruited for personality disorders, and the Adolescent sample, which recruited from adolescent inpatient settings where BPD is relatively common.

Partially consistent with our hypothesis, affective instability emerged as a highly central symptom across all three networks; however, in each network, other symptoms were not distinctively less central, with identity disturbance similarly highly central across all three samples. Relationship instability emerged as relatively central as well in the two adult samples. The relative centrality of affective instability is consistent with past research identifying the trait as central to the BPD construct (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Houben, Santangelo, Kleindienst, Tuerlinckx, Oravecz and Kuppens2015). The DSM-5 Section III Alternative Model of Personality Disorders (AMPD) includes emotional lability among the maladaptive traits diagnostic of BPD, which is a similar construct to affective instability (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The observed centrality of relationship and identity disturbance is consistent with both the DSM-5 AMPD and ICD-11 (World Health Organization, 2018) characterizations of BPD, which identify instability of relationships and self-image as core functional impairments of the disorder.

Despite networks being largely similar, several significant differences in node centrality and edge weight emerged that may relate to differences between the samples. When samples only differed by one major characteristic (age or BPD pathology), network differences were minimal, but the two samples differing on both factors demonstrated the greatest number of divergent features. Unstable relationships had greater relative centrality in adult networks only, whereas dissociation/paranoia was among the most central nodes only in the adolescent network. These findings suggest the possibility that age and/or level of BPD pathology may influence the functioning of some symptoms within the BPD network. Relationship-related dysfunction, as well as fear of abandonment, may become more interlinked with other BPD symptoms as individuals age and develop more significant romantic relationships. The greater centrality of paranoid and dissociative symptoms in the adolescent sample than the MIDAS sample may reflect age differences, but also differences in severity of BPD pathology. For adolescents with severe psychopathology, stress from other BPD symptoms may be more likely to result in paranoia and dissociation or signal risk for other severe mental illness that may develop later in life. Similarly, the greater expected influence between anger and impulsive behavior in adolescents, relative to the two adult networks, may reflect heightened anger fueling more dysregulated behavior in younger individuals. Given prior findings of a stronger relationship between anger and relationship dysfunction within adults as they age (Peckham et al., Reference Peckham, Jones, Snorrason, Wessman, Beard and Björgvinsson2020), it is also possible that the negative impact of maladaptive anger shifts as individuals develop, from primarily impulsive behavior in adolescence toward relationship impairment.

Replications across more samples with age and BPD severity differences are needed to test whether these sample characteristics are reliably linked to these differences in network characteristics or shared markers for varied prognoses. Future work should include multiple adolescent samples of varying levels of BPD, as well as samples of older adults, to better examine network constancy and changes over developmental course. Of note, none of these differences was sufficient to prevent strong correlations between the networks across all samples, suggesting an overall robust syndrome.

It is important to note that although network models are based on a causal theory, all analyses in the present manuscript are cross-sectional and correlational, and therefore causal inferences cannot be made from these findings. Rather, a key takeaway is that across each of these samples, generally similar patterns of associations between these symptoms emerge. These findings support a reproducible BPD syndrome, although given typically high rates of diagnostic co-occurrence with BPD, we acknowledge that this syndrome may be a broad indicator of personality pathology in general, and that more nuanced differences might emerge if symptoms of other disorders were also modeled (Sharp et al., Reference Sharp, Wright, Fowler, Frueh, Allen, Oldham and Clark2015). To test hypotheses that these pathways may be causal, longitudinal data are needed to model network development and flux over time, particularly across key developmental time periods such as adolescence. In addition, variables that may validate causal explanations (e.g. neurobiological data) would be important in future work. Another potential limitation in the analytic approach includes the potential for the NCT procedure to have limited power, so the null findings should be conservatively considered. Future research with other samples may benefit from utilizing new analytic approaches to compare networks as these are developed and validated.

A strength of this study is the use of three clinical samples with differing levels of care and recruitment approaches; however, generalizability is limited given that the samples are from two geographic regions in the US with majority White participants. It is essential to extend this line of enquiry to samples representing all forms of diversity that may affect BPD network structure, with adequate numbers to test whether differences exist across those groups or based on environmental factors. Although a strength of the study was the use of validated clinical interviews to establish BPD criteria and the assessment tools had notable similarities across samples (clinician-administered, semi-structured, delivered by experienced and trained interviewers who were monitored to prevent drift), the exact interviews and rating scales varied slightly between samples, and this may have affected results. Using interviews with the same coding scales would be ideal in future work.

Overall, this study provides support for a replicable network of BPD symptoms across adolescent and adult samples of variable levels of BPD severity. Our findings support both conceptualizations of BPD that place either affective instability (Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Harvey, Mitropoulou, Schmeidler, New, Goodman and Siever2002; Linehan, Reference Linehan1993) or identity disturbance (Fuchs, Reference Fuchs2007; Jørgensen, Reference Jørgensen2010) as central to the disorder, consistent with prior network models of BPD (Richetin et al., Reference Richetin, Preti, Costantini and De Panfilis2017). These findings augment mounting evidence that BPD is a relatively generalizable and valid syndrome across adolescents and adults, indicating the need to continue efforts to understand the phenomenology of the disorder, as well as to expand both research studies into and clinical services for adolescent BPD.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004931

Financial support

This research was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health NIMH (MH112889, Brown, PI Peters; MH050837, Brown University, PI Shea; MH050839, Columbia University and New York State Psychiatric Institute, PI Skodol; MH050840, McLean Hospital/Harvard Medical School; MH050838, Texas A&M, PI Morey; MH0580850, Yale University, PI McGlashen) and by the McNair Family foundation (University of Houston, PI Sharp). The funders did not have any role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Footnotes

*

Shared first authorship.

1 Elements of the precision matrix below ${\rm log} (p \times ((p-1) / 2)) \over \sqrt n$ were removed (Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann, & Borsboom, Reference Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann and Borsboom2012; Jankova & van de Geer, Reference Jankova, van de Geer, Maathuis, Drton, Lauritzen and Wainwright2019)

2 The CS value indicates the maximum percentage of cases that can be dropped from the data while retaining, with 95% probability, a correlation of at least 0.7 between the statistics based on the original network and those computed with fewer randomly sampled cases. Values greater than 0.5 are desirable, values greater than 0.25 are acceptable (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018).

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®) (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Becker, D. F., Grilo, C. M., Morey, L. C., Walker, M. L., Edell, W. S., & McGlashan, T. H. (1999). Applicability of personality disorder criteria to hospitalized adolescents: Evaluation of internal consistency and criterion overlap. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(2), 200205. doi:10.1097/00004583-199902000-00020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300. doi:10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D. (2017). A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry, 16(1), 513. doi:10.1002/wps.20375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chanen, A., Sharp, C., & Hoffman, P. (2017). Prevention and early intervention for borderline personality disorder: A novel public health priority. World Psychiatry, 16(2), 215216. doi:10.1002/wps.20429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, J., & Chen, Z. (2008). Extended Bayesian information criteria for model selection with large model spaces. Biometrika, 95(3), 759771. doi:10.1093/biomet/asn034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costantini, G., Richetin, J., Preti, E., Casini, E., Epskamp, S., & Perugini, M. (2019). Stability and variability of personality networks. A tutorial on recent developments in network psychometrics. Personality and Individual Differences, 136, 6878. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., & Linehan, M. M. (2009). A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: Elaborating and extending Linehan's theory. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 495510. doi:10.1037/a0015616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Houben, M., Santangelo, P., Kleindienst, N., Tuerlinckx, F., Oravecz, Z., … Kuppens, P. (2015). Unraveling affective dysregulation in borderline personality disorder: A theoretical model and empirical evidence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(1), 186198. doi:10.1037/abn0000021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., & Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 195212. doi:10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O. J., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., & Borsboom, D. (2012). qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(1), 118. doi:10.18637/jss.v048.i04.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, E. I., & Cramer, A. O. J. (2017). Moving forward: Challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 9991020. doi:10.1177/1745691617705892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, E. I., Eidhof, M. B., Palic, S., Costantini, G., Huisman-van Dijk, H. M., Bockting, C. L. H., … Karstoft, K.-I. (2018). Replicability and generalizability of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) networks: A cross-cultural multisite study of PTSD symptoms in four trauma patient samples. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(3), 335351. doi:10.1177/2167702617745092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, T. (2007). Fragmented selves: Temporality and identity in borderline personality disorder. Psychopathology, 40(6), 379387. doi:10.1159/000106468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, C., & Klonsky, E. (2009). Emotion dysregulation as a core feature of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(1), 2028. doi:10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffiths, M. (2011). Validity, utility and acceptability of borderline personality disorder diagnosis in childhood and adolescence: Survey of psychiatrists. The Psychiatrist, 35(1), 1922. doi:10.1192/pb.bp.109.028779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunderson, J. G. (2007). Disturbed relationships as a phenotype for borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(11), 16371640. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunderson, J. G., Shea, M. T., Skodol, A. E., McGlashan, T. H., Morey, L. C., Stout, , R. L., … Keller, M. B. (2000). The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study: Development, aims, design, and sample characteristics. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14(4), 300315. doi:10.1521/pedi.2000.14.4.300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslbeck, J. M. B., & Waldorp, L. J. (2018). How well do network models predict observations? On the importance of predictability in network models. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 853861. doi:10.3758/s13428-017-0910-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jankova, J., & van de Geer, S. (2019). Inference for high-dimensional graphical models. In Maathuis, M., Drton, M., Lauritzen, S, & Wainwright, M. (Eds.), Handbook of Graphical Models (pp. 325346). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, C. R. (2010). Invited essay: Identity and borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 24(3), 344364. doi:10.1521/pedi.2010.24.3.344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, E. A., & Meddaoui, B. (2021). Identity pathology and borderline personality disorder: An empirical overview. Current Opinion in Psychology, 37, 8288. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.08.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, I. B., Finlayson-Short, L., McCutcheon, L. K., Beard, H., & Chanen, A. M. (2015). The ‘self’ and borderline personality disorder: Conceptual and clinical considerations. Psychopathology, 48(5), 339348. doi:10.1159/000438827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenigsberg, H. W., Harvey, P. D., Mitropoulou, V., Schmeidler, J., New, A. S., Goodman, M., … Siever, L. J. (2002). Characterizing affective instability in borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(5), 784788. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laurenssen, E. M. P., Hutsebaut, J., Feenstra, D. J., Van Busschbach, J. J., & Luyten, P. (2013). Diagnosis of personality disorders in adolescents: A study among psychologists. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 7(1), 3. doi:10.1186/1753-2000-7-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder (1st ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Links, P., Heslegrave, R., & van Reekum, R. (1999). Impulsivity: Core aspect of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 13(1), 19. doi:10.1521/pedi.1999.13.1.1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michonski, J. D., Sharp, C., Steinberg, L., & Zanarini, M. C. (2013). An item response theory analysis of the DSM-IV borderline personality disorder criteria in a population-based sample of 11- to 12-year-old children. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 4(1), 1522. doi:10.1037/a0027948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. L., Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Jacobson, C. M. (2008). Fact or fiction: Diagnosing borderline personality disorder in adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review, 28(6), 969981. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.02.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peckham, A. D., Jones, P., Snorrason, I., Wessman, I., Beard, C., & Björgvinsson, T. (2020). Age-related differences in borderline personality disorder symptom networks in a transdiagnostic sample. Journal of Affective Disorders, 274, 508514. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penner, F., McLaren, V., Leavitt, J., Akca, O. F., & Sharp, C. (2019). Implicit and explicit mentalizing deficits in adolescent inpatients: Specificity and incremental value of borderline pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 34(Suppl. B), 6483. doi:10.1521/pedi_2019_33_463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfohl, B., Blum, N., & Zimmerman, M. (1997). Structured interview for DSM-IV personality: SIDP-IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Richetin, J., Preti, E., Costantini, G., & De Panfilis, C. (2017). The centrality of affective instability and identity in borderline personality disorder: Evidence from network analysis. PLoS One, 12(10), e0186695. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0186695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinaugh, D. J., Millner, A. J., & McNally, R. J. (2016). Identifying highly influential nodes in the complicated grief network. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(6), 747757. doi:10.1037/abn0000181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanislow, C., Grilo, C., & McGlashan, T. (2000). Factor analysis of the DSM-III-R borderline personality disorder criteria in psychiatric inpatients. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(10), 16291633. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanislow, C., Grilo, C., Morey, L., Bender, D., Skodol, A., Gunderson, J., … McGlashan, T. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of DSM-IV criteria for borderline personality disorder: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(2), 284290. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharp, C., & Fonagy, P. (2015). Practitioner review: Borderline personality disorder in adolescence – Recent conceptualization, intervention, and implications for clinical practice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(12), 12661288. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., Ha, C., Michonski, J., Venta, A., & Carbone, C. (2012). Borderline personality disorder in adolescents: Evidence in support of the childhood interview for DSM-IV borderline personality disorder in a sample of adolescent inpatients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53(6), 765774. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.12.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharp, C., Williams, L. L., Ha, C., Baumgardner, J., Michonski, J., Seals, , R., … Fonagy, P. (2009). The development of a mentalization-based outcomes and research protocol for an adolescent inpatient unit. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 73(4), 311338. doi:10.1521/bumc.2009.73.4.311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., Wright, A. G. C., Fowler, J. C., Frueh, B. C., Allen, J. G., Oldham, J., & Clark, L. A. (2015). The structure of personality pathology: Both general (‘g’) and specific (‘s’) factors? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(2), 387398. doi:10.1037/abn0000033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Shea, M. T., McGlashan, T. H., Morey, L. C., Sanislow, C. A., … Stout, R. L. (2005). The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study (CLPS): Overview and implications. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19(5), 487504. doi:10.1521/pedi.2005.19.5.487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Southward, M. W., & Cheavens, J. S. (2018). Identifying core deficits in a dimensional model of borderline personality disorder features: A network analysis. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(5), 685703. doi:10.1177/2167702618769560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tibshirani, R. (1996). Regression shrinkage and selection via the lasso. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological, 58(1), 267288. doi:10.1111/j.2517-6161.1996.tb02080.x.Google Scholar
Van Borkulo, C. D., Boschloo, L., Kossakowski, J., Tio, P., Schoevers, R. A., Borsboom, D., … Waldorp, L. J. (2016). Comparing network structures on three aspects: A permutation test. Manuscript submitted for publication. doi:10.13140/RG.2.2.29455.38569.Google Scholar
World Health Organization. (2018). International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics (11th Revision). Genève, Switzerland: World Health Organization. Retrieved from https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en.Google Scholar
Zanarini, M. C. (2003). Childhood Interview for DSM-IV Borderline Personality Disorder (CI-BPD). Belmont, MA:McLean Hospital and Harvard Medical School.Google Scholar
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Sickel, A. E., & Yong, L. (1996). The Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (DIPD-IV). Belmont, MA: McLean Hospital and Harvard Medical School.Google Scholar
Zanarini, M. C., Skodol, A. E., Bender, D., Dolan, R., Sanislow, C., Schaefer, E., … Gunderson, J. G. (2000). The collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study: Reliability of axis I and II diagnoses. Journal of Personality Disorders, 14(4), 291299. doi:10.1521/pedi.2000.14.4.291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, M. (2003). Integrating the assessment methods of researchers in routine clinical practice: The Rhode Island Methods to Improve Diagnostic Assessment and Services (MIDAS) project. In First, M. B. (Ed.), Standardized Evaluation in Clinical Practice (pp. 2974). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of BPD symptoms in three samples

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Individually estimated regularized partial Spearman correlation networks (a) and standardized expected influence (b) across three samples. Network edge thickness represents the degree of association. The gray area in the rings around the network nodes depicts predictability (the variance of a given node explained by all connected nodes). Ang, intense anger; Aff, affective instability; Emp, feelings of emptiness; IDD, identify disturbance; Dis, stress-related paranoia or dissociation; Abn, efforts to avoid abandonment; SIB, suicidal or self-injurious; Imp, self-damaging impulsivity; Rel, unstable relationships. All edges are positive. Edges with a magnitude of less than 0.1 are not displayed.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Expected influence centrality difference tests based on nanoparametric bootstrapping within each of the three samples. Black cell indicates a significant difference between the associated centrality estimates. The diagonal of each plot displays the observed expected influence for each of the nodes. Ang, intense anger; Aff, affective instability; Emp, feelings of emptiness; IDD, identify disturbance; Dis, stress-related paranoia or dissociation; Abn, efforts to avoid abandonment; SIB, suicidal or self-injurious; Imp, self-damaging impulsivity; Rel, unstable relationships.

Figure 3

Table 2. Significant (with Benjamini–Hochberg corrections) network comparison results contrasting node strength and edge weights in each pair of BPD symptom networks

Supplementary material: File

Peters et al. supplementary material

Peters et al. supplementary material

Download Peters et al. supplementary material(File)
File 27 KB