1. Introduction
Governments, international organizations, and private donors commit substantial resources each year to interventions working with ordinary citizens, and often youth, with the goal of promoting peace in violence-affected countries. If the violence is between ethnic groups, such interventions often involve bringing members of these groups together. Research has examined if contact with individual members can reduce prejudice toward the whole outgroup with hundreds of studies (Tropp et al., Reference Tropp, Hawi, Van Laar and Levin2012; Paluck et al., Reference Paluck, Green and Green2019). Yet, a disproportionally large number of these studies relies on surveys or takes place in a laboratory. Few have tested intergroup contact in real-world environments, and even fewer in conflict settings.
Those that do, such as Scacco and Warren (Reference Scacco and Warren2018), Mousa (Reference Mousa2020), or Zhou and Lyall (Reference Zhou and Lyall2022), have come away with results suggesting mixed effects in the short term with ambiguous implications for the longer term. In this paper, we examine the impact of youth contact in conflict settings with a randomized controlled trial (RCT) embedded within an existing program in Israel, and a “data fusion” analysis to characterize the effects of multiyear contact exposure. Our study thus contributes substantively to the understanding of contact in conflict settings, and methodologically to the estimation of long-term predictions from cross-sectional data through a combination of experimental and observational data.
We partnered with a civil society organization that has offered an intergroup contact intervention to Jewish-Israeli and Arab-Palestinian youths for 14 years across 20 communities. The contact experience is embedded within a sports program, similarly to other recent intergroup contact field studies (Mousa, Reference Mousa2020; Lowe, Reference Lowe2021). The program intends to be developmental, working with youth over multiple years. Hence, a fair evaluation ought to account for accumulated exposure over multiple years. Doing so would allow us to go beyond the relatively short-term exposure in recent field studies. In our field research, as in many similar cases, practical and ethical constraints make it difficult to sustain treatment-control differences to evaluate multiyear effects. One of the main contributions of our research is the strategy that we devised to overcome these constraints: using machine-learning techniques, we supplement a modestly sized, one-year experiment (N = 138) with a large repeated cross-sectional survey of one-year and multiyear participants (N = 645) which was more in line with the monitoring and evaluation strategy of our partner. In doing so, we contribute to the growing body of research that integrates observational and experimental data to estimate treatment effects (Künzel et al., Reference Künzel, Sekhon, Bickel and Yu2019; Colnet et al., Reference Colnet, Mayer, Chen, Dieng, Li, Varoquaux, Vert, Josse and Yang2020; Kallus and Mao, Reference Kallus and Mao2020; Li et al., Reference Li, Miao, Cai, Liu, Zhang, Xue and Geng2020; Rosenman et al., Reference Rosenman, Basse, Owen and Baiocchi2020; Imbens et al., Reference Imbens, Kallus, Mao and Wang2022; Degtiar and Rose, Reference Degtiar and Rose2023). The increased sample size also allows us to estimate group-specific effects with reasonable precision. Causal identification is based on the fact that the participants selected for the experiment are drawn from the same population as those who were part of the cross-sectional survey, and assumes that there are no systematic, unobservable difference between the experimental and cross-sectional survey samples. This analytical strategy, we argue, can be particularly useful to researchers who want to maximize learning while working with resource-constrained civil-society organizations.
While our evidence cannot affirm positive program effects in the one-year RCT, the analysis of multiyear exposure suggests that program effects become more pronounced over time in ways that clearly distinguish Jewish-Israeli from Arab-Palestinian participants. For Jewish-Israeli boys in particular, effects are strong and robust to attrition. These results highlight how challenging it is to implement a program that benefits all groups equally.
2. Contact in conflict settings
Intergroup contact theory has been lauded as one of the best-tested tools for reducing intergroup prejudice. This recommendation is often based on a seminal meta-analysis of 515 studies that concluded that intergroup contact works (Pettigrew and Tropp, Reference Pettigrew and Tropp2006). Yet, most of these studies are correlational and less than three percent involve groups in conflict. Recognizing this gap, a second meta-analysis of intergroup contact in applied settings assembled a database of an impressive 129 samples from outside of laboratory or survey contexts (Lemmer and Wagner, Reference Lemmer and Wagner2015). Again, the conclusion was that contact works—even in conflict settings. A closer look, however, reveals that only 11 samples evaluating five real-world programs stem from conflict settings. More than a third of the effect sizes are null or negative and none of the studies used a randomized design.
Considering how much scholarly attention intergroup contact theory has received, we know surprisingly little about its validity in real-world programs, especially in conflict settings. Conducting research on intergroup contact in applied conflict settings is particularly difficult which is why, despite great interest in the topic, few scholars have successfully implemented randomized designs there. Recent exceptions are one experiment in Nigeria (Scacco and Warren, Reference Scacco and Warren2018) and one in Iraq (Mousa, Reference Mousa2020), which find mixed evidence on intergroup relations with limited to no impact of intergroup contact on outgroup attitudes. Thus, before we can derive policy lessons from intergroup contact theory for applied conflict settings, we need more rigorous tests in applied conflict settings. Our innovative fusion design that combines RCTs with a repeated cross-sectional survey is a tool for conducting such tests. It thus offers technological and analytical advancements to intergroup contact research (Paolini et al., Reference Paolini, White, Tropp, Turner, Page-Gould, Barlow and Gómez2021).
Most intergroup contact experiments expose participants for a short time, often one research session, to the outgroup. Yet, intergroup contact is more likely to succeed if repeated: over time individuals develop resources to appraise intergroup interactions as non-stressful (Mendes et al., Reference Mendes, Blascovich, Hunter, Lickel and Jost2007) and bond with outgroup members (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Brody, Aron, Crandall and Schaller2005). This is particularly important in conflict societies where intergroup anxiety is higher, and the effects of repeated contact rarely evaluated. Our fusion design allows us to examine such multiyear contact effects, even when it is difficult to implement a multiyear RCT design.
Israel is an important context for studying intergroup contact, with research in applied programs painting a mixed picture. Non-randomized evaluations of a summer camp (Schroeder and Risen, Reference Schroeder and Risen2016) and several sport programs (Galily et al., Reference Galily, Leitner and Shimion2013b) showed reduced prejudice effects. A natural experiment found intergroup contact between Jewish-Israeli patients and Arab doctors reduced Jewish patients’ anti-Arab prejudice (Weiss, Reference Weiss2021). Other studies illuminate the challenges for implementing intergroup contact programs in the field: the emotionally charged nature of facilitated dialogues (Kahanoff, Reference Kahanoff2016), hardening of group boundaries in face of security threats and power discrepancy (Hammack, Reference Hammack2006), or even logistical challenges in executing these programs (Litvak-Hirsch et al., Reference Litvak-Hirsch, Galily, Leitner, Parnell and Pringle2018).
2.1 Majority versus minority participants and group status
Intergroup contact programs require participation of members from at least two adversarial groups. The program we worked with in Israel brings together youth from a majority (Jewish-Israelis) and a minority (Arab-Palestinians residing within Israel) group. Ideally, both groups should benefit from the program, although evidence suggests that minorities tend to benefit less, and are less likely to change their outgroup attitudes as a function of intergroup contact (Tropp and Pettigrew, Reference Tropp and Pettigrew2005). Creating a program that serves both groups equally in the field is complicated, as evidence suggests that members of different status groups have different priorities for intergroup encounters. Many civil-society interventions with a focus on commonalities (e.g., common humanity) reflect the preference of majority groups to be liked and accepted by the minority group (Dovidio et al., Reference Dovidio, Gaertner and Saguy2009). Meanwhile, members of the minority group tend to prefer for societal inequalities and injustices to be more directly addressed (Hässler et al., Reference Hässler, Ullrich, Sebben, Shnabel, Bernardino, Valdenegro, Van Laar, González, Visintin and Tropp2022). This dynamic leads us to expect the program to be less successful in changing the prejudice and ingroup-regulation behavior of Arab-Palestinian participants compared to Jewish-Israeli participants.
2.2 Intergroup contact outcomes
We measure prejudice through several measures of outgroup regard, but also assess ingroup regulation outcomes. In conflict-affected societies where everyday opportunities for intergroup contact are rare (Enos, Reference Enos2017), civil-society programs are often among the very few modes of intergroup contact. Despite their potential, these programs typically do not reach a significant share of a country's population. In addition to reducing prejudice, it is thus desirable that intergroup contact programs increase participants’ motivation to influence their non-participant ingroup peers (Ditlmann et al., Reference Ditlmann, Samii and Zeitzoff2017).
Influence strategies may include ingroup censuring, ingroup policing, persuasion of outgroup positive intent, and perspective sharing. Ingroup censuring (Ditlmann and Samii, Reference Ditlmann and Samii2016) and policing (Fearon and Laitin, Reference Fearon and Laitin1996) entail privately or publicly condemning an ingroup members’ aggression toward the outgroup. In terms of persuasion, people can convince their peers that outgroup members have benevolent instead of hostile intentions, thus disrupting hostile attribution biases (McGlothlin and Killen, Reference McGlothlin and Killen2006; van Dijk et al., Reference van Dijk, Thomaes, Poorthuis and Orobio de Castro2019), or share the perspective of the outgroup with their ingroup members (Bruneau and Saxe, Reference Bruneau and Saxe2012).
3. Research hypotheses
Based on the preceding discussion, our core prediction is in keeping with the original contact hypothesis:
H1a: Participating versus not participating in an intergroup contact program within a conflict setting causes an increase in positive outgroup regard.
H1b: The longer youths participate in the program, the stronger the positive effect of intergroup contact on participants’ outgroup regard.
Following past research which suggest that the association between intergroup contact and outgroup regard is less conclusive for minority than for majority group members (Tropp and Pettigrew, Reference Tropp and Pettigrew2005), we also tested if:
H1c: H1a and H1b are moderated by participants’ ethnicity.
As outlined earlier, another way in which contact experiences may positively affect group dynamics is by strengthening individuals’ tendency to influence their non-participant peers.
H2a: Participating versus not participating in an intergroup contact program within a conflict setting increases participants’ tendency to regulate their peers.
H2b: The longer youths participate in the program, the stronger the positive effect of intergroup contact on participants’ tendency to regulate their peers.
We also tested, following the same logic as above, if:
H2c: H2a and H2b are moderated by participants’ ethnicity.
Our pre-analysis plan also hypothesized effects on individual self-esteem, as a potential mediator of effects on peer regulation. We find no effects on self-esteem and, given space constraints, point readers to our Appendix (Section 9), which contains all of those results. The Appendix (Table 37) also explains all other deviations from our pre-analysis plan.
4. Intervention
Our study examines the impact of an existing field intervention that combines sports with intergroup contactFootnote 1 —a sports league for Arab-Palestinian and Jewish-Israeli youth in Israel of age 4–20. Our research focuses on young adolescents (from 8 to 16 year olds, with the majority of participants being between the age of 10 and 13), a period of intensive socialization into political and social life beyond the family (Lerner and Steinberg, Reference Lerner and Steinberg2009).
The organization—through schools or community organizations—invites youth to join a team with about 12 peers from the same residential community, ethnic group, and gender, leading with high-level sports training. Indeed, participants from both groups report the desire to play sports as their main reason for joining the program (Figure A1). This is echoed in anecdotes the non-governmental organization (NGO) collects in the field:
At first I joined [the organization] only to play sports and I did not know that it included Jews as well in the practices.
(Interview with an Arab-Palestinian participant, 2018)Footnote 2
The teams practice weekly with their ingroup. After about a month, joint practices between an Arab-Palestinian team and a Jewish-Israeli team begin. These joint practices involve sports drills, team-building exercises, and games between ethnically mixed (single-gender) teams. They take place approximately five to eight times throughout the season (fall to summer). In our RCT sample, each team has their own partner team, but partnering teams are not part of our sample. Even the rather modest contact between Arab-Palestinian and Jewish-Israeli youth in the form of joint practices represents a dramatic departure from the typical experience of youth in Israel, who tend to live in highly segregated communities (Hammack, Reference Hammack2011; Shwed et al., Reference Shwed, Kalish and Shavit2018). As described by one participant:
We can live five minutes from each other, and never meet or talk. [Name], who is a Palestinian participant in the program, she lives five minutes away from me but I would have never had the chance to meet her if it wasn't for the program. (Interview with a 14-year-old Jewish-Israeli participant during an intergroup meet-up session; June 2020)
Our partner organization aims to implement the optimal conditions for intergroup contact (Allport, Reference Allport1954). Team sport provides a common goal (during joint practices, teams are always ethnically mixed) and requires cooperation. To achieve status equality within the program, coaches and other leadership positions are distributed equally among Jewish-Israelis and Arab-Palestinians, while teams that practice together are matched on age, gender, and athletic skills. Coaches and program leaders encourage and model peaceful intergroup relations, thus satisfying the fourth of Allport's original conditions for optimal contact, i.e., support from relevant authorities.
To ensure ecological validity (i.e., study reflecting the complexity and dynamics of the real world), we integrated our research strategy as much as possible into the existing program. The capacities and resources of an NGO working in an extremely politicized context are already stretched and program implementation depends on the cooperation of diverse actors (teachers, coaches, communities for practice space, etc.) Our efforts not to disrupt this difficult-to-achieve balance sometimes came at a cost to our research design (e.g., smaller RCT sample), yet our newly proposed fusion design aims to overcome this challenge.
5. Research design
Our research design (Figure 1) includes an RCT (N = 138) component that allows us to estimate one-year effects, and then a component that fuses the RCT data with data from a general survey (N = 645) to estimate how effects accumulated over one, two, and up to three years of program participation.
5.1 RCT design
For the RCT, our implementing partner recruited participants across grades four to six (typically ages 10–13) within three research sites (locations A, B, and C within Figure 1). The three sites for the RCT were not randomly selected. For this reason, in the analysis that fuses the RCT and survey data, we control for covariates that might be out of balance across the sites.
In a manner that is consistent with the way that the program generally operates, teachers at schools and community centers in the RCT sites publicized the program and collected applications. Our partner leads with sports in recruiting which reduces concerns over how selection into our study population affects generalizability. Even if there was outcome-relevant selection into our sample (e.g., participants’ parents being particularly open minded) an average effect of the treatment on the treated is still a reasonable estimand. Were this or a similar extracurricular program to be scaled up, youths would still have to opt into it, and hence the effects we estimate are for the subpopulation of youths who would plausibly choose to participate in the program. Once recruited into the RCT, applicants were randomly assigned to the program (treatment group) or put on a waiting list that would permit access in a subsequent season (control group). Youth in the treatment group were placed on the sports teams that then went through the season-long curriculum described earlier.
The teams formed in the RCT sites included youth who were granted access through the RCT random assignment, as well as youth outside the RCT participant pool. The latter came through channels that could not be randomized and were not included in the research (e.g., school programs, younger age cohorts from the same organization that advanced to the next level). As a result, each year only a few slots in the teams were available for randomization. In each location, we filled these slots over five seasons (2014–2018, see Figure 1), with new cohorts of RCT participants. Control group participants were often admitted into the program after one season for ethical reasons, which is why we could not track RCT cohorts beyond one season. Nonetheless, our non-experimental survey data, which we incorporate using a method discussed below, allow us to make predictions about the effects of multiyear exposure under three assumptions described in the next section.
In locations A and B, treatment assigned was at the individual level. In location C, assignment occurred at the 12-person group level, because the available participants were already grouped into teams by a cooperating sports club that could not be split up. Ensuring consistency with this randomization protocol, our analysis below accounts for potential clustering among the group-assigned participants in location C (Gerber and Green, Reference Gerber and Green2012). Table 1 shows compliance rates and attrition. Out of the youth assigned to the control condition, nine did not comply. This happened because coaches or teachers had a strong preference that certain children should be on a team, either because the child would benefit the team a lot (typically due to strong athletic skills) or the child would benefit a lot. For ethical reasons, we did not intervene to prevent such crossovers. We treat those who did not comply with the treatment as non-compliers, and those for whom we were not able to obtain outcome data as attrition. We conducted endline surveys with RCT-treated and control youth at the end of the season, typically about seven months after assignment.
Table 2 shows the demographic composition of our RCT sample. While the distribution of basic demographic attributes is balanced between those assigned to treatment and control groups, the overall sample exhibits important idiosyncrasies (Appendix, Table 1). All Arab-Palestinian participants in our sample are female, largely a consequence of organization's deliberate efforts to improve Arab-Palestinian female engagement in sports. In our analyses of the RCT, as per our pre-analysis plan, we control for gender and location, which is perfectly predictive of ethnicity.
5.2 General survey and fusion design
Each year we also carried out a general cross-sectional survey (Table 3) with participants from the broader population of program sites from which the RCT sites were selected. The general survey functions as a census of participants at sites with programming comparable to the RCT. The aim of the general survey was to assess how program exposure relates to outcomes beyond the first year, and it uses largely the same measures as the RCT surveys. The larger sample size also gave us information on heterogeneity across Arab-Palestinian and Jewish-Israeli youth.
We “fuse” the RCT with observational data to estimate longer-term treatment effects, using a machine-learning method (random forest, Breiman, Reference Breiman2001). Our approach is most similar to that of Athey et al. (Reference Athey, Chetty and Imbens2020) and Imbens et al. (Reference Imbens, Kallus, Mao and Wang2022), who show how to combine RCT and observational samples to estimate effects that extend beyond the timeline of the RCT. The basic idea is to model the long-term outcomes in the observational data and then use that model to impute long-term outcomes for the RCT subjects. Athey et al. (Reference Athey, Chetty and Imbens2020) and Imbens et al. (Reference Imbens, Kallus, Mao and Wang2022) assume that the observational data have a panel structure, such that both short- and long-term outcomes are measured for each subject. Our situation is more restrictive in that our observational dataset consists of repeated cross sections. Thus, we cannot impute long-term outcomes for our RCT subjects using a model that relates outcome variables to each other over time. Rather, our imputations rely only on covariates. This is the key difference between our approach and that of Athey et al. (Reference Athey, Chetty and Imbens2020) and Imbens et al. (Reference Imbens, Kallus, Mao and Wang2022). We use a flexible random forest model as the basis of the imputations. As we describe below, we use a conservative bounding approach to address possibly endogenous attrition in the observational data. Our approach could be helpful for civil-society programs that lack the resources to collect longitudinal data, although we note that the conditions for causal identification are stronger than Athey et al. (Reference Athey, Chetty and Imbens2020) and Imbens et al. (Reference Imbens, Kallus, Mao and Wang2022) since our observational data do not contain direct information on how individuals’ outcomes evolve over time.
The longer-term effect that we target is the effect of continued participation in the program on individual's outcome trajectories as they progress through 10, 11, and 12 years of age. If youths remain in the program, then for each year they get older, they also accumulate another year in the program. In the RCT, we have youths of mixed ages that are either exposed to the program for one year or not at all; in the general survey, we have youths of mixed ages that are all exposed to the program, some for one year, others for two or three years. The fusion combines the information from the general survey and the RCT to make predictions about what would happen to RCT participants if they stayed in the program from the time they are 10 to the time they are 12, as compared to how their outcomes would evolve during those years without being in the program. We focus attention on 10–12 year olds because they are the majority participants in our RCT sample.Footnote 3 We use the covariate profile of the ten year olds to define a reference population. The fusion analysis was also specified in our pre-analysis plan.
More formally, let Y p(t) denote an individual's outcome p periods since the onset of treatment eligibility given assignment to treatment condition t. The difference in outcome trends while under the program as compared to never being in the program is given by the following sequence:
where the expectations (E[ ⋅ ]) are with respect to our reference population (the ten-year-old RCT participants). Age and time in the program are perfectly collinear here, and so time-in-program and age-specific program effects cannot be disentangled. In the Appendix (Section 5), we show that there is no appreciable interaction effect with age in the RCT, which provides indirect evidence that the fusion analysis is picking up on effects from accumulated exposure. In any case, the analysis is meaningful in characterizing how trends would differ depending on whether a person participated in the program from age 10 to 12.
A model of the data-generating process (DGP) and identifying assumptions are needed to bring all of this information together to identify the trend effect. Figure 2 illustrates the DGP that justifies our approach. The variables X and U denote observed and unobserved, respectively, background characteristics of individuals. The variable T is for whether a person is assigned to treatment or control. The variables Y 1 and Y 2 are outcomes in a first period and a subsequent period, respectively, and S is an indicator for whether an individual stays in the program in the subsequent period. We use Y 2 and S to characterize potential attrition biases, as described below.
The first assumption that is implicit under this graph is that the treatment operates the same way regardless of whether the site is an RCT site or a general program site. That is, we assume that an RCT participant's potential outcome under treatment would have been the same if, counterfactually, their site was just part of the general program. All of our control observations come from RCT sites. The second assumption from the graph is that selection into an RCT site depends, systematically, only on observed characteristics (X). We justify these assumptions from the fact that RCT sites were selected for incidental reasons: they were sites where new cohorts were already planned on the basis of considerations that had been used to initiate programming more generally. At the same time, given the clustered nature of the selection into an RCT site (even though treatment assignment within RCT sites was mostly at the individual level), we expect some incidental imbalances in background characteristics across RCT versus general program sites. Hence, we control for background characteristics, which we chose based on past research and the suggestions of the implementing organization. These covariates are the program year and individuals’ gender, ethnicity, region of residence, religiosity, parents’ location of birth, and parents’ occupations.
Another key assumption is that, conditional on X, individuals in the different RCT control group cohorts can be used to construct the control trend (E[Y 1(0)|Age = 10], E[Y 2(0)|Age = 11], E[Y 3(0)|Age = 12]). Consider the ten year olds that are in the control group in year one. Ideally we would observe their outcomes under control in years two and three to construct their cohort-specific control trend. However, we agreed with our implementation partner that individuals in the control group would have the opportunity to participate in subsequent years. Thus, we assume that, conditional on covariates, we can estimate this cohort-specific control trend by using outcomes from the 11-year-old control group members in year two, and 12-year-old control group members in year three.
Given these assumptions, for year one outcomes, we have that (Y 1(1), Y 1(0))⊥T| X, in which case,
where E[Y 1| T = 1, Age = 10, X = x] can be estimated from the combined RCT treatment group and general survey data, E[Y 1| T = 0, Age = 10, X = x] can be estimated from the RCT control group data, and the covariate distribution P(x|Age = 10) is derived from the RCT treatment and control group data.
Estimating effects beyond one year are complicated by the fact that some individuals drop out of the program each year, and this could occur for reasons that also affect outcomes in subsequent years. Figure 2 illustrates this possibility for outcomes in year two. (The same logic would apply for outcomes in year three.) We observe second-year-treated outcomes conditional on S, and so if we apply the method that we used for year one outcomes for year two outcomes, we would be estimating:
where the outcomes for the “no drop-out” types E[Y 2(1)|Age = 11, S = 1] could differ from the overall mean E[Y 2(1)|Age = 11] because S is a “collider” variable (Elwert and Winship, Reference Elwert and Winship2014). Now, if drop-out is endogenous only to T and X, then the approach that we used for year one outcomes would also work for year two (and year three) results, because the conditioning on T and X would remove any confounding. In Figure 2, this would imply that the gray-dashed arrows are not active, and our point estimates would be valid. However, if drop-out is endogenous to Y 1 or U as well as T and X (i.e., the gray-dashed arrows are active), then our approach would not identify the relevant effects. Because our general survey data are repeated cross sections (administrative constraints and privacy concerns made a panel survey impossible), we cannot control for any Y 1 variables. Even if we could, there may be unmeasured outcomes or background characteristics that confound the analysis.
From our discussions with program administrators, most of the year-to-year dropout was attributed to exogenous logistical factors, rather than participants deciding to leave because of a bad experience. Nonetheless, to be fully transparent, we use the trimming bounds of Lee (Reference Lee2009) to characterize “worst-case” consequences of endogenous attrition. We focus on an effect that pertains to “no drop-out” types in each period. Ideally, to estimate an effect for such “no drop-out” types, we would use an estimate of E[Y 2(0)|Age = 10, S = 1]. However, what we can estimate with our RCT control group data is
which we can see is based on a mixture of individuals for which S would be one or zero were it that they, counterfactually, were in the general program. Considering that we do not have outcome data for survey participants that dropped out of the program after year one, what we can do is to estimate Lee bounds on E[Y 2(0)|Age = 11, S = 1] by trimming the upper and lower tails of the control group distribution by the retention rate P(S = 1|Age = 11). We estimate this rate using the general survey data. We also follow Lee (Reference Lee2009) in using covariates to tighten the bounds. The details are explained in the Appendix (Section 12).
5.3 Outcome measures
Corresponding to our analytical framework, we measure the effects of the intervention on outgroup regard and ingroup regulation. We operationalize them with several indicators, each consisting of batteries of items (Table 4). We use youth-appropriate and validated measures of outgroup regard, as well as create new ingroup regulation measures building on the existing literature: tendency for ingroup censuring, ingroup policing, peer persuasion, and perspective sharing. Cross-sectional survey questions are largely identical to RCT questions, although they also included descriptive measures capturing program experience and additional operationalizations of our outcomes. Survey measures were identical for all participants irrespective of their ethnic membership with the exception of the measure of perspective sharing tendencies, since the perspective each group might share differs.
Given the contentiousness of the political situation and the fact that all participants in our sample are minors, measuring actual behavior on our outcomes of interest is particularly challenging. Our measures thus capture self-reported behavioral intentions as proxies for the actual behavior. We form the outgroup regard index using principal component analysis because we consider the seven different indicators to be different operationalizations of the same underlying construct. Our approach for the ingroup regulation index is different, because we are interested in a series of qualitatively different strategies that participants can employ. As such, the index of Ingroup Regulation is formed as a sum of Z-scores, treating all indicators equally.
6. Data analysis
To test the hypotheses about short-term effects with the RCT data, we estimate intention to treat (ITT) effects using a linear regression specification as per our pre-analysis plan (see Appendix, Section 5). To estimate effects of multiyear exposure with the fusion design, we carry out the following steps:
(1) Use the RCT control group to model control outcome trends E[Y p|T = 0, Age = a, D = 0, X = x] for (p, a) ∈ {(1, 10), (2, 11), (3, 12)} and the covariates (x) listed above (year, gender, ethnicity, region of residence, religiosity, parents’ location of birth, and parents’ occupations).Footnote 4 Modeling these outcome trends improves precision relative to just using the age-specific averages from the control group data, and also allows us to predict control trends for the same fixed reference population that we used for predicting treatment trends.
(2) Use the RCT treatment group and survey data to model outcome trends E[Y p|T = 1, Age = a, D = 0, X = x] for (p, a) ∈ {(1, 10), (2, 11), (3, 12)} and the same covariates, together with an indicator for whether the observation is from the RCT or survey.
(3) Construct the weighted averages (the $\int ({\cdot}) dP( x \mid {\rm Age} = 10)$ operation) for the reference population: ten-year-old RCT participants. For each ten-year-old RCT participant, we take their covariates and generate a prediction for their expected outcome under treatment and control at age 10, 11, and 12. We take the average of all of these predictions to obtain the estimated trajectory for “All” participants. We take the averages of predictions for the Arab-Palestinian girls, Jewish-Israeli boys, and Jewish-Israeli girls to get their respective group-specific estimated trends.
(4) Construct the Lee (Reference Lee2009) trimming bounds based on an estimates of P(S = 1|Age = 10, X = x).
(5) Bootstrap the entire process to obtain the confidence intervals. (The interpretation of the individual bootstrap here is that we are accounting for those who join the program being a sample from a broader population of youth who might participate each year.)
We use a flexible random forest model based on the bartMachine package in R (Kapelner and Bleich, Reference Kapelner and Bleich2016) to estimate the E[Y p|T = t, Age = a, D = 0, X = x] quantities. Our data are too sparse to do this by using a model-free, matching approach. This particular non-parametric modeling approach has proven highly effective in recent applied work, including in political science (Hill, Reference Hill2011; Green and Kern, Reference Green and Kern2012; Kern et al., Reference Kern, Stuart, Hill and Green2016; Samii et al., Reference Samii, Paler and Daly2017). As Athey et al. (Reference Athey, Tibshirani and Wager2019) show, the random forest model can be understood as deriving predictions as kernel-weighted averages, where the kernel weights are optimized so as to capture potentially complex functional relationships between covariates and outcomes without overfitting.
7. Results
In this section, we test and discuss our hypotheses about the program's impact on outgroup regard (H1) and on ingroup regulation (H2) separately, after which we integrate findings in the conclusion. For each of the two outcomes, we test the impact of the program in a one-year RCT (hypotheses: H1a, H2a),Footnote 5 after which we proceed by testing the impact of a multiyear exposure to the program (H1b, H2b) across all participants and then subgroups with our fusion analysis. We adjust for multiple comparisons controlling for false-discovery rate following Benjamini and Hochberg procedure (Benjamini and Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995), and report both adjusted and unadjusted results. Prior to presenting the results, we describe why participants join the program and how they experience it.
7.1 Descriptive results
Annual surveys show that respondents enjoyed participating in the program, ranking their interest in the sports activity, experiences with coaches, and the integrated practices very highly (Figure 3). The program also appears to fulfill an important condition for optimal intergroup contact: friendship potential. In total, 63 percent of participants made an outgroup friend in the program, a remarkably high number in a deeply segregated context. Friendship formation is significantly positively associated with the number of years in the program across both groups, with the effect being particularly strong for Jewish-Israeli participants. These positive descriptive results give us confidence that the program is implemented well.
7.2 Outgroup regard: RCT results
Hypothesis H1a stipulates that participating in a one-year intergroup contact program within a conflict setting increases outgroup regard. In Table 5, we present the results from the ITT analysis for all outgroup regard outcomes, pooling the data across members of both ethnic groups and controlling for covariates as specified in Section 6. With pooled data, we find no evidence to affirm a one-year effect on outgroup regard, with all adjusted p-values greater than 0.10.
7.3 Outgroup regard: fusion results
Hypothesis 1b stipulates that multiyear exposure to the program increases outgroup regard. We used social distance as our main indicator of outgroup regard in the fusion analysis, as it appears in both the RCT and the survey data. Results are presented in Figure 4. The graphs show point estimates for under treatment and control along with 95 percent bootstrap confidence intervals for these estimates. If drop-out is endogenous only to T and X, then the point estimates for years two and three are valid. If drop-out is endogenous also to previous period outcomes or unobservables, we use the trimming bounds to characterize the full range of possible point estimates for the latent “no drop-out” types in the control group in each period. The gray-shaded area shows the trimming bounds. The width of the bars depends on the drop-out rates after each year, which determines how much of outcome variable distribution will be trimmed to form the bounds. We can assess whether estimated effects are robust to program attrition by checking whether the point estimate for the treatment group is outside the gray-shaded area.
The figures show that the fusion analysis replicates the null effects for social distance that we observe in the RCT after the first year of the program. The fusion point estimates show a positive, though imprecisely estimated effect for participants who took part in the program for two (β = 0.98, S.E. = 0.95, p = 0.29) and three years (β = 1.8, S.E. = 1.09, p = 0.1). The positive effects were strongest for Jewish-Israeli boys who were part of the program for three years (β = 1.99, S.E. = 1.2, p = 0.08).
7.4 Outgroup regard: discussion
With our RCT results, we cannot affirm the hypothesis that the program increases outgroup regard in participants’ first year. The fusion analyses suggest modest but positive effects after second and third years of participation for Jewish participants, with results that cannot be explained away by worst-case endogenous drop-out for boys. To further explore if the intensity of contact experience mattered, we also analyzed the results for a subgroup of survey respondents who participated in an interethnic league throughout the year (Table 6). These tend to be the most talented players, with the most intensive program experience. Figure (b) shows that these players indeed report significantly more positive social distance scores (t = 12.44, p < 0.01) compared to the rest of participants. These descriptive results corroborate the conclusion from the fusion results: multiyear, repeated exposure to the outgroup is needed for intergroup contact to have a positive, prejudice-reducing impact in conflict-ridden societies.
7.5 Ingroup peer regulation: RCT results
Hypothesis H2a stipulates that participating in a one-year intergroup contact program within a conflict setting increases one's engagement in ingroup regulation. In Table 7, we present the ingroup regulation results from the ITT analysis pooling across ethnic groups and controlling for covariates as specified within Section 6. With pooled data, we find no evidence of a one-year effect on inclination to regulate ingroup members for aggressions toward the outgroup.
7.6 Ingroup peer regulation: fusion results
We use the fusion analysis to test hypothesis 2b about multiyear exposure and hypothesis 2c about moderation by ethnicity. We find some significant results for perspective sharing tendencies, but not for ingroup censuring nor ingroup policing. Figure 5 presents the perspective sharing results (for null results for ingroup censuring and ingroup policing, see Appendix 10). Unlike for social distance, we did not assess perspective sharing with precisely the same instruments in the RCT and in the survey. Instead, for the fusion we selected a survey question that asks how often participants defended the outgroup's perspective in discussions with ingroup members. As for the social distance outcome, in year one the results mostly replicate the null effect from the RCT. The fusion point estimates show a positive effect for participants who took part in the program for two (β = 0.41, S.E. = 0.31, p = 0.19) and three years (β = 0.73, S.E. = 0.32, p = 0.02). The positive effects were strongest for Jewish-Israeli boys who were part of the program for three years (β = 1.06, S.E. = 0.36, p < 0.01).
7.7 Ingroup peer regulation: discussion
The RCT cannot affirm our hypothesis that the program increases ingroup regulation in participants’ first year. But the fusion analyses suggest modest but positive effects for perspective sharing after second and third years of participation for Jewish participants, with results that cannot be explained away by worst-case endogenous drop-out for boys. As for social distance, we again explored results for league participants. Figure 6 shows that these players indeed report significantly more perspective sharing than their non-league peers. Again, this descriptive evidence corroborates the argument that the intensity of exposure to the outgroup matters.
8. General discussion and conclusion
We set out to study how combining sports and intergroup contact between youths might contribute to peace in the context of the ongoing violent conflict in Israel. We formulated six hypotheses about the impact of one-year and multiyear intergroup contact on outgroup regard and ingroup regulation, and of the heterogeneity by ethnic group. We find no significant effects of one-year contact. The analysis of multiyear contact suggests that the effects of the program on outgroup regard and perspective sharing become more pronounced over time in a way that clearly distinguishes Jewish-Israeli from Arab-Palestinian participants. To our surprise, the fusion results suggest heterogeneity beyond ethnicity—by gender. These results raise interesting questions about how the effects of contact interventions may depend on one's understanding of one's own ethnicity, as filtered through the experience of one's other group memberships (Ghavami et al., Reference Ghavami, Kogachi and Graham2020). While an association between intergroup contact and prejudice reduction appears to have been well-established in the survey and lab-based literature, our field-experimental evidence is quite mixed, in a manner that is similar to other field-experimental work in conflict settings by Scacco and Warren (Reference Scacco and Warren2018) and Mousa (Reference Mousa2020). Designing effective interventions in conflict settings on the basis of the logic of contact theory is neither trivial nor certain to generate consistently strong effects. Research in field settings is the best way to learn more about this challenge.
Two key insights emerge from our research for the design of contact interventions. First, for intergroup contact in a conflict setting to have positive benefits, one year (with an average of eight sessions of playing in ethnically mixed teams) may not be enough. The results of our fusion analysis—which rely on critical assumptions that we believe are plausible in our context but may not always be met—suggest modest yet positive effects associated with two to three years of program exposure. Positive results for participants who took part in the more intensive league program provide further suggestive evidence of the importance of multiyear exposure. This insight is consistent with recent scholarship proposing that people have to receive a certain dosage of intergroup contact for it to yield stronger positive effects. The findings on length of exposure raise two questions that ought to be the focus of further research. First, what dosage of contact is minimally needed and how may that differ for different groups of participants? This question is crucial for practitioners who want to set up intergroup contact programs in conflict settings: if they fail to reach the threshold, they may do more harm than good. Second, what exactly happens in high-intensity contact that ultimately brings about the positive effects? MacInnis and Page-Gould (Reference MacInnis and Page-Gould2015) propose several processes; the one most consistent with our data is that more intense exposure to the outgroup has greater potential for bonding and friendship formation. Future research should more systematically test the processes that occur during high-intensity intergroup contact over time.
The second key insight from our research is that even a well-designed and well-received intergroup contact program may not achieve positive results for all groups. Initially insignificant program effects start intensifying more clearly over time for Jewish-Israeli participants, and in particular, Jewish-Israeli boys. In contrast, we never observe any impact of the program on Arab-Palestinian participants (in our sample, all girls). This may not be surprising in light of recent scholarship questioning the benefits of joint social activities for members of minority groups (Hässler et al., Reference Hässler, Ullrich, Sebben, Shnabel, Bernardino, Valdenegro, Van Laar, González, Visintin and Tropp2022). Programs that want to serve multiple communities may have to structure their program differently (e.g., including dialogues about inequalities, Saguy et al., Reference Saguy, Pratto, Dovidio and Nadler2009) or offer alternative benefits to the minority group (e.g., career opportunities as our partner organization does). Our partner organization believes that these conversations happen over time after trust has been built—results from league participants are consistent with that hypothesis which future research should test.
Finally, we illustrated a strategy for overcoming limitations of many RCTs: small samples and short follow-up times. We combined the RCT with a cross-sectional survey and machine-learning methods, and used a highly agnostic bounds approach to address attrition. This analysis, contingent upon several critical assumptions, provides a template for those seeking to capitalize on opportunities to study innovative interventions in the field. It allows us to find that intergroup contact within a conflict setting is most successful when it offers a multiyear, intensive experience that takes group asymmetries into account. This demonstrates that—even after 70 years of research on the topic—the design, implementation, and evaluation of intergroup contact interventions in conflict-ridden society remain challenging.
Supplementary material
The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2024.8.
To obtain replication material for this article, https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/M3DXXJ