Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T15:18:01.115Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is there a role for vitamin D in supporting cognitive function as we age?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2017

Niamh Aspell*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Medicine, School of Medicine, Trinity Centre for Health Sciences, St. James's Hospital Campus, Dublin, Ireland
Brian Lawlor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Mercer's Institute for Successful Ageing, St. James's Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
Maria O'Sullivan
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Medicine, School of Medicine, Trinity Centre for Health Sciences, St. James's Hospital Campus, Dublin, Ireland
*
*Corresponding author: N. Aspell, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Globally, an estimated 46 million people are currently living with dementia and this figure is projected to increase 3-fold by 2050, highlighting this major public health concern and its substantial associated healthcare costs. With pharmacological treatment yet to reach fruition, the emphasis on evidence-based preventative lifestyle strategies is becoming increasingly important and several modifiable lifestyle factors have been identified that may preserve cognitive health. These include good cardiovascular health, physical activity, low alcohol intake, smoking and a healthy diet, with growing interest in vitamin D. The aim of the present paper is to review the evidence supporting the potential roles of vitamin D in ageing and cognitive health in community-dwelling older adults. Furthermore, to describe the utility and challenges of cognitive assessments and outcomes when investigating vitamin D in this context. Evidence indicates that serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) may impact brain health. There is a biological plausibility from animal models that vitamin D may influence neurodegenerative disorders, through several mechanisms. Epidemiological evidence supports associations between low serum 25(OH)D concentrations and poorer cognitive performance in community-dwelling older populations, although an optimal 25(OH)D level for cognitive health could not be determined. The effect of raising 25(OH)D concentrations on cognitive function remains unclear, as there is a paucity of interventional evidence. At a minimum, it seems prudent to aim to prevent vitamin D deficiency in older adults, with other known common protective lifestyle factors, as a viable component of brain health strategies.

Type
Conference on ‘What governs what we eat?’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2017 

As we experience population ageing more of us can expect to reach and enjoy our old age. This demographic shift should be seen as a time of opportunity; however, as a result, we are also experiencing an increase in age-related diseases. The global incidence of dementia is increasing at a rate of one new case every 3 s, with associated medical, social and healthcare costs far exceeding the capacity of most countries(1, Reference Comas-Herrera, Wittenberg and Pickard2). Dementia is a syndrome, usually progressive and chronic in nature, in which there is deterioration in cognitive function beyond what might be expected from normal ageing(3). Due to the degenerative nature of the disease, sufferers lose their ability to perform routine tasks, experience poor quality of life and a loss of autonomy. In 2017, Public Health England reported dementia to be the leading cause of death in older adults in England, overtaking CVD, stroke and lung cancer for the first time. Similar trends have been expressed globally(4), which are largely driven by our increasing older adult population, longer life expectancies and improved diagnostic procedures(Reference Ferri, Prince and Brayne5, Reference Lutz, Sanderson and Scherbov6). In the absence of curative treatments, the focus on maintaining cognitive health, reducing the risk of developing dementia and delaying the onset is now a key priority for public health authorities and governments(Reference Dehnel7).

Dementia risk increases with advancing age, family history and genetic factors, for example, carriers of ApoE ε4 genotype. Dementia is of course, not an inevitable consequence of older age. Several modifiable lifestyle factors have been identified including CVD, diabetes, smoking and obesity(Reference Barnes and Yaffe8). Indeed modifiable factors may promote resilience in ApoE ε4 gene carriers(Reference Kaup, Nettiksimmons and Harris9, Reference Schöttker, Jorde and Peasey10). A role for vitamin D in the aetiology of cognitive impairment and dementia is plausible, supported by substantial mechanistic and epidemiological data, although intervention studies remain sparse. Vitamin D is a steroid hormone, which directly or indirectly regulates thousands of genes acting mainly via the vitamin D receptor (VDR)(Reference Holick11, Reference Nagpal, Na and Rathnachalam12). The VDR is expressed on multiple tissue sites, including the brain, cardiovascular and musculoskeletal systems where it acts on the expression of several gene target products as well as a range of non-genomic effects. An estimated one billion people globally are vitamin D insufficient which provides an opportunity for intervention(Reference Holick11, Reference Lips13). The aim of the present review is to discuss vitamin D in ageing and examine the evidence for the potential role of vitamin D deficiency and brain health and possible levels likely to support cognitive performance in community-dwelling older adults. Furthermore, we aim to consider the complexities of investigating the effects of vitamin D on cognitive outcomes in ageing.

Vitamin D physiology: a focus on ageing

Vitamin D metabolism

Vitamin D is a precursor of the active form 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D (calcitriol) and is present in two forms; vitamin D3 (cholecalciferol) and vitamin D2 (ergocalciferol). The main source in human subjects is via action of solar ultraviolet-B (UV-B) radiation (270–300 nm) on skin, converting 7-dehydrocholesterol (provitamin D3) into pre-vitamin D3, which is then rapidly converted to vitamin D3. Vitamin D2 is produced via UV-B radiation on plant sources such as mushrooms and yeast. Natural dietary sources of vitamin D3 are few and include fish liver oils, oily fish, egg yolks and some fortified foods (e.g. some dairy and breakfast cereals) and supplements. Irrespective of how it is acquired, vitamin D offers limited function until it has been activated, a process which requires two hydroxylation steps(Reference Bouillon, Carmeliet and Daci14, Reference DeLuca15). Firstly in the liver by a number of enzymes but primarily vitamin D325-hydroxylase (CYP2RA)(Reference Schuster16), which converts it to the inactive precursor, 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D), the prominent circulating form used to determine vitamin D status in human subjects. The second hydroxylation step occurs in the tubular cells of the kidney, by action of the enzyme 25vitamin D3-1α-hydroxylase (CYP27B1), to the biologically active metabolite, 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3. This active metabolite is an important modulator of calcium and phosphate homeostasis, importantly; however, both the activating enzyme CYP27B1 and the active form of vitamin D, 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3, are present in many non-renal tissues including the human brain, immune cells, cardiovascular systems and pancreatic islets. Due to the ability to be synthesised endogenously, vitamin D is widely accepted not as a classical vitamin but as a steroid hormone, furthermore, its synthesis in the human brain has led to it being commonly referred to as a neurosteroid(Reference Holick17).

Factors influencing vitamin D status in ageing

Several factors influence vitamin D status including skin pigmentation, use of sunscreen or concealing clothing, season, latitude, being older or institutionalised, obesity, malabsorption, renal and liver disease and medication use. In general, UV-B radiation on skin is the main source of vitamin D for human subjects. In countries north of the equator (40–60°N), vitamin D UV-B doses are inadequate for 6 months of the year (October–March)(Reference Webb, Kline and Holick18). Many other environmental factors implicate vitamin D UV-B dose availability including the ozone layer, cloud cover, air pollution, surface reflection and altitude(Reference Engelsen19). With advancing age skin integrity decreases; skin thinness and a reduction in transdermal cholesterol reduce the efficiency of UV-B vitamin D production, as much as 50 % in older adults compared with younger adults(Reference MacLaughlin and Holick20). Behavioural and social changes seen with advancing age further limit cutaneous vitamin D production. Less time spent outdoors due to ill health or limited mobility (institutionalised or homebound), medication use (loop diuretics, statins, glucocorticoids), changes in body composition (increase in fat and decrease in muscle), sun avoidance (melanoma risk), reduced skin exposure (clothing and colder temperatures) and sunscreen use are all significant barriers that contribute to inadequate UV-B vitamin D production in older adults(Reference de Jongh, van Schoor and Lips21Reference Wortsman, Matsuoka and Chen25).

The UK Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition recommends an intake of 10 µg/d (400 IU/d) vitamin D for older adults, a target hard to achieve by dietary contribution alone unless oily fish is eaten daily. Experts in the UK and Ireland recommend that during the winter months and for those at risk of deficiency, vitamin D supplementation should be considered(26, 27). Evidence suggests vitamin D supplement uptake is typically low in the population, including older adults(Reference Cashman, Muldowney and McNulty28). For the most part, dietary sources are limited and not generally consumed in adequate amounts by older adults(Reference Cashman, Muldowney and McNulty28, Reference Cashman, Hill and Lucey29), with an exception for those residing in countries where vitamin D food fortification is in place (United States, Canada, Sweden and Finland). The effects of age on intestinal absorption and decreased renal function may hinder vitamin D metabolism and availability(Reference Kinyamu, Gallagher and Balhorn30, Reference Kinyamu, Gallagher and Petranick31). It has been hypothesised that the metabolism of vitamin D may be reduced due to a decline of intestinal VDR distribution in older adults; however, few studies have been conducted in human subjects, with small samples and conflicting findings(Reference Ebeling, Sandgren and DiMagno32, Reference Kinyamu, Gallagher and Prahl33).

Vitamin D deficiency and ageing

Serum 25(OH)D is accepted as the most accurate measure of vitamin D status; however, there is less agreement on how to define deficiency in the general population or specifically to older adults. The Institute of Medicine advocate serum 25(OH)D concentrations ≥50 nmol/l (≥20 ng/ml) for bone health, and with levels <30 nmol/l (<12·5 ng/ml) considered to be deficient(Reference Ashwell, Stone and Stolte34). The term vitamin D insufficiency is used to describe serum levels of ≥30–50 nmol/l (≥12·5–20 ng/ml)(35). In contrast, the endocrine society recommends serum 25(OH)D levels >75 nmol/l to maximise an effect on bone and muscle metabolism. Suboptimal levels of vitamin D are much more common than clinical toxicity which is rare(Reference Vieth36). Reported vitamin D deficiency rates in published studies vary widely, with considerable methodological differences including the definition of serum 25(OH)D status, sample size, population, sex-specific cohorts, latitude, season and mode of vitamin D assay. Prevalence estimates for vitamin D deficiency in community-dwelling older adults across the European Union and the UK are summarised in (Table 1), along with the specific 25(OH)D cut-off criteria applied to define deficiency. As illustrated in (Table 1), deficiency ranged from 0 to 100 %. Overall, however, it is apparent that suboptimal vitamin D status is widespread in older adults, irrespective of the definition applied. For example, the Health Survey in England reported that 13 % of females and 8 % of males were vitamin D deficient, using the more conservative cut-off of <20 nmol/l(Reference Hirani and Primatesta37), at 5 years follow-up deficiency rates increased to 15 and 9·6 %, respectively(Reference Hirani, Tull and Ali38). When defining criteria is set at <50 nmol/l, 62·5 % of females and 50 % of males were classed as vitamin D deficient in this community-dwelling cohort. However, deficiency rates were almost double in cohorts defined as cognitively impaired(Reference McCarroll, Beirne and Casey39). In a recent study of Irish adults, a 10 % higher prevalence of vitamin D deficiency was reported in adults aged 65 years and older compared to middle-aged adults, 35·7 and 44·0 %, respectively(Reference Cashman, Muldowney and McNulty28).

Table 1. Prevalence of 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) deficiency in community-dwelling adults, aged over 50 years in Europe

D-def, serum 25(OH)D deficient; T, total; S, summer; W, winter; m, male; f, female.

* Deficiency defined <22·5 nmol/l.

Deficiency defined <37 nmol/l.

Vitamin D and cognitive function in ageing: considerations for assessment and outcome measures

The concept that cognitive function can be influenced by diet or specific nutrients has garnered interest in recent years. One of the most formidable methodological barriers evident across nutrition and cognition studies is the heterogeneity in cognitive assessments used, which will ultimately influence the outcome and the interpretation of the findings.

The term cognitive function refers to a variety of brain functions and processes which include receiving external information, processing this internally and responding with a behaviour. It can be seen as a hierarchy, going from overall (global) to domain-specific cognitive function. The most commonly used cognitive outcome measure in vitamin D studies are global tests, namely the Mini-Mental State Examination, and consequently, most associations were seen with a global measure(Reference Brouwer-Brolsma, Feskens and Steegenga40Reference Wilson, Houston and Kilpatrick44). Whilst this clinical screening test is widely used, it has limited use in research for detecting subtle changes in cognitive function in response to short-term interventions, such as vitamin D supplementation trials(Reference Spencer, Wendell and Giggey45). Domain-specific functions include memory, executive functioning, attention, perceptual functions, psychomotor abilities and language skills. Domain-specific measures are useful but reliance on a single domain assessment may lead to cognitive changes going undetected due to intra-individual variability(Reference Petersen46). Assessing cognitive function is complex and the importance of utilising multiple domains and assessing further sub-processes within each domain has been highlighted in recent years(Reference Fisk, Merry and Rockwood47).

Inter-individual performance in different domains vary and each individual has a unique profile of strengths and weaknesses in different domains(Reference Kanai and Rees48), so it is not explicit to define people on global function or domain-specific function alone. Performing well on one domain is often dependent on traits of another cognitive process or domains, for example, retaining new information in a memory test is highly dependent on attention(Reference Carriere, Cheyne and Smilek49). Studies which report having assessed participants’ cognitive function but use only a single domain outcome measure should be interpreted with caution unless the hypothesis predicts a specific effect on memory; even then including a mix of tests will better determine whether the predicted memory-specific function was indeed affected specifically.

Other factors related to cognitive assessments

Each domain is highly influenced by other mental factors, a psychological concept called central arousal which describes internal states such as mental fatigue and needs to be considered when assessing cognitive performance(Reference Eysenck, Derakshan and Santos50). Mood, sleep, anxiety and depression are highly correlated with cognitive performance and can significantly alter the outcome(Reference Conroy, Golden and Jeffares51, Reference Simpson, Maylor and McConville52).

The assessment of cognitive function is further complicated in defining the cognitive status of study participants, a distinct difference must be acknowledged when considering cognitive changes which are age-related and those which are a result of disease. Furthermore, inter-individual variability in the form and severity of cognitive decline is largely influenced by education and health-related factors. To accurately assess changes in cognitive status, these factors must also be considered, whilst acknowledging that older adults vary widely in the extent to which they are affected by these changes(Reference Kanai and Rees48). For example, to our knowledge, no studies of vitamin D status and cognitive performance considered assessment of pre-morbid IQ, which is highly correlated with most cognitive tests, since decline is essentially relative to pre-morbid ability(Reference Barker-Collo, Bartle and Clarke53), a score within the ‘normal’ range can, in fact, represent cognitive decline for an individual with high levels of pre-morbid functioning.

Types of outcome measures commonly used in vitamin D and cognition research

Neuropsychological performance tests are the methods most commonly used in vitamin D and cognition studies. Cognitive function can be assessed by a number of validated pen-and-paper or computer-based tests. Direct measures of brain location and effectiveness of many cognitive processes can be assessed using electroencephalograms, or assessment of brain activation associated with specific domains of cognitive function through functional MRI, often considered the gold standard, however, like all tests is subject to limitations. The relationship between neuronal activity and functional ability is not fully understood(Reference Poldrack and Wagner54) and their use in measuring nutritional effects should be interpreted carefully. Emerging mobile technologies which obtain real-time information regarding abilities to perform activities of daily living, which are highly correlated with neuropsychological tests, may change the way cognitive research is conducted, in way of preventative strategies for dementia(Reference Allard, Husky and Catheline55).

As the evidence for an underlying link between vitamin D and cognitive performance remains inconclusive, the best approach is to include a battery of cognitive tests that cover a variety of domains; this may help identify the specific cognitive processes involved. Several large-scale ageing studies incorporate a comprehensive battery of validated cognitive tests(Reference Hannigan, Coen and Lawlor56, Reference Kenny, Coen and Frewen57); their use in cognitive studies on vitamin D may be helpful. However, the logistics, time and practicalities of applying comprehensive cognitive batteries in vitamin D studies may be a barrier. Recently we reported preliminary findings that this was feasible and acceptable in adults aged 60 years and older, at three time points over a 6-month period(Reference Aspell, Lawlor and O'Suillivan58).

Epidemiological evidence; low vitamin D status and cognitive performance in healthy older adults

In the decade succeeding the discovery of the VDR and 1-α-hydroxylase in the human brain, a plethora of evidence for a relationship between serum 25(OH)D and cognitive function has been presented. Data from healthy older adults without known cognitive impairment provides comparable evidence in terms of evaluating the contribution of vitamin D and successful ageing; however, this information is less plentiful. To date, the majority of cross-sectional studies support an association between hypovitaminosis D and poorer cognitive performance. Yet many use only brief measures of cognitive function or adjust for few or limiting confounding factors. A recent cross-sectional study demonstrated that older adults with low 25(OH)D status (<30 nmol/l) performed significantly worse than those with levels >75 nmol/l, including greater processing speed and mental flexibility(Reference van Schoor, Comijs and Llewellyn43). This evidence was drawn from a well-designed large national study of ageing conducted in Amsterdam, which employed an in-depth cognitive assessment and gathered detailed demographic and lifestyle information. In contrast, the largest cross-sectional study conducted to date found no association between 25(OH)D status and cognitive function(Reference Tolppanen, Williams and Lawlor59), comprising 4831 participants of the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. The result may be attributed to the single memory outcome measure used to assess cognitive function. Three large population cohorts have since demonstrated a significant relationship between low 25(OH)D status (typically <30 nmol/l) and poorer cognitive performance, using comprehensive global and domain-specific outcomes, compared with adequate 25(OH)D levels (typically >75 nmol/l)(Reference van Schoor, Comijs and Llewellyn43, Reference Wilson, Houston and Kilpatrick44, Reference Bartali, Devore and Grodstein60). Other studies have demonstrated a relationship also, however, methodological issues were noted, with small sample sizes, limited analysis of confounding factors and single measures of cognitive performance(Reference Brouwer-Brolsma, Feskens and Steegenga40, Reference Brouwer-Brolsma, van de Rest and Tieland61Reference Seamans, Hill and Scully63). Cross-sectional studies are considered to provide weak evidence, due to the known issue of reverse causality, in that poorer cognitive performance and the onset of dementia may influence vitamin D concentrations through behavioural and dietary changes.

Longitudinal studies suggest that vitamin D deficiency is associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment and incidence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease (AD)(Reference Littlejohns, Henley and Lang64). One such study conducted with 10 186 older adults followed up after 30 years, revealed that those with 25(OH)D levels <25 nmol/l had a greater combined risk of developing AD than those with levels >50 nmol/l(Reference Afzal, Bojesen and Nordestgaard65). Another prospective study revealed that those with 25(OH)D concentrations at baseline, and at 3- and 6-year follow-up performed worse in global function and executive functioning but not in tasks of attention(Reference Llewellyn, Lang and Langa66). Whilst most studies demonstrate a link between serum 25(OH)D deficiency and poorer cognitive performance or incidence of dementia using a measure of global or domain-specific cognitive function(Reference Balion, Griffith and Strifler67), it is not clear if vitamin D deficiency is a risk factor for cognitive impairments or a result of poorer overall status and ill health(Reference van der Schaft, Koek and Dijkstra68).

Intervention evidence; the effects of vitamin D supplementation on cognitive performance

There are limited data from intervention studies. To date, three studies have been published investigating the effect of vitamin D supplementation on cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Overall one of three reported a significant positive effect on cognitive measures (Table 2). Recently, the first prospective intervention, using 50 µg/d (2000 IU) vitamin D3 reported no significant improvement in tasks of visual memory among eighty-two community-dwelling adults(Reference Pettersen69). Of note, was the heterogeneity between participants, as only 30 % of this small sample were aged over 60 years.

Table 2. Intervention studies; effect of vitamin D supplementation and cognitive performance outcomes in healthy older adults

B, baseline; PI, post-intervention; T, treatment; PL, placebo; SDMT, symbol digit modalities test; DS-F, digit span forward; DS-B, digit span backward; CANTAB, Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery; 25(OH)D, 25-hydroxyvitamin D; PRM, pattern recognition memory; PAL, paired associates learning; MMSE, mini-mental state examination; CAB, cognitive assessment battery; FAB, frontal assessment battery; WHISCA, women's health initiative study of cognitive aging; DSM-IV, diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; Sig., significant.

Annweiler et al. (Reference Annweiler, Herrmann and Fantino70), however, reported a positive finding in a small retrospective study (n 44), cognition was measured using a global measure and a behavioural assessment among patients attending an outpatient clinic, without cognitive impairment at baseline(Reference Annweiler, Herrmann and Fantino70). Incomplete 25(OH)D data were available for all participants, so determining optimal levels for cognitive performance are unobtainable from this investigation.

In the largest intervention study conducted to date, in community-dwelling females across the USA, no effect for vitamin D supplementation and incidence of dementia or cognitive performance was seen at follow-up (7·8 years)(Reference Rossom, Espeland and Manson71). Whilst an in-depth cognitive assessment was conducted, the intervention design was poor, the entire study population were open to consuming supplemental vitamin D of 15 µg/d (600 IU/d) during the study period, irrespective of treatment allocation, this left further ambiguity in terms of true treatment effects as serum 25(OH)D concentrations were not measured at the end of the study, providing no evidence for optimal serum 25(OH)D levels and cognitive function.

Preliminary work from our group shows no effect on global function and domain-specific tasks of executive function and attention, following 6-month vitamin D3 intervention of 50 µg/d (2000 IU/d) in healthy older adults; further work on domain-specific cognitive outcomes is ongoing(Reference Aspell, Lawlor and O'Suillivan58). Overall evidence from intervention studies is in its early stages, whilst published studies to data are inconclusive. This may be attributable to study design; nevertheless, these shortcomings have helped inform well-designed interventions(Reference Bischoff-Ferrari72), findings of which are likely to be available in the near future.

Vitamin D and Alzheimer's disease

Other interventions have been conducted in participants with mild–moderate AD and institutionalised older adults; however both showed no effect, were of short duration and add little in terms of preventable, risk factors for AD. One reported an improvement in attention and reaction times in 139 ambulatory subjects who were supplemented with vitamin D for 6 months(Reference Dhesi, Jackson and Bearne73). The other study was retrospective in design, non-blinded and involved only thirty-two participants presenting with subjective memory complaint, had a short duration of treatment of 28 d and used only one limited test of cognition(Reference Przybelski and Binkley74). Several studies report low levels of 25(OH)D in the community in patients with AD(Reference Buell, Dawson-Hughes and Scott75Reference Sato, Asoh and Oizumi77).

Vitamin D and cognitive decline: mechanisms behind the link

Most research in this field is primarily derived from animal studies and key findings are detailed later. For a full overview of the literature examining the link between vitamin D in ageing and cognitive health, see Eyles et al. and Groves et al. (Reference Eyles, Burne and McGrath78, Reference Groves, McGrath and Burne79). It is hypothesised that vitamin D exerts its effects via genomic and non-genomic pathways(Reference Fernandes de Abreu, Eyles and Feron80Reference Ramagopalan, Heger and Berlanga82). Exact mechanisms are unclear, but evidence suggests that it may protect against cognitive dysfunction through its effect on neuroprotection, neurotransmission, synaptic plasticity, immune modulation, neuronal calcium regulation and enhanced nerve conduction(Reference Brown, Bianco and McGrath83Reference Garcion, Wion-Barbot and Montero-Menei85), with secondary protective effects on vascular systems and modulation of vascular risk factors(Reference Wang, Pencina and Booth86).

Vitamin D metabolism and the central nervous system

The discovery of 1α-hydroxylase and metabolic pathways for vitamin D in the human brain(Reference Eyles, Smith and Kinobe87) and cerebrospinal fluid support a localised catabolic pathway for vitamin D in the central nervous system. The three enzymes necessary for complete synthesis and catabolism of active vitamin D has also been expressed(Reference Balabanova, Richter and Antoniadis88, Reference Reinhardt and Horst89). Serum 25(OH)D has also been evidenced to cross the blood–brain barrier a characteristic shared by other neurosteroids(Reference Kalueff, Minasyan and Keisala90).

Vitamin D receptor and the central nervous system

The nuclear functions of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 are mediated through the VDR, a member of the nuclear receptor family identified in over 2700 genomic sites(Reference Ramagopalan, Heger and Berlanga82, Reference Petkovich, Brand and Krust91). Experimental scientists have demonstrated the extensive mapping of the VDR in the rat brain(Reference Feron, Burne and Brown92Reference Walbert, Jirikowski and Prufer95), showing a similar distribution in human brain tissues(Reference Eyles, Smith and Kinobe87), with the earliest evidence in post-mortem brains of AD and Huntington's disease patients(Reference Eyles, Smith and Kinobe87, Reference Sutherland, Somerville and Yoong96). The identification of its distribution in neuronal and glial cells in the human brain followed thereafter, supporting a functional role in light of local production of active vitamin D in the human brain(Reference Eyles, Smith and Kinobe87). Areas identified in both animal and human brain tissue show similar patterns(Reference Stumpf, Sar and Clark94), in peripheral neurons,(Reference Tague and Smith97) and in several cell types of the central nervous system(Reference Prufer, Veenstra and Jirikowski93, Reference Walbert, Jirikowski and Prufer95, Reference Cornet, Baudet and Neveu98). The action of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 upon binding to the VDR has been linked with a diverse range of biological systems such as immune modulation, cell growth and cell differentiation all of which impact brain function via interaction with target genes(Reference Gronemeyer, Gustafsson and Laudet99, Reference Omdahl, Morris and May100).

Vitamin D and neuroprotection

Animal and cell culture evidence suggest that vitamin D may protect the structure and integrity of neurons through detoxification pathways and neurotrophin synthesis(Reference Wang, Wu and Cherng101). Vitamin D is known to affect the expression of three of the four neurotrophins; nerve growth factor, glial-derived nerve factor and neurotrophin 3(Reference Neveu, Naveilhan and Baudet102). Treatment of vitamin D in adult rats resulted in increased glial-derived nerve factor expression and immunoreactivity after dopamine toxicity damage(Reference Sanchez, Lopez-Martin and Segura103). Furthermore, vitamin D3 may attenuate neurotoxicity; administration of calcitriol has been shown to increase levels of antioxidants, such as glutathione in the rat brain which protects oligodendrocytes and the integrity of nerve conduction pathways critical to mental processing(Reference Chen, Lin and Chiu104). The in vitro analysis shows vitamin D treatment inhibits production TNF-α and IL 6, suggesting an anti-inflammatory role(Reference Lefebvre d'Hellencourt, Montero-Menei and Bernard105).

Vitamin D and regulation of intraneuronal calcium

Elevated levels of calcium in the brain leads to neurotoxicity. Three calcium-binding proteins have been shown to be modulated by vitamin D in brain tissues, calbindin, parvalbumin and calretinin(Reference Alexianu, Robbins and Carswell106). All three are widely and uniquely distributed in the adult brain and are believed to serve a neuroprotective role as calcium buffers(Reference Fierro and Llano107) as well as being involved in critical brain functions. In vitamin D-deficient mice, certain calcium channels are shown to be unregulated leading to an increase in Ca2+(Reference Zhu, Zhou and Yang108) and in vitro evidence has shown that vitamin D can down-regulate calcium channels(Reference Brewer, Thibault and Chen109).

Vitamin D and secondary vascular protection

Vascular dementia is the second most commonly occurring type of dementia after AD, though markedly different can also co-exist as ‘mixed dementia’. Vascular dementia is characterised by cognitive dysfunction secondary to ischemic or haemorrhagic brain lesions due to cerebrovascular disease or CVD. This type of brain damage may result from an influx of excitatory amino acids, inflammatory responses and changes in cell function, which result in excessive calcium entry(Reference Román, Tatemichi and Erkinjuntti110). With these changes presents an increase in intracellular nitric oxide production and increased oxidative stress.

Vitamin D may help improve vascular-related brain disease by mediating harmful effects of inflammation, calcium dysregulation and increased oxidative stress. During transient ischemic events, transforming growth factor and glial-derived nerve factor are upregulated in hippocampal cells to promote survival(Reference Garcion, Sindji and Leblondel111). As described earlier, vitamin D enhances innate antioxidative defences by increasing glutathione and glial-derived nerve factor concentrations(Reference Garcion, Sindji and Leblondel111Reference Wion, MacGrogan and Neveu113). These particular changes were shown to attenuate ischemic brain disease in rodents(Reference Wang, Wu and Cherng101). Furthermore, 1,25 dihydroxyvitamin D inhibits inducible nitric oxide synthetase(Reference Garcion, Nataf and Berod114), an enzyme that is up-regulated during ischaemic events and in patients with AD(Reference Vodovotz, Lucia and Flanders115). It is responsible for generating nitric oxide which is known to cause damage to neurons and oligodendrocytes at high concentrations(Reference Law, Gauthier and Quirion116).

It is plausible that vitamin D may influence vascular-related dementia via indirect mechanisms. Therapeutic intervention with vitamin D regulates blood pressure(Reference Lind, Wengle and Wide117, Reference Pfeifer, Begerow and Minne118), cardiac hypertrophy(Reference O'Connell, Berry and Jarvis119) and plasma rennin activity(Reference Burgess, Hawkins and Watanabe120, Reference Li, Kong and Wei121). There is an inverse relationship between vitamin D levels and congestive heart failure(Reference Zittermann, Schleithoff and Tenderich122) and vitamin D insufficiency is associated with incident CVD(Reference Wang, Pencina and Booth86).

Future directions

All being considered, a functional role for vitamin D in the central nervous system is still not clear but the potential for neuronal and glial cell-specific actions, e.g. proliferation, differentiation and immune modulation are well evidenced in animal models. Research into vitamin D and cognition is likely to benefit from strong collaborations between scientists trained in both disciplines. Whilst much effort is being made to standardise reporting of vitamin D status, similar standarisation of cognitive outcomes in vitamin D studies would progress our understanding. Current guidelines show that daily supplementation of 50 µg/d (2000 IU/d) is acceptable in community-dwelling adults without medical supervision(Reference Hanley, Cranney and Jones123). In countries where mandatory food fortification is in place, fortification levels remain low and therefore contribute no risk for intoxication, even when supplemental D is adhered to. It is warranted to counsel vitamin D supplementation in this population.

Conclusion

Dementia risk is a prominent concern for all individuals, governments and health professionals worldwide. Identification of modifiable risk factors is at the forefront of the ageing research agenda. There is plausible biological evidence for a role of vitamin D status in brain health from animal models. Epidemiological evidence supports this relationship but few randomised controlled trials have been conducted and show inconsistent findings. It is of great importance as vitamin D deficiency is a worldwide pandemic and is seen most commonly in older adults, residing in northerly latitudes and those of non-white ethnicity, particularly during the winter months. We are unable to indicate an optimal 25(OH)D level that may support cognitive function in healthy older adults, it seems at a minimum we should first aim to prevent deficiency through vitamin D public health strategies. There are many unanswered questions regarding a role for vitamin D and cognitive ageing; (1) What, if any, is the optimal level required to support neuroprotection? (2) At what life stage is intervention most likely to optimise brain health? (3) Is the link mediated by other lifestyle factors such as physical function? Well-designed randomised controlled trials using valid comprehensive cognitive assessments are required to determine if raising 25(OH)D concentrations through supplemental vitamin D improves cognitive health and contributes a viable component of lifestyle approaches to maintain brain health.

Financial support

N. A. is supported by a scholarship from the Irish Research Council.

Conflict of interest

None.

Authorship

N. A., B. L. and M. O'S. wrote the manuscript and approved the final draft of the submitted manuscript.

References

1.Alzheimer's Disease International (2015) World Alzheimer's Report 2015: The Global Impact of Dementia. https://www.alz.co.uk/research/world-report-2015 (accessed July 2017).Google Scholar
2.Comas-Herrera, A, Wittenberg, R, Pickard, L et al. (2007) Cognitive impairment in older people: future demand for long-term care services and the associated costs. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 22, 10371045.Google Scholar
3.World Health Organization (2017) Dementia. http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs362/en/ (accessed August 2017).Google Scholar
4.Public Health England (2017) Health Profile for England. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/health-profile-for-england (accessed June 2017).Google Scholar
5.Ferri, CP, Prince, M, Brayne, C et al. (2006) Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet 366, 21122117.Google Scholar
6.Lutz, W, Sanderson, W & Scherbov, S (2008) The coming acceleration of global population ageing. Nature 451, 716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Dehnel, T (2013) The European dementia prevention initiative. Lancet Neurol 12, 227228.Google Scholar
8.Barnes, DE & Yaffe, K (2011) The projected impact of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer's disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol 10, 819828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Kaup, AR, Nettiksimmons, J, Harris, TB et al. (2015) Cognitive resilience to apolipoprotein E ε4: contributing factors in black and white older adults. JAMA Neurol 72, 340348.Google Scholar
10.Schöttker, B, Jorde, R, Peasey, A et al. (2014) Vitamin D and mortality: meta-analysis of individual participant data from a large consortium of cohort studies from Europe and the United States. BMJ 348, g7963.Google Scholar
11.Holick, MF (2007) Vitamin D deficiency. N Engl J Med 357, 266281.Google Scholar
12.Nagpal, S, Na, S & Rathnachalam, R (2005) Noncalcemic actions of vitamin D receptor ligands. Endocr Rev 26, 662687.Google Scholar
13.Lips, P (2010) Worldwide status of vitamin D nutrition. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 121, 297300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Bouillon, R, Carmeliet, G, Daci, E et al. (1998) Vitamin D metabolism and action. Osteoporos Int 8, S013-S019.Google Scholar
15.DeLuca, HF (2004) Overview of general physiologic features and functions of vitamin D. Am J Clin Nutr 80, 1689s-1696s.Google Scholar
16.Schuster, I (2011) Cytochromes P450 are essential players in the vitamin D signaling system. Biochim Biophys Acta 1814, 186199.Google Scholar
17.Holick, MF (2008) Sunlight, UV-radiation, vitamin D and skin cancer: how much sunlight do we need? Adv Exp Med Biol 624, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Webb, AR, Kline, L & Holick, MF (1988) Influence of season and latitude on the cutaneous synthesis of vitamin D3: exposure to winter sunlight in Boston and Edmonton will not promote vitamin D3 synthesis in human skin. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 67, 373378.Google Scholar
19.Engelsen, O (2010) The relationship between ultraviolet radiation exposure and vitamin D status. Nutrients 2, 482495.Google Scholar
20.MacLaughlin, J & Holick, MF (1985) Aging decreases the capacity of human skin to produce vitamin D3. J Clin Invest 76, 15361538.Google Scholar
21.de Jongh, RT, van Schoor, NM & Lips, P (2017) Changes in vitamin D endocrinology during aging in adults. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol 453, 144150.Google Scholar
22.Matsuoka, LY, Wortsman, J, Hanifan, N et al. (1988) Chronic sunscreen use decreases circulating concentrations of 25-hydroxyvitamin D. A preliminary study. Arch Dermatol 124, 18021804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Matsuoka, LY, Wortsman, J & Hollis, BW (1990) Use of topical sunscreen for the evaluation of regional synthesis of vitamin D3. J Am Acad Dermatol 22, 772775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Snijder, MB, van Dam, RM, Visser, M et al. (2005) Adiposity in relation to vitamin D status and parathyroid hormone levels: a population-based study in older men and women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 90, 41194123.Google Scholar
25.Wortsman, J, Matsuoka, LY, Chen, TC et al. (2000) Decreased bioavailability of vitamin D in obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 72, 690693.Google Scholar
26.Food Safety Authority of Ireland (2017) Vitamin D. https://www.fsai.ie/faq/vitamin_d.html (accessed August 2017).Google Scholar
27.Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition (2016) SACN vitamin D and health report. (accessed August 2017).Google Scholar
28.Cashman, KD, Muldowney, S, McNulty, B et al. (2012) Vitamin D status of Irish adults: findings from the National Adult Nutrition Survey. Br J Nutr 109, 12481256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Cashman, KD, Hill, TR, Lucey, AJ et al. (2008) Estimation of the dietary requirement for vitamin D in healthy adults. Am J Clin Nutr 88, 15351542.Google Scholar
30.Kinyamu, HK, Gallagher, JC, Balhorn, KE et al. (1997) Serum vitamin D metabolites and calcium absorption in normal young and elderly free-living women and in women living in nursing homes. Am J Clin Nutr 65, 790797.Google Scholar
31.Kinyamu, HK, Gallagher, JC, Petranick, KM et al. (1996) Effect of parathyroid hormone (hPTH[1–34]) infusion on serum 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D and parathyroid hormone in normal women. J Bone Miner Res 11, 14001405.Google Scholar
32.Ebeling, PR, Sandgren, ME, DiMagno, EP et al. (1992) Evidence of an age-related decrease in intestinal responsiveness to vitamin D: relationship between serum 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 and intestinal vitamin D receptor concentrations in normal women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 75, 176182.Google Scholar
33.Kinyamu, HK, Gallagher, JC, Prahl, JM et al. (1997) Association between intestinal vitamin D receptor, calcium absorption, and serum 1,25 dihydroxyvitamin D in normal young and elderly women. J Bone Miner Res 12, 922928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34.Ashwell, M, Stone, EM, Stolte, H et al. (2010) UK food standards agency workshop report: an investigation of the relative contributions of diet and sunlight to vitamin D status. Br J Nutr 104, 603611.Google Scholar
35.Institute of Medicine (2010) Committee to Review Dietary Reference Intakes for Vitamin D and Calcium. Dietary Reference Intakes for Calcium and Vitamin D.Google Scholar
36.Vieth, R (2007) Vitamin D toxicity, policy, and science. J Bone Miner Res 22, V6468.Google Scholar
37.Hirani, V & Primatesta, P (2005) Vitamin D concentrations among people aged 65 years and over living in private households and institutions in England: population survey. Age Ageing 34, 485491.Google Scholar
38.Hirani, V, Tull, K, Ali, A et al. (2010) Urgent action needed to improve vitamin D status among older people in England! Age Ageing 39, 6268.Google Scholar
39.McCarroll, K, Beirne, A, Casey, M et al. (2015) Determinants of 25-hydroxyvitamin D in older Irish adults. Age Ageing 44, 847853.Google Scholar
40.Brouwer-Brolsma, EM, Feskens, EJ, Steegenga, WT et al. (2013) Associations of 25-hydroxyvitamin D with fasting glucose, fasting insulin, dementia and depression in European elderly: the SENECA study. Eur J Nutr 52, 917925.Google Scholar
41.Kuzma, E, Soni, M, Littlejohns, TJ et al. (2016) Vitamin D and memory decline: two population-based prospective studies. J Alzheimers Dis 50, 10991108.Google Scholar
42.Toffanello, ED, Coin, A, Perissinotto, E et al. (2014) Vitamin D deficiency predicts cognitive decline in older men and women: the Pro.V.A. study. Neurology 83, 22922298.Google Scholar
43.van Schoor, NM, Comijs, HC, Llewellyn, DJ et al. (2016) Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D and cognitive functioning. Int Psychogeriatr 28, 759768.Google Scholar
44.Wilson, VK, Houston, DK, Kilpatrick, L et al. (2014) Relationship between 25-hydroxyvitamin D and cognitive function in older adults: the health, aging and body composition study. J Am Geriatr Soc 62, 636641.Google Scholar
45.Spencer, RJ, Wendell, CR, Giggey, PP et al. (2013) Psychometric limitations of the mini-mental state examination among nondemented older adults: an evaluation of neurocognitive and magnetic resonance imaging correlates. Exp Aging Res 39, 382397.Google Scholar
46.Petersen, RC (2004) Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med 256, 183194.Google Scholar
47.Fisk, JD, Merry, HR & Rockwood, K (2003) Variations in case definition affect prevalence but not outcomes of mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 61, 11791184.Google Scholar
48.Kanai, R & Rees, G (2011) The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 12, 231.Google Scholar
49.Carriere, J, Cheyne, J & Smilek, D (2008) Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: the affective consequences of mindlessness. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 835847.Google Scholar
50.Eysenck, MW, Derakshan, N, Santos, R et al. (2007) Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51.Conroy, RM, Golden, J, Jeffares, I et al. (2010) Boredom-proneness, loneliness, social engagement and depression and their association with cognitive function in older people: a population study. Psychol Health Med 15, 463473.Google Scholar
52.Simpson, EEA, Maylor, EA, McConville, C et al. (2014) Mood and cognition in healthy older European adults: the Zenith study. BMC Psychol 2, 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53.Barker-Collo, S, Bartle, H, Clarke, A et al. (2008) Accuracy of the National adult reading test and spot the word estimates of premorbid intelligence in a non-clinical New Zealand sample. NZ J. Psychol 37, 5361.Google Scholar
54.Poldrack, RA & Wagner, AD (2004) What can neuroimaging tell Us about the mind? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 13, 177181.Google Scholar
55.Allard, M, Husky, M, Catheline, G et al. (2014) Mobile technologies in the early detection of cognitive decline. PLoS ONE 9, e112197.Google Scholar
56.Hannigan, C, Coen, RF, Lawlor, BA et al. (2015) The NEIL memory research unit: psychosocial, biological, physiological and lifestyle factors associated with healthy ageing: study protocol. BMC Psychol 3, 20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
57.Kenny, RA, Coen, RF, Frewen, J et al. (2013) Normative values of cognitive and physical function in older adults: findings from the Irish longitudinal study on ageing. J Am Geriatr Soc 61, S279-S290.Google Scholar
58.Aspell, NH, Lawlor, BA & O'Suillivan, M. editor Does Vitamin D Supplementation Improve Cognitive Performance? A Randomised Double-Blind Placebo-Controlled Pilot Trial in Healthy Older Adults. The Nutrition Society Irish Conference ‘What governs what we eat?’; 21 June 2017; Queens University.Google Scholar
59.Tolppanen, AM, Williams, D & Lawlor, DA (2011) The association of circulating 25-hydroxyvitamin D and calcium with cognitive performance in adolescents: cross-sectional study using data from the third National health and nutrition examination survey. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 25, 6774.Google Scholar
60.Bartali, B, Devore, E, Grodstein, F et al. (2014) Plasma vitamin D levels and cognitive function in aging women: the nurses’ health study. J Nutr Health Aging 18, 400406.Google Scholar
61.Brouwer-Brolsma, EM, van de Rest, O, Tieland, M et al. (2013) Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D is associated with cognitive executive function in Dutch prefrail and frail elderly: a cross-sectional study exploring the associations of 25-hydroxyvitamin D with glucose metabolism, cognitive performance and depression. J Am Med Dir Assoc 14, 852.e859–817.Google Scholar
62.Menant, JC, Close, JC, Delbaere, K et al. (2012) Relationships between serum vitamin D levels, neuromuscular and neuropsychological function and falls in older men and women. Osteoporos Int 23, 981989.Google Scholar
63.Seamans, KM, Hill, TR, Scully, L et al. (2010) Vitamin D status and measures of cognitive function in healthy older European adults. Eur J Clin Nutr 64, 11721178.Google Scholar
64.Littlejohns, TJ, Henley, WE, Lang, IA et al. (2014) Vitamin D and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. Neurology 83, 920928.Google Scholar
65.Afzal, S, Bojesen, SE & Nordestgaard, BG (2014) Reduced 25-hydroxyvitamin D and risk of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Alzheimers Dement 10, 296302.Google Scholar
66.Llewellyn, DJ, Lang, IA, Langa, KM et al. (2010) Vitamin D and risk of cognitive decline in elderly persons. Arch Intern Med 170, 11351141.Google Scholar
67.Balion, C, Griffith, LE, Strifler, L et al. (2012) Vitamin D, cognition, and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology 79, 13971405.Google Scholar
68.van der Schaft, J, Koek, HL, Dijkstra, E et al. (2013) The association between vitamin D and cognition: a systematic review. Ageing Res Rev 12, 10131023.Google Scholar
69.Pettersen, JA (2017) Does high dose vitamin D supplementation enhance cognition? A randomized trial in healthy adults. Exp Gerontol 90, 9097.Google Scholar
70.Annweiler, C, Herrmann, FR, Fantino, B et al. (2012) Effectiveness of the combination of memantine plus vitamin D on cognition in patients with Alzheimer disease: a pre-post pilot study. Cogn Behav Neurol 25, 121127.Google Scholar
71.Rossom, RC, Espeland, MA, Manson, JAE et al. (2012) Calcium and Vitamin D Supplementation and Cognitive Impairment in the Women's Health Initiative. J Am Geriatr Soc 60, 21972205.Google Scholar
72.Bischoff-Ferrari, H (2016) DO-Health. http://do-health.eu/wordpress/ (accessed August 2017).Google Scholar
73.Dhesi, JK, Jackson, SH, Bearne, LM et al. (2004) Vitamin D supplementation improves neuromuscular function in older people who fall. Age Ageing 33, 589595.Google Scholar
74.Przybelski, RJ & Binkley, NC (2007) Is vitamin D important for preserving cognition? A positive correlation of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D concentration with cognitive function. Arch Biochem Biophys 460, 202205.Google Scholar
75.Buell, JS, Dawson-Hughes, B, Scott, TM et al. (2010) 25-Hydroxyvitamin D, dementia, and cerebrovascular pathology in elders receiving home services. Neurology 74, 1826.Google Scholar
76.Kipen, E, Helme, RD, Wark, JD et al. (1995) Bone Density, Vitamin D Nutrition, and Parathyroid Hormone Levels in Women with Dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 43, 10881091.Google Scholar
77.Sato, Y, Asoh, T & Oizumi, K (1998) High prevalence of vitamin D deficiency and reduced bone mass in elderly women with Alzheimer's disease. Bone 23, 555557.Google Scholar
78.Eyles, DW, Burne, THJ & McGrath, JJ (2013) Vitamin D, effects on brain development, adult brain function and the links between low levels of vitamin D and neuropsychiatric disease. Front Neuroendocrinol 34, 4764.Google Scholar
79.Groves, NJ, McGrath, JJ & Burne, TH (2014) Vitamin D as a neurosteroid affecting the developing and adult brain. Annu Rev Nutr 34, 117141.Google Scholar
80.Fernandes de Abreu, DA, Eyles, D & Feron, F (2009) Vitamin D, a neuro-immunomodulator: implications for neurodegenerative and autoimmune diseases. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, Suppl. 1, S265S277.Google Scholar
81.Khanal, RC & Nemere, I (2007) The ERp57/GRp58/1,25D3-MARRS receptor: multiple functional roles in diverse cell systems. Curr Med Chem 14, 10871093.Google Scholar
82.Ramagopalan, SV, Heger, A, Berlanga, AJ et al. (2010) A ChIP-seq defined genome-wide map of vitamin D receptor binding: associations with disease and evolution. Genome Res 20, 13521360.Google Scholar
83.Brown, J, Bianco, JI, McGrath, JJ et al. (2003) 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 induces nerve growth factor, promotes neurite outgrowth and inhibits mitosis in embryonic rat hippocampal neurons. Neurosci Lett 343, 139143.Google Scholar
84.Buell, JS & Dawson-Hughes, B (2008) Vitamin D and neurocognitive dysfunction: preventing ‘D'ecline? Mol Aspects Med 29, 415422.Google Scholar
85.Garcion, E, Wion-Barbot, N, Montero-Menei, CN et al. (2002) New clues about vitamin D functions in the nervous system. Trends Endocrinol Metab 13, 100105.Google Scholar
86.Wang, TJ, Pencina, MJ, Booth, SL et al. (2008) Vitamin D deficiency and risk of cardiovascular disease. Circulation 117, 503511.Google Scholar
87.Eyles, DW, Smith, S, Kinobe, R et al. (2005) Distribution of the vitamin D receptor and 1α-hydroxylase in human brain. J Chem Neuroanat 29, 2130.Google Scholar
88.Balabanova, S, Richter, HP, Antoniadis, G et al. (1984) 25-Hydroxyvitamin D, 24, 25-dihydroxyvitamin D and 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D in human cerebrospinal fluid. Klin Wochenschr 62, 10861090.Google Scholar
89.Reinhardt, TA & Horst, RL (1989) Self-induction of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 metabolism limits receptor occupancy and target tissue responsiveness. J Biol Chem 264, 1591715921.Google Scholar
90.Kalueff, A, Minasyan, A, Keisala, T et al. (2006) The vitamin D neuroendocrine system as a target for novel neurotropic drugs. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets 5, 363371.Google Scholar
91.Petkovich, M, Brand, NJ, Krust, A et al. (1987) A human retinoic acid receptor which belongs to the family of nuclear receptors. Nature 330, 444450.Google Scholar
92.Feron, F, Burne, TH, Brown, J et al. (2005) Developmental vitamin D3 deficiency alters the adult rat brain. Brain Res Bull 65, 141148.Google Scholar
93.Prufer, K, Veenstra, TD, Jirikowski, GF et al. (1999) Distribution of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 receptor immunoreactivity in the rat brain and spinal cord. J Chem Neuroanat 16, 135145.Google Scholar
94.Stumpf, W, Sar, M, Clark, S et al. (1982) Brain target sites for 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3. Science 215, 14031405.Google Scholar
95.Walbert, T, Jirikowski, GF & Prufer, K (2001) Distribution of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 receptor immunoreactivity in the limbic system of the rat. Horm Metab Res 33, 525531.Google Scholar
96.Sutherland, MK, Somerville, MJ, Yoong, LK et al. (1992) Reduction of vitamin D hormone receptor mRNA levels in Alzheimer as compared to Huntington hippocampus: correlation with calbindin-28k mRNA levels. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 13, 239250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
97.Tague, SE & Smith, PG (2011) Vitamin D receptor and enzyme expression in dorsal root ganglia of adult female rats: modulation by ovarian hormones. J Chem Neuroanat 41, 112.Google Scholar
98.Cornet, A, Baudet, C, Neveu, I et al. (1998) 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D3 regulates the expression of VDR and NGF gene in Schwann cells in vitro. J Neurosci Res 53, 742746.Google Scholar
99.Gronemeyer, H, Gustafsson, JA & Laudet, V (2004) Principles for modulation of the nuclear receptor superfamily. Nat Rev Drug Discov 3, 950964.Google Scholar
100.Omdahl, JL, Morris, HA & May, BK (2002) Hydroxylase enzymes of the vitamin D pathway: expression, function, and regulation. Annu Rev Nutr 22, 139166.Google Scholar
101.Wang, JY, Wu, JN, Cherng, TL et al. (2001) Vitamin D(3) attenuates 6-hydroxydopamine-induced neurotoxicity in rats. Brain Res 904, 6775.Google Scholar
102.Neveu, I, Naveilhan, P, Baudet, C et al. (1994) 1, 25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 regulates NT-3, NT-4 but not BDNF mRNA in astrocytes. Neuroreport 6, 124126.Google Scholar
103.Sanchez, B, Lopez-Martin, E, Segura, C et al. (2002) 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D3 increases striatal GDNF mRNA and protein expression in adult rats. Mol Brain Res 108, 143146.Google Scholar
104.Chen, KB, Lin, AM & Chiu, TH (2003) Systemic vitamin D3 attenuated oxidative injuries in the locus coeruleus of rat brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 993, 313324; discussion 345–319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
105.Lefebvre d'Hellencourt, C, Montero-Menei, CN, Bernard, R et al. (2003) Vitamin D3 inhibits proinflammatory cytokines and nitric oxide production by the EOC13 microglial cell line. J Neurosci Res 71, 575582.Google Scholar
106.Alexianu, ME, Robbins, E, Carswell, S et al. (1998) 1Alpha, 25 dihydroxyvitamin D3-dependent up-regulation of calcium-binding proteins in motoneuron cells. J Neurosci Res 51, 5866.3.0.CO;2-K>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
107.Fierro, L & Llano, I (1996) High endogenous calcium buffering in Purkinje cells from rat cerebellar slices. J Physiol 496 (Pt 3), 617625.Google Scholar
108.Zhu, Y, Zhou, R, Yang, R et al. (2012) Abnormal neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of adult mice lacking 1,25-dihydroxy vitamin D3 (1,25-(OH)2D3). Hippocampus 22, 421433.Google Scholar
109.Brewer, LD, Thibault, V, Chen, KC et al. (2001) Vitamin D hormone confers neuroprotection in parallel with downregulation of L-type calcium channel expression in hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 21, 98108.Google Scholar
110.Román, GC, Tatemichi, TK, Erkinjuntti, T et al. (1993) Vascular dementia Diagnostic criteria for research studies: report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop. Neurology 43, 250250.Google Scholar
111.Garcion, E, Sindji, L, Leblondel, G et al. (1999) 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D3 regulates the synthesis of gamma-glutamyl transpeptidase and glutathione levels in rat primary astrocytes. J Neurochem 73, 859866.Google Scholar
112.Naveilhan, P, Neveu, I, Wion, D et al. (1996) 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D3, an inducer of glial cell line-derived neurotrophic factor. Neuroreport 7, 21712175.Google Scholar
113.Wion, D, MacGrogan, D, Neveu, I et al. (1991) 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D3 is a potent inducer of nerve growth factor synthesis. J Neurosci Res 28, 110114.Google Scholar
114.Garcion, E, Nataf, S, Berod, A et al. (1997) 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D3 inhibits the expression of inducible nitric oxide synthase in rat central nervous system during experimental allergic encephalomyelitis. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 45, 255267.Google Scholar
115.Vodovotz, Y, Lucia, MS, Flanders, KC et al. (1996) Inducible nitric oxide synthase in tangle-bearing neurons of patients with Alzheimer's disease. J Exp Med 184, 14251433.Google Scholar
116.Law, A, Gauthier, S & Quirion, R (2001) Say NO to Alzheimer's disease: the putative links between nitric oxide and dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Brain Res Rev 35, 7396.Google Scholar
117.Lind, L, Wengle, B, Wide, L et al. (1989) Reduction of blood pressure during long-term treatment with active vitamin D (alphacalcidol) is dependent on plasma renin activity and calcium status. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Am J Hypertens 2, 2025.Google Scholar
118.Pfeifer, M, Begerow, B, Minne, HW et al. (2001) Effects of a short-term vitamin D(3) and calcium supplementation on blood pressure and parathyroid hormone levels in elderly women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 86, 16331637.Google Scholar
119.O'Connell, TD, Berry, JE, Jarvis, AK et al. (1997) 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D3 regulation of cardiac myocyte proliferation and hypertrophy. Am J Physiol 272, H1751-H1758.Google Scholar
120.Burgess, ED, Hawkins, RG & Watanabe, M (1990) Interaction of 1,25-dihydroxyvitamin D and plasma renin activity in high renin essential hypertension. Am J Hypertens 3, 903905.Google Scholar
121.Li, YC, Kong, J, Wei, M et al. (2002) 1,25-Dihydroxyvitamin D(3) is a negative endocrine regulator of the renin-angiotensin system. J Clin Invest 110, 229238.Google Scholar
122.Zittermann, A, Schleithoff, SS, Tenderich, G et al. (2003) Low vitamin D status: a contributing factor in the pathogenesis of congestive heart failure? J Am Coll Cardiol 41, 105112.Google Scholar
123.Hanley, DA, Cranney, A, Jones, G et al. (2010) Vitamin D in adult health and disease: a review and guideline statement from Osteoporosis Canada (summary). CMAJ 182, 13151319.Google Scholar
124.Cooper, C, McLaren, M, Wood, PJ et al. (1989) Indices of calcium metabolism in women with hip fractures. Bone Miner 5, 193200.Google Scholar
125.Vir, SC & Love, AH (1978) Vitamin D status of elderly at home and institutionalised in hospital. Int J Vitam Nutr Res 48, 123130.Google Scholar
126.Solanki, T, Hyatt, RH, Kemm, JR et al. (1995) Are elderly Asians in Britain at a high risk of vitamin D deficiency and osteomalacia? Age Ageing 24, 103107.Google Scholar
127.Mavroeidi, A, O'Neill, F, Lee, PA et al. (2010) Seasonal 25-hydroxyvitamin D changes in British postmenopausal women at 57 degrees N and 51 degrees N: a longitudinal study. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 121, 459461.Google Scholar
128.Lips, P, Hosking, D, Lippuner, K et al. (2006) The prevalence of vitamin D inadequacy amongst women with osteoporosis: an international epidemiological investigation. J Intern Med 260, 245254.Google Scholar
129.Lips, P, van Ginkel, FC, Jongen, MJ et al. (1987) Determinants of vitamin D status in patients with hip fracture and in elderly control subjects. Am J Clin Nutr 46, 10051010.Google Scholar
130.Forouhi, NG, Luan, J, Cooper, A et al. (2008) Baseline serum 25-hydroxy vitamin d is predictive of future glycemic status and insulin resistance: the Medical Research Council Ely Prospective Study 1990–2000. Diabetes 57, 26192625.Google Scholar
131.Hill, TR, O'Brien, MM, Lamberg-Allardt, C et al. (2006) Vitamin D status of 51–75-year-old Irish women: its determinants and impact on biochemical indices of bone turnover. Public Health Nutr 9, 225233.Google Scholar
132.Hill, T, Collins, A, O'Brien, M et al. (2005) Vitamin D intake and status in Irish postmenopausal women. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, 404410.Google Scholar
133.Elia, M & Stratton, RJ (2005) Geographical inequalities in nutrient status and risk of malnutrition among English people aged 65 y and older. Nutrition 21, 11001106.Google Scholar
134.Andersen, R, Molgaard, C, Skovgaard, LT et al. (2005) Teenage girls and elderly women living in northern Europe have low winter vitamin D status. Eur J Clin Nutr 59, 533541.Google Scholar
135.Woitge, HW, Scheidt-Nave, C, Kissling, C et al. (1998) Seasonal variation of biochemical indexes of bone turnover: results of a population-based study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 83, 6875.Google Scholar
136.Boonen, S, Vanderschueren, D, Cheng, XG et al. (1997) Age-related (type II) femoral neck osteoporosis in men: biochemical evidence for both hypovitaminosis D- and androgen deficiency-induced bone resorption. J Bone Miner Res 12, 21192126.Google Scholar
137.van der Wielen, RP, Lowik, MR, van den Berg, H et al. (1995) Serum vitamin D concentrations among elderly people in Europe. Lancet 346, 207210.Google Scholar
138.Chapuy, MC, Schott, AM, Garnero, P et al. (1996) Healthy elderly French women living at home have secondary hyperparathyroidism and high bone turnover in winter. EPIDOS Study Group. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 81, 11291133.Google Scholar
139.Macdonald, HM, Mavroeidi, A, Barr, RJ et al. (2008) Vitamin D status in postmenopausal women living at higher latitudes in the UK in relation to bone health, overweight, sunlight exposure and dietary vitamin D. Bone 42, 9961003.Google Scholar
140.McCarthy, D, Collins, A, O'Brien, M et al. (2006) Vitamin D intake and status in Irish elderly women and adolescent girls. Ir J Med Sci 175, 14.Google Scholar
141.Brouwer-Brolsma, EM, Dhonukshe-Rutten, RAM, van Wijngaarden, JP et al. (2016) Low vitamin D status is associated with more depressive symptoms in Dutch older adults. Eur J Nutr 55, 15251534.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Prevalence of 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) deficiency in community-dwelling adults, aged over 50 years in Europe

Figure 1

Table 2. Intervention studies; effect of vitamin D supplementation and cognitive performance outcomes in healthy older adults