Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T00:08:10.095Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Symposium on ‘Nutrition and health in children and adolescents’ Session 2: Dietary quality and dietary recommendations in children and adolescents Dietary quality and adequacy of micronutrient intakes in children

A meeting of the Nutrition Society hosted by the Irish Section was held on 14–16 June 2006 at University College Cork, Cork, Republic of Ireland

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 March 2007

Inga Thorsdottir*
Affiliation:
Unit for Nutrition Research, University of Iceland and Landspitali-University Hospital, IS-101Reykjavik, Iceland
Björn S. Gunnarsson
Affiliation:
Unit for Nutrition Research, University of Iceland and Landspitali-University Hospital, IS-101Reykjavik, Iceland
*
*Corresponding author: Professor Inga Thorsdottir, fax +354 543 4824, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Presented are longitudinal studies, extending from infancy (n 180) to 2 years of age (n 130) and 6 years of age (>70% participation) of diet and Fe status in a population with high birth weight, high frequency of breast-feeding and, at the time of the study, high intake of cow's milk during the weaning period. The association between socio-demographic and dietary factors was also studied, together with Fe status in early childhood and developmental status at 6 years. Fe status was found to be poorer than in the neighbouring Nordic countries. Every fifth 1-year-old was Fe-deficient (serum ferritin <12 μg/l and mean corpuscular volume <74 fl). It was demonstrated by regression analysis that Fe status was negatively associated with cow's milk consumption at 9–12 months (significant at >460 g/d) and was weakly positively associated with fish, meat and Fe-fortified cereal consumption. Fe-deficient infants had a shorter duration of breast-feeding, and breast-feeding was related to slower growth, which can protect from worsening Fe status. Fe deficiency was less common at ages 2 and 6 years. Maternal factors associated with lower adherence to the recommended infant diet were less education, lower age and smoking. In a multiple stepwise regression analysis that included food factors, socio-demographic factors were not found to be associated with Fe status. Fe-depleted and Fe-deficient 1-year-olds had lower fine motor scores when they were 6 years old than those who were not Fe-deficient or Fe-depleted. The findings of these studies have already led to changes in the local recommendations for diet in infancy. The results suggest that Fe deficiency at 12 months of age affects development at 6 years of age. The studies indicated that mothers with less education, who smoked and who were younger needed more guidance concerning recommendations about diet in infancy.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 2006

Abbreviations: MCV

mean corpuscular volume

rsp

Spearman's rho correlation

SF

serum ferritin

Dietary quality, including sufficient micronutrients, is one of the most important factors for the health and well-being of today's children in developing and developed countries. The nutritional needs of children should be constantly reviewed and the levels for requirements and recommended intakes redefined according to the latest evidence. Late in 2004 a new edition of Nordic Nutrition Recommendations, Integrating Nutrition and Physical Activity was published (Alexander et al. Reference Alexander, Anderssen, Aro, Becker, Fogelholm, Lyhne, Meltzer, Pedersen and Thorsdottir2004). These recommendations are based on the present nutrition situation in the Nordic countries and are to be used for planning diets that satisfy the nutritional needs of each age-group, i.e. covering the physiological requirement for growth and metabolism. In general, an adequate diet is a prerequisite for overall good health and contributes to the risk reduction of diet-associated diseases. In many cases the values for infants and children have been derived from adult data using either body weight or energy requirement as a basis for the estimation. Recommendations for single nutrients are not given for the age-group 0–6 months except for vitamin D. Vitamin D supplementation is required at northern latitudes in order to ensure infants do not develop rickets. Fe deficiency is probably the most frequently observed nutrient deficiency worldwide.

The main reasons for inadequate Fe status in infants and young children are considered to be fast growth in infancy, which leads to a high demand for Fe (Dallmann et al. Reference Dallmann, Siimes and Stekel1980), and also both low Fe content and Fe bioavailability in the weaning diet (Lind et al. Reference Lind, Lönnerdal, Persson, Stenlund, Tennefors and Hernell2003). A full-term infant of normal weight usually has sufficient Fe stores for the first 6 months of life, but subsequently Fe from dietary sources play an increasing role in supplying Fe for the body (Department of Health, 1994). Fe homeostasis is maintained through absorption. Compared with many nutrients Fe is poorly absorbed, and another feature of human Fe metabolism is the absence of an excretory pathway. From studies on adults it has been shown that absorption in the intestine depends on the Fe status of the body, the amount and type of Fe in the diet and the composition of the meal (Bothwell et al. Reference Bothwell, Baynes, MacFarlane and MacPhail1989; Lynch et al. Reference Lynch, Skikne and Cook1989; Brune et al. Reference Brune, Rossander-Hultén, Hallberg, Gleerup and Sandberg1992; Lonnerdal & Hernell, Reference Lonnerdal and Hernell1994). It has been suggested that during the first 6 months the infant may not be able to down regulate their absorption of Fe as efficiently as children and adults (Domellof & Hernell, Reference Domellof and Hernell2002). Thus, it is important to have an appropriate diet that simultaneously avoids Fe deficiency and Fe overload.

The negative association between cow's milk intake and Fe status in infancy is the best-known interrelationship affecting Fe status in young childhood (Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995; Gill et al. Reference Gill, Vincent and Segal1997; Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Mulder, van't Hof, Hoey and Gibney1998; Lawson et al. Reference Lawson, Thomas and Hardiman1998; Bramhagen & Axelsson, Reference Bramhagen and Axelsson1999; Thane et al. Reference Thane, Walmsley, Bates, Prentice and Cole2000). Prolonged duration of exclusive breast-feeding, i.e. up to 9 months of age, has also been associated with poor Fe status (Pizarro et al. Reference Pizarro, Yip, Dallman, Olivares, Hertrampf and Walter1991). Birth weight is positively associated with Fe status in infancy (Wharf et al. Reference Wharf, Fox, Fairweather-Tait and Cook1997) and at ⩽12 (Persson et al. Reference Persson, Lundstrom, Lonnerdal and Hernell1998) and 18 months of age (Sherriff et al. Reference Sherriff, Emond, Hawkins and Golding1999). However, fast growth during the first year is negatively associated with Fe status (Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995) and birth weight is inversely related to postnatal growth (Thorsdottir & Birgisdottir, Reference Thorsdottir and Birgisdottir1998; Atladottir & Thorsdottir, Reference Atladottir and Thorsdottir2000). Thus, it was important to evaluate the association between Fe status and body size and growth, as well as dietary factors.

The official recommendation for the infant diet in Iceland has been similar to that of most countries; however, Iceland has differed from most countries in its recommendation on weaning: cow's milk after the age of 6 months (Steingrimsdottir, Reference Steingrimsdottir1995; Palsson et al. Reference Palsson, Sigurdsson and Gudbjornsdottir1996). However, cow's milk is known to negatively influence the Fe status in infants (Tunnessen & Oski, Reference Tunnessen and Oski1987; Mills, Reference Mills1990). It was therefore of great concern and theoretical interest to investigate the effect of the frequent use of cow's milk for infants between 6 and 12 months of age on the Fe status at 12 months in the Icelandic population.

In the present studies the effects of food intake and growth on Fe status were investigated at the ages of 12 months, 2 years and 6 years in Icelandic children who were born into an affluent society with a high birth weight (among the highest in the world) and a high frequency of breast-feeding (Thorsdottir & Birgisdottir, Reference Thorsdottir and Birgisdottir1998) and, at the time of the infant study, a high intake of cow's milk during the weaning period. The effects of Fe deficiency in early childhood on developmental status were also studied on starting school or at 6 years of age. The findings of these studies have already led to a change in the recommendations in Iceland, and a special milk product for infants from 6 months of age and children ⩽2 years has been developed. As social factors may affect food habits and the feeding of infants, the effects of several socio-demographic factors on adherence to recommendations for feeding infants were also investigated.

Subjects and methods

Study design

The study design is outlined in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the study design showing the data collected and used in the analysis for the two cohorts of Icelandic children: infant cohort (cohort 1); 2-year-old cohort (cohort 2).

Study population

The studies are based on two random cohorts: a longitudinal study on infant nutrition, cohort 1 (n 180); a cross-sectional study of 2-year-olds, cohort 2 (n 130). Of the eligible participants >70% were involved in the Fe-status surveys at age 1, 2 and 6 years.

Parents were contacted and given information about the studies and invited to participate on behalf of their children. All individual information was confidential and informed consent was obtained. The studies were approved by the Local Ethical Committee at Landspitali-University Hospital and by the Icelandic Data Protection Commission. The two cohorts and methods have been described in Thorsdottir et al. (Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsson, Atladóttir, Michaelsen and Palsson2003) and Gunnarsson et al. (Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2004).

Growth

Growth data were collected at healthcare centres. In the infant study information on weight and length was gathered from the maternity wards involved in the study and the participating families' healthcare centres. At 2 and 6 years of age the children's height and weight were measured in a clinical examination at Landspitali-University Hospital and other centres. Information was also obtained about the children's birth weight from their birth records.

Food records

Consumption was recorded throughout the first year (once monthly) in the infant study and information was collected about breast-feeding for the participants who were recruited at the age of 2 years. Weighed-food records for 2 d at 2, 4, 6, 9 and 12 months of age and for 3 d at 2 and 6 years of age were used in the analysis of food and nutrient intake.

Blood samples

Blood samples were taken at a visit to the University Hospital at 1, 2 and 6 years of age for analyses of Hb, serum ferritin (SF), serum transferrin receptors, mean corpuscular volume (MCV) and erythrocyte distribution width (for details of analytical methods, see Thorsdottir et al. Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsson, Atladóttir, Michaelsen and Palsson2003; Gunnarsson et al. Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2004, Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2005).

The cut-off points used to identify Fe-deficiency anaemia at 1 and 2 years of age were Hb <105 g/l, SF <12 μg/l and MCV <74 fl, and SF and MCV, with the same cut-off points, were used to identify Fe deficiency. If children had an SF of <12 μg/l they were diagnosed as having depleted Fe stores. The cut-off value for Hb of 105 g/l has been used and considered to be appropriate for infants (Siimes et al. Reference Siimes, Salmenpera and Perheentupa1984; Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995), as well as the cut-off values for MCV of 74 fl for infants and small children (Hercberg et al. Reference Hercberg, Papoz, Galan, Guery, Farnier and Rossignol1987; Fuchs et al. Reference Fuchs, Farris, DeWier, Hutchinson, Warrier, Doucet and Suskind1993; Gill et al. Reference Gill, Vincent and Segal1997). The cut-off value for SF of 12 μg/l is in accordance with WHO criteria (International Nutritional Anemia Consultative Group/World Health Organization/UNICEF, 1998; World Health Organization, 2001). At 6 years of age the cut-off points used for Fe-deficiency anaemia were Hb <115 g/l, SF <15 μg/l and MCV <76 fl, and SF and MCV, with same cut-off points, were used to identify Fe deficiency. Depleted Fe stores were diagnosed as SF of <15 μg/l. The cut-off values for Hb of 115 g/l and for SF of 15 μg/l are in accordance with World Health Organization (2001) criteria for the age-group 5–11 years in the case of Hb and for >5 years of age in the case of SF. The cut-off value for MCV of 76 fl is in accordance with the criteria used in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey II for 5–10-year-olds (Expert Scientific Working Group, 1985).

The Icelandic Developmental Inventory

At 6 years of age the Icelandic Developmental Inventory was completed by the mothers of seventy-seven participants from cohort 1, but as there were only thirty-six participants from cohort 2 no results are presented.

The Icelandic Developmental Inventory evaluates the motor and verbal development of children aged 3–6 years by collecting information from their mothers. The mothers check a list of 208 standardized questions about their child's abilities, giving scores on six subtests: (1) gross motor; (2) fine motor; (3) self help; (4) comprehension; (5) expression; (6) learning. The development and standardization of the inventory has been described previously (Thorsdottir et al. Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsdottir, Kvaran and Gretarsson2005) and has been proven to be reliable and to give valid information comparable with other tests (Gudmundsson & Gretarsson, Reference Gudmundsson and Gretarsson1993, Reference Gudmundsson and Gretarsson1994).

Socio-demographic factors

The study of infants provided information about the parents' age, education, occupation and smoking status, the family income and the number of the child's siblings. Three levels of education were used: secondary school (<11 years); gymnasium (13–14 years); university level (>15 years). The parent's educational level was measured as the highest level completed. Mothers were divided into three categories in relation to their smoking status: mothers who did not smoke; mothers who smoked, but not during pregnancy; mothers who smoked, including throughout the period of pregnancy and lactation. Family income was measured as the monthly income of the family and divided into five groups. The number of other children was divided into two groups: mothers who had previously not had a child or had one child; mothers who had two or more children before giving birth to the child in the present study.

Calculations and statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS for Windows version 12.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Data are presented as means, standard deviations and percentiles. Student's t test, Mann-Whitney U test and Fisher's exact test were used to identify differences between two groups, and Kruskal Wallis test or ANOVA test was used for three or more groups. Spearman's rho correlations (r sp) as well as stepwise linear multiple regression analyses were performed to evaluate linear associations between food and nutrient intakes, as well as other parameters, and Fe status indices. As the distribution was skewed, SF values were logarithmically transformed. The level of significance was taken as P<0·05.

Results

Iron status, diet and growth

Table 1 shows Fe status indices at age 12 months, 2 years and 6 years and the number of children below (or above) the respective cut-off values. Table 2 shows the frequency of depleted Fe stores, Fe deficiency and anaemia for the participating children at different ages, indicating that the prevalence of Fe deficiency at 12 months was high and that Fe deficiency was less common at the age of 2 and at 6 years.

Table 1. Iron status indices at the ages of 12 months, 2 years and 6 years for cohorts of Icelandic childrenFootnote *

(Values are means and standard deviations)

SF, serum ferritin; MCV, mean corpuscular volume; TfR, serum transferrin receptors; RDW, erythrocyte distribution width.

* For details of subjects and procedures, see pp. 367–368.

Cut-off values: for 1- and 2-year-old children: Hb <105 g/l, MCV <74 fl, SF <12 μg/l, TfR >8·5 mg/l, RDW >18%; for 6-year-old children: Hb <115 g/l, MCV<76 fl, SF<15 μg/l.

Table 2. Frequency (%) of depleted iron stores, iron deficiency and iron-deficiency anaemia for 1-, 2- and 6-year-old Icelandic children participating in the studyFootnote *

* For details of subjects and procedures, see pp. 367–368.

It has been reported previously (Thorsdottir et al. Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsson, Atladóttir, Michaelsen and Palsson2003) that the Fe-deficient infants had a shorter duration of breast-feeding (5·5 (sd 2·3) months) than the non-Fe-deficient children (7·9 (sd 3·2) months; P=0·001). Furthermore, regression analyses, adjusted for gender and duration of cow's milk feeding, have shown associations between log SF and the duration of both exclusive (B 0·04 (se 0·02); adjusted R 2 0·05; P=0·011; n 105) and total breast-feeding (B 0·03 (se 0·01); adjusted R 2 0·14; P=0·027; n 105), and between serum transferrin receptors:log SF and duration of total breast-feeding (B−0·44 (se 0·15); adjusted R 2 0·07; P=0·005; n 94), indicating a better Fe status with longer duration of breast-feeding.

The strongest food factor associated with Fe status at 12 months and 2 years of age was found to be cow's milk (Thorsdottir et al. Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsson, Atladóttir, Michaelsen and Palsson2003; Gunnarsson et al. Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2004). Fe status indices at 1 year of age were negatively associated with cow's milk consumption at 9–12 months, and were significant at a consumption of >460 g/d (P<0·05; see also Thorsdottir et al. Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsson, Atladóttir, Michaelsen and Palsson2003). All Fe status indices, except Hb, differed significantly between the children receiving <500 ml cow's milk daily and those receiving >500 ml cow's milk daily (Table 3). The effect of cow's milk was not seen at the age of 6 years.

Table 3. Mean iron status indices at 1 and 2 years of age in Icelandic children consuming either >500 g cow's milk/d or <500 g cow's milk/d at 9–12 months and 2 years respectively*

SF, serum ferritin; MCV, mean corpuscular volume.

* For details of subjects and procedures, see pp. 367–368.

Multiple regression analyses of Fe status indices and the intake of selected food items revealed that the consumption of cow's milk was almost universally negatively associated with Fe status indices at both 1 and 2 years of age, but not at 6 years of age (Table 4) (Gunnarsson et al. Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2006a). At 6 years of age a partial correlation of borderline significance was observed between log SF and cow's milk products (apart from cow's milk, butter and cheese; r −0·169; P=0·058). At 1 and 6 years of age the consumption of meat and fish emerged as a positive predictor for log SF, while juice consumption was associated with Hb at both ages, negatively at 1 year and positively at 6 years. At 6 years of age juice consumption and vitamin C intake were highly correlated (r sp 0·797; P<0·001), while at 12 months of age the correlation was lower (r sp 0·343; P=0·001), indicating the consumption of juices with a lower amount of vitamin C at 12 months than at 6 years. At both 1 and 2 years of age biscuits and crackers were positively associated with Fe status indices. At 6 years of age no associations were observed between MCV and the food items selected for analysis. In general, the association between the total intake of single nutrients and Fe status was similar to that for the food items containing those nutrients, e.g. a negative association between Ca and Fe status indices at 1 and 2 years of age. Breast-feeding was also related to slower growth, during the first year in particular, which seemed to protect from worsening Fe status. The effects of growth and body size on Fe status have been thoroughly described (Thorsdottir et al. Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsson, Atladóttir, Michaelsen and Palsson2003; Gunnarsson et al. Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2004, Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2005). In general, faster growth from birth to 12 months and 2 years of age seems to indicate a worse Fe status at that age, but Fe deficiency at 12 months and 2 years of age is associated with slower growth up to 6 years of age.

Table 4. Multiple regression analyses of food factors influencing iron status indices of cohorts of Icelandic children at 1 year (consumed at 12 months and the average for 9 and 12 months), 2 years and 6 yearsFootnote *

SF, serum ferritin; MCV, mean corpuscular volume; TfR, serum transferrin receptors; RDW, erythrocyte distribution width.

* For details of subjects and procedures, see pp. 367–368.

The independent variables used in the multiple regression analysis were as follows: breast milk (only in the infant study); cow's milk; other cow's milk products apart from butter and cheese; porridges (only in the infant study); meat products; fish products (meat and fish products combined in studies of 2- and 6-year-olds); fruits; vegetables (fruits and vegetables were combined in the infant study); bread, biscuits and crackers; cakes (only in the infant study); Fe-fortified breakfast cereals, butter and cheese (separated in the study of 6-year-olds); juices (with and without added sugar). The intake quantities used for cow's milk and juices were 100 g/d, but g/d for other food items. Only the variables reaching statistical significance are given in the table. Variables are adjusted for gender.

The effect of supplement intake was tested for 6-year-olds. The associations between multivitamin–mineral supplement intake (containing Fe) and Fe status was at best marginal (SF (μg/l): mean non-consumers (n 108) 23·3 (sd 10·2); supplement consumers (n 21) 30·0 (sd 16·1); P=0·070). None of the twenty-one children with depleted Fe stores consumed multivitamin–mineral supplements compared with twenty-one of the 108 non-Fe-depleted children (P=0·024). The intake of supplemental vitamin C and Fe was not associated with Fe status.

Early iron deficiency and development

Children who were Fe-deficient at 1 year of age (n 10) had lower fine motor scores at 6 years of age (46·7 (sd 4·1)) than non-Fe-deficient children (49·3 (sd 2·0); n 56; P=0·011). Children with depleted Fe stores at 1 year of age (n 26) also had lower fine motor scores (48·0 (sd 3·3)) than their non-Fe-depleted peers (49·5 (sd 1·8); n 40; P=0·045; see Fig. 2) (Gunnarsson et al. Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2006b).

Fig. 2. Fine motor scores at 6 years of age in a cohort of Icelandic children who at the age of 1 year were iron-deficient (ID) or non-iron-deficient (NID), or who had depleted iron stores (DIS) or non-depleted iron stores (N-DIS). For details of the study population, the evaluation of motor development and the study design, see p. 367, p. 368 and Fig. 1 respectively. Values are means and standard deviations represented by vertical bars. Mean value was significantly different from that for the ID group: ** P=0·011. Mean value was significantly different from that for the DIS group: † P=0·045.

Fine motor score was marginally negatively correlated with the level of serum transferrin receptors (r sp −0·21; P=0·071; n 63), but significantly correlated with serum transferrin receptors:log SF (r sp −0·28; P=0·032; n 61).

Stepwise linear multiple regression analyses, including Fe status at 1 or 2 years and 6 years, showed that Hb at 6 years was positively associated with the gross motor score (adjusted R 2 0·05; P=0·019; n 96), while Hb at 1 and 2 years was positively associated with the expression score along with gender (boys>girls; adjusted R 2 0·11; P=0·037; n 96).

Diet and socio-demographic status

Multiple regression analysis that included socio-demographic factors showed that longer exclusive breast-feeding is associated with more education (P=0·022) and non-smoking (P=0·013), and that longer total breast-feeding (exclusive+partial) is associated with non-smoking (P=0·004), which explains 22 and 19% of the variance respectively. Mothers with the least education (<11 years; n 25) breast-fed exclusively for an average of 2·2 (sd 1·6) months, while mothers with a medium level of education (13–14 years; n 27) breast-fed for 3·4 (sd 1·7) months and mothers with a high level of education (>15 years; n 44) for 3·4 (sd 1·6) months (the least-educated group was significantly different from the other two groups; P=0·023 and P=0·014 respectively). The duration of total breast-feeding was shorter among mothers who smoked (3·6 (sd 3·6 months); n 8) than among mothers who did not smoke (7·6 (sd 3·1) months; n 80; P=0·003), and also among mothers who smoked but not during pregnancy (9·0 (sd 3·0) months; n 8; P=0·003).

When examining the intake of certain dietary factors at 9 and 12 months of age, linear multiple regression analysis showed that smoking and the age of the mother were significantly associated with added sugar intake when controlling for all the other socio-demographic factors. The intake of added sugar among children of mothers with the least education (15 (sd 9) g; n 20) was higher than that among children of mothers in the two higher education classes combined (11 (sd 6) g; n 67; P=0·018). Also, a difference was seen in the intake of added sugar according to the mothers' smoking habits (P<0·001); children of mothers who smoked (n 5) consumed more added sugar (23 (sd 8) g) than children of mothers who smoked, but not during pregnancy (6 (sd 6) g; n 7; P<0·001) and non-smoking mothers (12 (sd 6) g; n 75; P=0·001).

Multiple regression analyses showed that when adjusted for all the other socio-demographic factors the education of the mother was associated with fruit intake and mothers' smoking habits were associated with vegetable intake. Socio-demographic factors explained 11% of the variance for fruit intake and 13% of the variance for vegetable intake. The intake of fruit was lower among the children of mothers with the least education (65 (sd 50) g; n 20) than among children of mothers with a high level of education (104 (sd 57) g; n 43; P=0·021). The fruit consumption of children whose mothers had a medium level of education (91 (sd 49) g; n 24) was not different from that for the other groups. The vegetable intake of children of mothers who smoked but not during pregnancy (n 7) was lower (8 (sd 7) g) than that of children whose mothers did not smoke (46 (sd 32) g; n 75; P=0·008). Children of mothers who smoked (35 (sd 42) g; n 5) were not significantly different from the other two groups in their intake of vegetables.

Discussion

In a generally well-nourished population with high birth weight and a high frequency of breast-feeding, but in which cow's milk has been commonly used between 6 and 12 months of age, 20% of 12-month-old children and 9% of 2-year-old children were found to be Fe-deficient. The Fe status improved from 12 months of age to 6 years of age. The percentage of children with depleted Fe stores at 1, 2 and 6 years of age were 41, 28 and 16 respectively. Three children at the age of 1 year (2·7%), one child at 2 years and none at 6 years had Fe-deficiency anaemia.

In comparison with other studies, Fe status seems to be lower in Icelandic 1-year-olds than in children from neighbouring countries. In a Swedish study 26% of 12-month-old infants were defined as having depleted Fe stores (SF <12 μg/l; Persson et al. Reference Persson, Lundstrom, Lonnerdal and Hernell1998). In Danish 9-month-old infants the average Hb and MCV values reported are similar to those found in the present study, but none of the Danish infants was observed to have Fe-deficiency anaemia, and mean SF levels (37 μg/l; Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995) are higher than those for the Icelandic 1-year-olds (17 μg/l). The Icelandic 2-year-olds had a slightly lower Fe status than that observed in other studies of children in that age-group from other Nordic countries (Fagerli et al. Reference Fagerli, Wandel, Olsen, Ek, Thorstensen, Brekke and Borch-Iohnsen1996; Bramhagen & Axelsson, Reference Bramhagen and Axelsson1999), but a similar Fe status to that found in the UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey of children aged 1·5–2·5 years (Thane et al. Reference Thane, Walmsley, Bates, Prentice and Cole2000). On the other hand, for 2-year-old Irish children (Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Mulder, van't Hof, Hoey and Gibney1998) the values reported for Hb and erythrocyte distribution width are similar to those in the present study, although their mean SF values are lower, and about 50% were found to have an SF of <10 μg/l. Although Fe status has been shown to be much better in the 6-year-olds than in the younger children (Gunnarsson et al. Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2005), Fe status indices show tracking, mainly in MCV but also in SF and Hb, demonstrating for the first time tracking of Fe status from 1 to 6 years of age. Tracking has been observed in SF between 6 and 9 months in a study of Danish infants (Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995), and in a cohort of Irish children earlier Fe status was found to be a predictor for Fe status at 36 months of age (Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Mulder, van't Hof, Hoey and Gibney1998).

Dietary factors

Total breast-feeding duration was positively associated with Fe status. One possible explanation for this finding is that children who are breast-fed longer grow slower (Atladottir & Thorsdottir, Reference Atladottir and Thorsdottir2000), and slower growth induces better Fe status at the age of 12 months (Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995; Thorsdottir et al. Reference Thorsdottir, Gunnarsson, Atladóttir, Michaelsen and Palsson2003). Additionally, breast-fed infants receive less cow's milk. The positive effects of breast-feeding on Fe status in the present study do not reflect the results of several other studies (Pizarro et al. Reference Pizarro, Yip, Dallman, Olivares, Hertrampf and Walter1991; Oti-Boateng et al. Reference Oti-Boateng, Seshadri, Petrick, Gibson and Simmer1998). However, the negative effect on Fe status in the study of Pizarro et al. (Reference Pizarro, Yip, Dallman, Olivares, Hertrampf and Walter1991) was observed when breast milk was the only milk provided for ≥9 months; in the present study the median duration of total breast-feeding was 8 months and that of exclusive breast-feeding was shorter.

Cow's milk consumption was the dietary factor most strongly associated (negatively) with Fe status indices in both the infant study and the study of 2-year-olds. A negative association between Fe status and cow's milk consumption has also been reported in many other studies of infants and young children (Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995; Gill et al. Reference Gill, Vincent and Segal1997; Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Mulder, van't Hof, Hoey and Gibney1998; Lawson et al. Reference Lawson, Thomas and Hardiman1998; Bramhagen & Axelsson, Reference Bramhagen and Axelsson1999; Thane et al. Reference Thane, Walmsley, Bates, Prentice and Cole2000). This strong association has led to the widespread recommendation that cow's milk should be avoided as the infant's main drink until the age of 12 months; many of the Fe-containing follow-on formulas are recommended by the manufacturer for use until the age of 2 years. The results have also led to a change in the Icelandic recommendation, and the effect of this change is now being investigated in about 300 infants. That study will be completed in 2007.

In both the infant study and the study of 2-year-olds the effect of cow's milk on Fe status was observed in those consuming >500 g/d. Ca has been shown to inhibit Fe absorption in single-meal studies (Cook et al. Reference Cook, Dassenko and Whittaker1991; Hallberg et al. Reference Hallberg, Brune, Erlandsson, Sandberg and Rossander-Hulten1991), but whether the effect of Ca on Fe absorption persists in a complete diet has been questioned (Reddy & Cook, Reference Reddy and Cook1997). In the present study Fe status indices were not found to be as strongly associated with Ca intake as with cow's milk, which might indicate that some other factors in cow's milk play a contributing role in affecting Fe status. High consumption of cow's milk might displace foods from other food groups in the diet, which has been indicated with cow's milk consumption >400 g/d in the UK National Diet and Nutrition Survey (Thane et al. Reference Thane, Walmsley, Bates, Prentice and Cole2000). Gastrointestinal bleeding associated with early cow's milk intake, probably associated with cow's-milk proteins, has been mentioned as a possible cause of Fe deficiency in infants (Ziegler et al. Reference Ziegler, Fomon, Nelson, Rebouche, Edwards, Rogers and Lehman1990). Tunnessen & Oski (Reference Tunnessen and Oski1987) have concluded that the main factor responsible for Fe inadequacy in infants receiving cow's milk from 6 months of age is the low content of Fe rather than gastrointestinal bleeding in infants caused by cow's milk proteins. Also, gastrointestinal bleeding diminishes in late infancy and is probably negligible at 12 months (Ziegler et al. Reference Ziegler, Jiang, Romero, Vinco, Frantz and Nelson1999). Interestingly, no effect of cow's milk consumption on Fe status was seen at 6 years of age. Indeed, the percentage of the energy consumed in the form of cow's milk and cow's milk products was much less than that at 1 and 2 years of age (19 v. 36 and 27 at 1 and 2 years respectively), which might partly explain why the association with cow's milk at 6 years of age is not stronger.

Consumption of meat and fish was found to be positively associated with Fe status indices at 12 months and 6 years old, but not at 2 years old. Comparable results have been observed both in young (Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995; Engelmann et al. Reference Engelmann, Davidsson, Sandstrom, Walczyk, Hurrell and Michaelsen1998a; Thane et al. Reference Thane, Walmsley, Bates, Prentice and Cole2000) and older children (Thane et al. Reference Thane, Bates and Prentice2003). These foods are relatively rich in Fe and contain considerable amounts of the better-absorbed haem-Fe together with a ‘meat-factor’, probably cysteine-containing peptides, that enhances the absorption of non-haem-Fe (Engelmann et al. Reference Engelmann, Sandstrom and Michaelsen1998b). However, Fe intake was not related to Fe status at 1, 2 or 6 years of age in the present study, which is similar to observations in some other studies (Duggan et al. Reference Duggan, Steel, Elwys, Harbottle and Noble1991; Thane et al. Reference Thane, Walmsley, Bates, Prentice and Cole2000). Several reasons have been proposed for this lack of association, e.g. Fe absorption increases in individuals with low Fe stores and Fe bioavailability depends both on the form of Fe in foods and the presence of dietary enhancers or inhibitors of absorption in meals (Gibson, Reference Gibson1999). Juices were found to be positively associated with Hb at 6 years of age, but negatively associated with Hb and MCV at 12 months. The reason for the discrepancy observed might be the vitamin C content of the juices, as vitamin C was also associated positively with Hb. Vitamin C is generally acknowledged as being a promoter of Fe absorption (Hallberg et al. Reference Hallberg, Brune and Rossander-Hulthen1987; Fairweather-Tait et al. Reference Fairweather-Tait, Fox, Wharf and Eagles1995), and in the present studies was positively related to Fe status both in infancy and at 6 years of age. The explanation for this difference is the higher consumption of citrus juices containing more vitamin C at 6 years of age than at 12 months, which was indicated by a high correlation between juice and vitamin C at 6 years of age but not at 1 year.

Fe-fortified breakfast cereals were positively associated with SF, an effect that has not been shown in comparably young children elsewhere, although Fe-fortified infant cereals have been shown to improve Fe status (Walter, Reference Walter1993). The negative effect of bread consumption on Fe status seen in the infant study has been reported elsewhere (Michaelsen et al. Reference Michaelsen, Milman and Samuelson1995). The association of biscuits and crackers with Fe status in 1- and 2-year-olds cannot easily be explained, since not only do the biscuits and crackers most popular with children in Iceland contain relatively small amounts of Fe, but they also contain more known inhibitors of Fe absorption than enhancers.

In 6-year-olds the intake of multivitamin–mineral supplements containing Fe was positively associated with SF, but multivitamin mixtures without Fe, usually containing only B-vitamins and vitamin C, were not significantly associated with Fe status indices. Relatively few children were consuming vitamin C supplements, and their Fe status indices were not significantly different from those of children not consuming supplemental vitamin C. Also, only six children took Fe supplements, and some of these children did so in conjunction with multivitamin–minerals, so it was difficult to establish whether Fe supplements as such had any impact, but there appeared to be a small tendency to a positive effect. Several studies have found a relationship between multivitamin–mineral supplement intake and Fe status in children in industrialized countries. Lawson et al. (Reference Lawson, Thomas and Hardiman1998) have found better Fe status in 2-year-old Indian children living in England who consume vitamin supplements, and Australian preschool children receiving vitamin supplements tend to be less likely to be Fe-depleted (Karr et al. Reference Karr, Alperstein, Causer, Mira, Lammi and Fett1996). Also, the US National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey II data indicate that vitamin–mineral supplement users in the age-groups 3–4 years and 5–10 years have higher mean Hb and SF levels respectively (Looker et al. Reference Looker, Sempos, Johnson and Yetley1987). Multivitamin supplements including minerals, or Fe-fortified cereals, may be of importance for those children at risk for Fe deficiency, but it does not seem to be of importance for healthy children to take supplements to prevent Fe deficiency. Varying the diet to include fish or meat as well as cow's milk, and including foods containing vitamin C may be more important.

Early iron deficiency and development

The possible long-lasting negative effect of Fe deficiency, especially when severe and accompanied by anaemia, has been widely documented (Palti et al. Reference Palti, Pevsner and Adler1983; Dommergues et al. Reference Dommergues, Archambeaud, Ducot, Gerval, Hiard, Rossignol and Tchernia1989; Lozoff et al. Reference Lozoff, Jimenez and Wolf1991, Reference Lozoff, Jimenez, Hagen, Mollen and Wolf2000; Hurtado et al. Reference Hurtado, Claussen and Scott1999). Only one study has shown complete reversal of test score differences after treatment in infancy (Idjradinata & Pollitt, Reference Idjradinata and Pollitt1993), but these results do not necessarily exclude possible differences in intellectual function in later life (Gordon, Reference Gordon2003). It is still uncertain whether the long-lasting developmental deficit in children with Fe-deficiency anaemia in infancy and early childhood is a result of Fe deficiency or possibly some confounding factors. Most studies relating to this area have been conducted in populations that are relatively deprived socially and economically, where there are many confounding environmental factors (for review, see Grantham-McGregor & Ani, Reference Grantham-McGregor and Ani2001). The present study was conducted in an environment in which many of these confounding factors were absent. Data from cohort 1, the infant study, did not identify differences in Fe status indices at the age of 1 year according to socio-demographic variables (Gunnarsson et al. Reference Gunnarsson, Thorsdottir and Palsson2006b). The current findings are different from those of most other studies, which in general have reported anaemia to be associated with developmental deficit. One of the few exceptions is the study by Oski et al. (Reference Oski, Honig, Helu and Howanitz1983), which found Fe-deficient children (defined as Hb >110 g/l; SF <12 μg/l; erythrocyte protoporphyrin >0·3 mg/l) to have lower developmental scores than non-Fe-deficient children, but the scores were corrected after 1 week of treatment. Similar findings have been reported in a short-term study by Walter et al. (Reference Walter, Kowalskys and Stekel1983).

The present results show that early Fe deficiency is mainly associated with less development in motor ability. Long-lasting motor differences have been related to delayed myelination, although the mechanisms behind this association are not understood (Lozoff et al. Reference Lozoff, Jimenez, Hagen, Mollen and Wolf2000). Lozoff et al. (Reference Lozoff, Brittenham, Wolf, McClish, Kuhnert, Jimenez, Jimenez, Mora, Gomez and Krauskoph1987) have also reported that a decline in motor development occurs in infants at a higher cut-off value for Hb (<105 g/l) than a decline in mental development (<100 g/l), possibly suggesting that motor development can be impaired with less-severe Fe deficiency than mental development.

Infant diet and socio-demographic status

The present findings from the infant study show that in mothers with similar healthcare support throughout pregnancy and infancy and with infants who are healthy at birth adherence to recommendations relating to breast-feeding, as well as other infant feeding recommendations, is associated with mothers' education and smoking, with the age of mother also playing a part (Gudnadóttir et al. Reference Gudnadóttir, Gunnarsson and Thorsdottir2006). Similarly, the education of the mother also appears to influence the intake of added sugar, as has been observed elsewhere (van den Boom et al. Reference van den Boom, Kimber and Morgan1995), and is the only significant factor associated with the fruit intake of 9–12-month-old infants. Similar associations with socio-demographic factors have been observed in other Nordic countries (Hornell et al. Reference Hornell, Aarts, Kylberg, Hofvander and Gebre-Medhin1999; Lande et al. Reference Lande, Andersen, Baerug, Trygg, Lund-Larsen, Veierod and Bjorneboe2003).

These findings indicate the need for altered support to increase adherence to new recommendations in the case of mothers with the least education (secondary school or less), mothers who smoke throughout pregnancy and lactation, and mothers who are younger than 27 years. The same support may be needed for parents with the lowest income, as they appeared to show a similar tendency to low compliance with recommendations. It is important to offer these groups support and education to ensure better nutritional status of their children.

Dietary quality and adequate micronutrient intake

The present studies show that a child's micronutrient status is affected by diet, even the diet in the years before the measurements, as well as growth. This outcome underlines the importance of diet and growth in infancy for adequate nutrition. There are many micronutrients that need to be studied, and the recent focus on vitamin D will probably further increase the interest in n-3 fatty acids and marine food. A varied diet is of major importance for preschool and older children to ensure adequacy of micronutrients, in addition to breast-feeding in the first months; exclusive breast-feeding is recommended up to 6 months of age and further breast-feeding as part of a healthy diet as long as it suits the mother and the child (Alexander et al. Reference Alexander, Anderssen, Aro, Becker, Fogelholm, Lyhne, Meltzer, Pedersen and Thorsdottir2004). In relation to the Fe status of Icelandic children, it is important to monitor the effects of the altered recommendations. Furthermore, it is of high importance for the micronutrient status of the children to investigate how a high fish intake can be re-introduced into the Icelandic diet to ensure the previous high intake of vitamin D, marine fatty acids and I. Recent studies in Europe, the ProChildren Project, have also shown that Icelandic 11-year-olds have the lowest fruit and vegetable intake among this age-group in Europe (Klepp et al. Reference Klepp, Perez Rodrigo, Thorsdottir, Due, Vaz de Almeida and Elmadfa2005; Yngve et al. Reference Yngve, Wolf, Poortvliet, Elmadfa, Brug and Ehrenblad2005). These studies also suggest approaches, through increased availability, self-efficacy and other social factors, that can be used to encourage increased intake of fruits and vegetables (Sandvik et al. Reference Sandvik, De Bourdeaudhuij, Due, Brug, Wind and Bere2005).

References

Alexander, J, Anderssen, SA, Aro, A, Becker, W, Fogelholm, M, Lyhne, N, Meltzer, HM, Pedersen, J & Thorsdottir, I (2004) Nordic Nutrition Recommendations 2004. Integrating Nutrition and Physical Activity, 4th ed. Copenhagen, Denmark: Nordic Council of Ministers.Google Scholar
Atladottir, H & Thorsdottir, I (2000) Energy intake and growth of infants in Iceland – a population with high frequency of breast-feeding and high birth weight. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 54, 695701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bothwell, TH, Baynes, RD, MacFarlane, BJ & MacPhail, AP (1989) Nutritional requirements and food iron absorption. Journal of Internal Medicine 226, 357365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramhagen, AC & Axelsson, I (1999) Iron status of children in southern Sweden: effects of cow's milk and follow-on formula. Acta Paediatrica 88, 13331337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brune, M, Rossander-Hultén, L, Hallberg, L, Gleerup, A & Sandberg, A-S (1992) Iron absorption from bread in humans: inhibiting effects of cereal fiber, phytate and inositol phosphates with different numbers of phosphate groups. Journal of Nutrition 122, 442449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, JD, Dassenko, SA & Whittaker, P (1991) Calcium supplementation: effect on iron absorption. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 53, 106111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dallmann, PR, Siimes, MA & Stekel, A (1980) Iron deficiency in infancy and childhood. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 33, 86118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Department of Health (1994) Weaning and the Weaning Diet. Report on Health and Social Subjects no 45, London: H. M. Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Domellof, M & Hernell, O (2002) Iron-deficiency anaemia during the first two years of life. Scandinavian Journal of Nutrition 46, 2030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dommergues, JP, Archambeaud, MP, Ducot, B, Gerval, Y, Hiard, C, Rossignol, C & Tchernia, G (1989) Carence en fer et tests de developpement psychomoteur. Etude longitudinale entre l'age de 10 mois et l'age de 4 ans (Iron deficiency and psychomotor development tests. Longitudinal study between 10 months and 4 years of age). Archives de Pédiatrie 46, 487490.Google ScholarPubMed
Duggan, MB, Steel, G, Elwys, G, Harbottle, L & Noble, C (1991) Iron status, energy intake, and nutritional status of healthy young Asian children. Archives of Disease in Childhood 66, 13861389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelmann, MD, Davidsson, L, Sandstrom, B, Walczyk, T, Hurrell, RF & Michaelsen, KF (1998a) The influence of meat on nonheme iron absorption in infants. Pediatric Research 43, 768773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelmann, MD, Sandstrom, B & Michaelsen, KF (1998b) Meat intake and iron status in late infancy: an intervention study. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition 26, 2633.Google ScholarPubMed
Expert Scientific Working Group (1985) Summary of a report on assessment of the iron nutritional status of the United States population. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 42, 13181330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagerli, RA, Wandel, M, Olsen, PT, Ek, J, Thorstensen, K, Brekke, O-L & Borch-Iohnsen, B (1996) Iron status and diet at two years of age: a longitudinal study of healthy Norwegian immigrant children. Scandinavian Journal of Nutrition 40, 5863.Google Scholar
Fairweather-Tait, SJ, Fox, T, Wharf, SG & Eagles, J (1995) The bioavailability of iron in different weaning foods and the enhancing effect of a fruit drink containing ascorbic acid. Pediatric Research 37, 389394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, VE, Mulder, J, van't Hof, MA, Hoey, HM & Gibney, MJ (1998) A longitudinal study of iron status in children at 12, 24 and 36 months. Public Health Nutrition 1, 93100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, GJ, Farris, RP, DeWier, M, Hutchinson, SW, Warrier, R, Doucet, H & Suskind, RM (1993) Iron status and intake of older infants fed formula vs cow milk with cereal. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 58, 343348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, SA (1999) Iron intake and iron status of preschool children: associations with breakfast cereals, vitamin C and meat. Public Health Nutrition 2, 521528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gill, DG, Vincent, S & Segal, DS (1997) Follow-on formula in the prevention of iron deficiency: a multicentre study. Acta Paediatrica 86, 683689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, N (2003) Iron deficiency and the intellect. Brain & Development 25, 38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grantham-McGregor, S & Ani, C (2001) A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. Journal of Nutrition 131, 649S668S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gudmundsson, E & Gretarsson, SJ (1993) The reliability and validity of the Icelandic parent. Development inventory for children from 36 months to 72 months. Educational and Psychological Measurement 53, 571584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gudmundsson, E & Gretarsson, SJ (1994) Adults' estimates of pre-school children's verbal and motor abilities compared with those children's scores on an individual developmental test. Early Development and Parenting 3, 161170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gudnadóttir, M, Gunnarsson, BS & Thorsdottir, I (2006) Effects of sociodemographic factors on adherence to breastfeeding and other important infant dietary recommendations. Acta Paediatrica 95, 419424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnarsson, BS, Thorsdottir, I & Palsson, G (2004) Iron status in 2-year-old Icelandic children and associations with dietary intake and growth. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 58, 901906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnarsson, BS, Thorsdottir, I & Palsson, G (2005) Iron status in 6-year-old children: associations with growth and earlier iron status. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 59, 761767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnarsson, BS, Thorsdottir, I & Palsson, G (2006a) Associations of iron status with dietary and other factors in 6-year-old children. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition (In the Press).Google Scholar
Gunnarsson, BS, Thorsdottir, I, Palsson, G & Gretarsson, SJ (2006b) Iron status at 1 and 6 years vs. developmental scores at 6 years in a well-nourished affluent population. Acta Paediatrica (In the Press).Google Scholar
Hallberg, L, Brune, M, Erlandsson, M, Sandberg, AS & Rossander-Hulten, L (1991) Calcium: effect of different amounts on nonheme- and heme-iron absorption in humans. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 53, 112119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallberg, L, Brune, M & Rossander-Hulthen, L (1987) Is there a physiological role of vitamin C in iron absorption? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 498, 324332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hercberg, S, Papoz, L, Galan, P, Guery, MF, Farnier, MA & Rossignol, C (1987) Iron status and dietary pattern in young children. Nutritional Reports International 35, 307315.Google Scholar
Hornell, A, Aarts, C, Kylberg, E, Hofvander, Y & Gebre-Medhin, M (1999) Breastfeeding patterns in exclusively breastfed infants: A longitudinal prospective study in Uppsala, Sweden. Acta Paediatrica 88, 203211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurtado, EK, Claussen, AH & Scott, KG (1999) Early childhood anaemia and mild or moderate mental retardation. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 69 115119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Idjradinata, P & Pollitt, E (1993) Reversal of developmental delays in iron-deficient anaemic infants treated with iron. Lancet 341, 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
International Nutritional Anemia Consultative Group/World Health Organization/UNICEF (1998) Guidelines for the Use of Iron Supplements to Prevent and Treat Iron Deficiency Anemia. Washington, DC: ILSI.Google Scholar
Karr, M, Alperstein, G, Causer, J, Mira, M, Lammi, A & Fett, MJ (1996) Iron status and anaemia in preschool children in Sydney. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health 20, 618622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klepp, KI, Perez Rodrigo, C, Thorsdottir, I, Due, P, Vaz de Almeida, MD, Elmadfa, Iet al. (2005) Promoting and sustaining health through increased vegetable and fruit consumption among European schoolchildren: The Pro Children Project. Journal of Public Health 13, 97101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lande, B, Andersen, LF, Baerug, A, Trygg, KU, Lund-Larsen, K, Veierod, MB & Bjorneboe, GE (2003) Infant feeding practices and associated factors in the first six months of life: the Norwegian infant nutrition survey. Acta Paediatrica 92, 152161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawson, MS, Thomas, M & Hardiman, A (1998) Iron status of Asian children aged 2 years living in England. Archives of Disease in Childhood 78, 420426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lind, T, Lönnerdal, B, Persson, L-Å, Stenlund, H, Tennefors, C & Hernell, O (2003) Effect of weaning cereals with different phytate content on haemoglobin, iron stores, and serum zinc – a randomized intervention from 6–12 months of age. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 78, 168175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonnerdal, B & Hernell, O (1994) Iron, zinc, copper, and selenium status of breastfed infants and infants fed trace element fortified milk-based infant formula. Acta Paediatrica 83, 367373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Looker, AC, Sempos, CT, Johnson, CL & Yetley, EA (1987) Comparison of dietary intakes and iron status of vitamin-mineral supplement users and nonusers, aged 1–19 years. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 46, 665672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lozoff, B, Brittenham, GM, Wolf, AW, McClish, DK, Kuhnert, PM, Jimenez, E, Jimenez, R, Mora, LA, Gomez, I & Krauskoph, D (1987) Iron deficiency anemia and iron therapy effects on infant developmental test performance. Pediatrics 79, 981995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lozoff, B, Jimenez, E, Hagen, J, Mollen, E & Wolf, AW (2000) Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics 105, E51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lozoff, B, Jimenez, E & Wolf, AW (1991) Long-term developmental outcome of infants with. iron deficiency. New England Journal of Medicine 325, 687694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lynch, SR, Skikne, BS & Cook, JD (1989) Food iron absorption in idiopathic hemochromatosis. Blood 74, 21872193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michaelsen, KF, Milman, N & Samuelson, G (1995) A longitudinal study of iron status in healthy Danish infants: effects of early iron status, growth velocity and dietary factors. Acta Paediatrica 84, 10351044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mills, AF (1990) Surveillance for anaemia: risk factors in patterns of milk intake. Archives of Disease in Children 65, 428431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oski, FA, Honig, AS, Helu, B & Howanitz, P (1983) Effect of iron therapy on behavior performance in nonanaemic, iron-deficient infants. Pediatrics 71, 877880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oti-Boateng, P, Seshadri, R, Petrick, S, Gibson, RA & Simmer, K (1998) Iron status and dietary iron intake of 6–24 month-old children in Adelaide. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health 34, 250253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palsson, G, Sigurdsson, JA & Gudbjornsdottir, H (1996) Health Care of Infants. Reykjavik, Iceland: Oddi Ltd.Google Scholar
Palti, H, Pevsner, B & Adler, B (1983) Does anaemia in infancy affect achievement on developmental and intelligence tests? Human Biology 55, 183194.Google ScholarPubMed
Persson, LA, Lundstrom, M, Lonnerdal, B & Hernell, O (1998) Are weaning foods causing impaired iron and zinc status in 1-year-old Swedish infants? A cohort study. Acta Paediatrica 87, 618622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizarro, F, Yip, R, Dallman, PR, Olivares, M, Hertrampf, E & Walter, T (1991) Iron status with different infant feeding regimens: relevance to screening and prevention of iron deficiency. Journal of Pediatrics 118, 687692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reddy, MB & Cook, JD (1997) Effect of calcium intake on nonheme-iron absorption from a complete diet. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 65, 18201825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandvik, C, De Bourdeaudhuij, I, Due, P, Brug, J, Wind, M, Bere, Eet al. (2005) Personal, social and environmental factors regarding fruit and vegetable intake among schoolchildren in nine European countries. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism 49, 255266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherriff, A, Emond, A, Hawkins, N & Golding, J (1999) Haemoglobin and ferritin concentrations in children aged 12 and 18 months. ALSPAC Children in Focus Study Team. Archives of Disease in Childhood 80, 153157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siimes, MA, Salmenpera, L & Perheentupa, J (1984) Exclusive breast-feeding for 9 months: risk of iron deficiency. Journal of Pediatrics 104, 196199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steingrimsdottir, L (1995) Infant Diet. Reykjavik, Iceland: Isafold Ltd.Google Scholar
Thane, CW, Bates, CJ & Prentice, A (2003) Risk factors for low iron intake and poor iron status in a national sample of British young people aged 4–18 years. Public Health Nutrition 6, 485496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thane, CW, Walmsley, CM, Bates, CJ, Prentice, A & Cole, TJ (2000) Risk factors for poor iron status in British toddlers: further analysis of data from the National Diet and Nutrition Survey of children aged 1·5–4·5 years. Public Health Nutrition 3, 433440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorsdottir, I & Birgisdottir, BE (1998) Different weight gain in women of normal weight before pregnancy: postpartum weight and birth weight. Obstetrics and Gynecology 92, 377383.Google ScholarPubMed
Thorsdottir, I, Gunnarsdottir, I, Kvaran, MA & Gretarsson, SJ (2005) Maternal body mass index, duration of exclusive breastfeeding and children's developmental status at the age of 6 years. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 59, 426431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorsdottir, I, Gunnarsson, BS, Atladóttir, H, Michaelsen, KF & Palsson, G (2003) Iron status at 12 months of age – effects of body size, growth and diet in a population with high birth weight. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 57, 505513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunnessen, WW Jr & Oski, FA (1987) Consequences of starting whole cow milk at 6 months of age. Journal of Pediatrics 111, 813816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Boom, SA, Kimber, AC & Morgan, JB (1995) Weaning practices in children up to 19 months of age in Madrid. Acta Paediatrica 84, 853858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walter, T (1993) Impact of iron deficiency on cognition in infancy and childhood. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 47, 307316.Google ScholarPubMed
Walter, T, Kowalskys, J & Stekel, A (1983) Effect of mild iron deficiency on infant mental developmental scores. Journal of Pediatrics 102, 519522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wharf, SG, Fox, TE, Fairweather-Tait, SJ & Cook, JD (1997) Factors affecting iron stores in infants 4–18 months of age. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 51, 504509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2001) Iron Deficiency Anaemia. Assessment, Prevention and Control. A Guide for Programme Managers. WHO/NHD/01.3. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Yngve, A, Wolf, A, Poortvliet, E, Elmadfa, I, Brug, J, Ehrenblad, Bet al. (2005) Fruit and vegetable intake in a sample of 11-year-old children in 9 European countries: The Pro Children Cross-Sectional Survey. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism 49, 236245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziegler, EE, Fomon, SJ, Nelson, SE, Rebouche, CJ, Edwards, BB, Rogers, RR & Lehman, LJ (1990) Cow milk feeding in infancy: further observations on blood loss from the gastrointestinal tract. Journal of Pediatrics 116, 1118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziegler, EE, Jiang, T, Romero, E, Vinco, A, Frantz, JA & Nelson, SE (1999) Cow's milk and intestinal blood loss in late infancy. Journal of Pediatrics 135, 720726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the study design showing the data collected and used in the analysis for the two cohorts of Icelandic children: infant cohort (cohort 1); 2-year-old cohort (cohort 2).

Figure 1

Table 1. Iron status indices at the ages of 12 months, 2 years and 6 years for cohorts of Icelandic children*(Values are means and standard deviations)

Figure 2

Table 2. Frequency (%) of depleted iron stores, iron deficiency and iron-deficiency anaemia for 1-, 2- and 6-year-old Icelandic children participating in the study*

Figure 3

Table 3. Mean iron status indices at 1 and 2 years of age in Icelandic children consuming either >500 g cow's milk/d or <500 g cow's milk/d at 9–12 months and 2 years respectively*

Figure 4

Table 4. Multiple regression analyses of food factors influencing iron status indices of cohorts of Icelandic children at 1 year (consumed at 12 months and the average for 9 and 12 months), 2 years and 6 years*

Figure 5

Fig. 2. Fine motor scores at 6 years of age in a cohort of Icelandic children who at the age of 1 year were iron-deficient (ID) or non-iron-deficient (NID), or who had depleted iron stores (DIS) or non-depleted iron stores (N-DIS). For details of the study population, the evaluation of motor development and the study design, see p. 367, p. 368 and Fig. 1 respectively. Values are means and standard deviations represented by vertical bars. Mean value was significantly different from that for the ID group: ** P=0·011. Mean value was significantly different from that for the DIS group: † P=0·045.