Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T16:01:47.527Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Midfrontal theta reactivity to conflict and error are linked to externalizing and internalizing respectively

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 April 2024

Phoebe S.-H. Neo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Otago, New Zealand
Shabah M. Shadli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Otago, New Zealand
Neil McNaughton
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Otago, New Zealand
Martin Sellbom*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Otago, New Zealand
*
Corresponding author: Martin Sellbom; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Dimensional psychopathology scores measure symptom severity; cutting across disorder categories. Their clinical utility is high given comorbidity, but their neural basis is unclear. We used scalp electroencephalography (EEG) to concurrently assess neural activity across internalizing and externalizing traits. “Theta rhythm” (4–7 Hz) spectral power at the frontal midline site Fz in specific goal conflict and action error phases within a trial of a Stop-Signal Task was extracted using process-specific contrasts. A final sample of 146 community participants (63 males, 83 females; mean age = 36; SD = 9; range = 18 – 56), oversampled for externalizing disorder (49% diagnosed with a DSM-5 externalizing disorder), also supplied psychopathology and personality data. We used the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory−3 (MMPI-3) to measure symptoms and traits of psychopathology. An MMPI-3 measure of the higher-order internalizing psychopathology spectrum was positively correlated with action error theta. An MMPI-3 measure of the higher-order spectrum of externalizing psychopathology was negatively correlated with goal-conflict theta. We showed that goal-conflict and error theta activity are higher-order processes that index psychopathology severity. The associations extend into the nominally healthy range, and so reflect theta-related factors that apply to the general population as well as patients with sub-threshold diagnoses.

Type
Empirical Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

The current Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) lists ∼300 categories of psychiatric disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). While clinically useful, its nosology does not account for comorbidity and transdiagnostic features (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2022; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Kotov, Watson, Forbes, Eaton and Bach2018). Hierarchically-structured dimensional models of psychopathology are increasingly seen as important targets for the study of neural mechanisms that are common across psychiatric disorders (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger and Watson2018; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021).

Analysis of co-occurring psychiatric diagnoses (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Ormel, Petukhova, McLaughlin, Green, Russo and Alonso2011; Krueger, Reference Krueger1999) extracts two higher-order spectra of covariance cutting across DSM-5 categories (often with a third spectrum of “thought disorder,” Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017). “Internalizing” spans mood and anxiety disorders (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Ormel, Petukhova, McLaughlin, Green, Russo and Alonso2011; Krueger, Reference Krueger1999) such as major depression, generalized anxiety, social anxiety, and panic disorder. Internalizing correlates with a broad range of clinical and personality trait dimensions that reflect dysfunctional, high negative, and/or low positive, affect (Naragon-Gainey et al., Reference Naragon-Gainey, McMahon and Park2018; Sellbom et al., Reference Sellbom, Ben-Porath and Bagby2008). “Externalizing” spans disorders of disinhibition and socially deviant behavior (Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Hobbs, Conway, Dick, Dretsch, Eaton and Latzman2021) such as attention deficit hyperactivity, antisocial personality, and alcohol and substance use. Externalizing correlates with a broad range of clinical and trait dimensions that reflect poor impulse control, and social and behavioral antagonism (Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Hobbs, Conway, Dick, Dretsch, Eaton and Latzman2021; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Markon, Patrick, Benning and Kramer2007).

Currently, neural studies using dimensional measures of the higher-order domains of internalizing and externalizing are limited (Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2011; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021). Existing work has largely examined categorical differences between those with a disorder and healthy controls (e.g., McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Gyurkovics, Palmer and Makeig2021; Pasion & Barbosa, Reference Pasion and Barbosa2019). A recent review of the extant literature identified midfrontal theta activity (4–7 Hz) as providing important indices of cognitive vulnerabilities across psychiatric disorders that form not only part of the internalizing (anxiety, OCD) but also externalizing (ADHD and substance abuse) spectrum (McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Gyurkovics, Palmer and Makeig2021). It should be noted that the theta indices reviewed (McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Gyurkovics, Palmer and Makeig2021) spanned different study tasks and conditions, and also included non-oscillatory evoked potentials (e.g., the N2 and error-related-negativity, ERN) that have been associated with phase resetting of ongoing rhythmic theta activity (Cavanagh et al., Reference Cavanagh, Zambrano-Vazquez and Allen2012; Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, Bogacz, Holroyd, Nieuwenhuis and Cohen2007). So, “midfrontal theta” measurements are functionally heterogeneous, and different mechanisms (Cohen, Reference Cohen2014) likely mediate the theta associations of different forms of psychopathology.

Theta rhythms increase in amplitude with signals of novelty (Harper et al., Reference Harper, Malone and Iacono2017), negative feedback (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Watts, Schmidt and Bernat2018), conflict (Cohen & Donner, Reference Cohen and Donner2013; Neo et al., Reference Neo, Thurlow and McNaughton2011; Pinner & Cavanagh, Reference Pinner and Cavanagh2017) and error (Cohen, Reference Cohen2011; Pasion & Barbosa, Reference Pasion and Barbosa2019; Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, Botvinick and Cohen2004; Zavala et al., Reference Zavala, Tan, Ashkan, Foltynie, Limousin, Zrinzo and Brown2016). These different activations may reflect a fundamental control process that is generally required when internal or external goals are changing, and require attention and monitoring (Cohen, Reference Cohen2014; McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Gyurkovics, Palmer and Makeig2021). Of particular relevance here, it has been shown that midfrontal theta activity is hyper-reactive (heightened attention) in internalizing (Cavanagh & Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2015; Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Klawohn and Meyer2019); and hypo-reactive (poor monitoring) in externalizing (Burwell et al., Reference Burwell, Malone, Bernat and Iacono2014; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Bernat and Patrick2007; Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Malone, Burwell, Bernat and Iacono2013).

This opposite relation of midfrontal theta activity to internalizing versus externalizing has not yet been assessed within the same sample. Studies have been restricted to either internalizing or externalizing psychopathology. Does a single form of theta activation by a single event exert opposite impacts on internalizing and externalizing; and, if so, how? Theoretically, collateral input to different areas would make this possible. For example, increases in dopamine can have opposite effects on reward and punishment processing (Jean-Richard-Dit-Bressel et al., Reference Jean-Richard-Dit-Bressel, Killcross and McNally2018). Also, if this is the case, do theta reactivities to different events show similar opposing links with internalizing and externalizing?

To address these questions, in the current study, we parsed theta activity in the Stop-Signal Task (SST, Logan et al., Reference Logan, Cowan and Davis1984) with different statistical contrasts applied to distinct periods within a trial to extract midfrontal theta activity specific to “conflict,” and “error.” We then tested both the conflict and the error associations with dimensional measures of both internalizing and externalizing.

In the SST, on GO trials participants make a mouse click as quickly as they can to a go-signal. Occasionally, STOP trials occur when a stop signal occurs after the go-signal; and participants should inhibit their mouse click. Both conflict and error are assessed by first subtracting power differences between STOP and GO trials and then applying different contrasts applied at different time points.

For conflict, measurements are made locked to the timing of the stop signal in STOP trials. The stop signal is presented with short, medium, and long delays, which deliver low, high (stop and go are similar), and low conflict conditions, respectively. Contrasting high with the average of low conflict generates an index of theta activity specific to conflict described in the neuropsychological theory of the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000). Here, the conflict is a result of the competing goals of quick responding and its inhibition, postulated to peak in trials with medium stop-signal delays. SST studies have shown such conflict activity at the midfrontal site Fz (McNaughton et al., Reference McNaughton, Swart, Neo, Bates and Glue2013; Neo et al., Reference Neo, Thurlow and McNaughton2011).

For error, measurements are made locked to the timing of the mouse click. In the SST, an error occurs when a participant fails to stop clicking on a STOP trial. So, on STOP trials, a click represents a mistake, in contrast to a click in GO trials. To extract processing specific to error, we contrasted failed STOP with successful GO trials, ignoring stop-signal delay. The majority of the EEG studies on error processing have focused on the ERN (Pasion & Barbosa, Reference Pasion and Barbosa2019). As mentioned earlier, the ERN is a response-locked potential, but is also associated with midfrontal theta rhythmic activity (Cavanagh et al., Reference Cavanagh, Zambrano-Vazquez and Allen2012; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Stern and Gehring2007; Yeung et al., Reference Yeung, Bogacz, Holroyd, Nieuwenhuis and Cohen2007).

Based on the literature just reviewed, which suggests hyper- and hypo-midfrontal reactivity in a range of internalizing and externalizing disorders, we hypothesized that both conflict and error theta activity at the midfrontal site Fz, would be positively correlated with the higher-order domain measure of internalizing; and negatively correlated with externalizing. Given the positive correlation between internalizing and externalizing (Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2006), we also predicted that the hypothesized relationships would be mediated by non-shared factors, that is, via theta variances specific to internalizing and externalizing respectively.

1. Methods and materials

1.1. Participants

The data were extracted from an archival database collected as part of a larger study on externalizing disorders and have not been previously published. Participants were recruited from the community with Facebook advertisements and flyers designed to target a sample over-weighted towards externalizing psychopathology. In two separate advertisements, we advertised for 1) individuals to take part in a brain and personality study; and 2) for individuals with drug, alcohol, or anger management problems – or with ADHD or impulsivity. After exclusion (see section on exclusion below), the final sample size consisted of 146 participants (63 males, 83 females; mean age = 36; SD = 9; range = 18 – 56). Structured clinical interviews (First et al., Reference First, Williams, Karg and Spitzer2015, Reference First, Williams, Benjamin and Spitzer2016) found 49% of the final sample met criteria for at least one DSM-5 externalizing diagnosis: ADHD (any type, 27%); history of conduct disorder (23%); antisocial personality disorder (21%); alcohol use disorder (18%); and cannabis use disorder (12%). These rates are substantially higher than a typical community sample (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Ormel, Petukhova, McLaughlin, Green, Russo and Alonso2011), consistent with overweighting towards externalizing problems during recruitment. The remaining half of the sample included healthy controls for externalizing disorders. Ethical approval was provided by the University of Otago Ethics Committee (Health), Approval number: H16/031.

1.2. Personality and psychopathology measures

The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory−3 (MMPI-3) (Ben-Porath & Tellegen, Reference Ben-Porath and Tellegen2020) measures maladaptive symptoms, behaviors, and personality traits in accordance with contemporary theories of psychopathology and personality (Sellbom et al., Reference Sellbom, Kremyar and Wygant2021); and is aligned with modern dimensional models of psychopathology (Simms et al., Reference Simms, Wright, Cicero, Kotov, Mullins-Sweatt, Sellbom and Zimmermann2021). It is designed for clinical assessment and has representative general community norms. It includes 335 true/false items that aggregate onto 10 validity scales and 42 substantive content scales that consist of Higher-Order (HO) scales; Restructured Clinical (RC) scales; Specific Problems (SP) scales; and the PSY-5 scales. The HO, RC and SP scales are hierarchically organized with item overlap of scales across (but not within) each level. The HO scales of Emotional/Internalizing Dysfunction (EID) and Behavioral/Externalizing Dysfunction (BXD) represent the highest order and therefore, most generalized measurements of internalizing and externalizing syndromes. EID and BXD form the primary measures of internalizing and externalizing in our hypotheses testing.

To facilitate comparisons with existing work on the associations between personality and psychopathology, we also used the Behavioral Inhibition (BIS) and Behavioral Activation System (BAS) scales from Carver and White’s (Carver & White, Reference Carver and White1994) BIS/BAS measure. The BIS and BAS scales are widely used as self-report measures of neurobiological systems that mediate personality traits in the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) (Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton and Corr2008; Krupić et al., Reference Krupić, Corr, Ručević, Križanić and Gračanin2016). Consistent with the dimensional models of psychopathology, in the RST, personality traits, when manifested at extreme levels, are significant contributors to psychopathology. Neural BIS contributes to internalizing; and neural BAS contributes to externalizing (Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2014). The BIS/BAS scales are administered in a single questionnaire that consists of 20 items where participants rate on a 1–4 scale the extent that they agree with each statement/item. The BAS consists of three sub-scales: Drive, Fun Seeking and Reward responsiveness. To reduce multiple testing, we used the total score of the sub-scales here. Although the MMPI-3 inventory also includes scales of abnormal personality, that is, the PSY-5 scales (Harkness et al., Reference Harkness, Finn, McNulty and Shields2012), like the other scales in the MMPI-3 inventory, they were excluded in our analyses due to overlapping items with EID and BXD.

1.3. The stop-signal task (SST)

The SST was originally designed to assess motor inhibition (Logan et al., Reference Logan, Cowan and Davis1984). Here, we used an SST version, as described in Shadli et al. (Reference Shadli, Glue, McIntosh and McNaughton2015), modified to optimally assess “conflict.” On a GO trial, participants make a left/right mouse click as fast as possible when they see a left/right arrow. As shown in Fig. 1, on a STOP trial, a tone sounds after the onset of the arrow, signaling the task requirement to withhold the mouse click. The tone occurs only occasionally (1 STOP trial counterbalanced amongst 3 GO trials). The Stop-Signal Delay (SSD) determines when the tone is presented from the onset of the arrow. As SSD increases, the chances of failing to inhibit the motor response also increase. STOP trials with short, medium, and long delays were distributed across the task using separate “staircases”. Short delay and long delay staircases adjusted the SSDs depending on prior reaction times averaged over recent GO trials. The medium delay staircase SSD was adjusted up or down by 30 ms after correct or failed stopping, respectively (intended to track 50% stopping). There were 99 STOP trials and 296 GO trials in total spread over three blocks with rest breaks in between. Prior to the test trials, participants completed 30 practice GO trials with no STOP trials.

Figure 1. Sequence of events in a trial. Onset of the tone from the time of the arrow presentation (SSDs) in a Stop trial are variable. A Go trial follows the same event sequence but without the onset of the tone. A smiley/frowny face is presented for a successful/unsuccessful withholding of a mouse click in a Stop trial. In a Go trial, a smiley/frowny face is presented for correct/incorrect responses. ms: milliseconds. ITI: 500 ms to 4000 ms; SSDs: see Table 1; A FI: Go Correct = 1000 ms; Stop Fail = 1500 ms; Stop Correct = 1700 ms.

1.4. EEG acquisition and processing

EEG recording was referenced to CPz and sampled at 512 Hz. EEG was recorded from FP1, FPz, FP2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, T7, T8 C3, Cz, C4, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, M1 and M2 with an Advanced Neuro Technology (ANT) amplifier and ANT caps with AgCl electrodes. Impedances were tested with ANT software (eego) and reduced to below 10 KΩ. Data from 30 participants were sampled at 256 Hz. Given that the analysis was of power averaged over 4–7 Hz, and since this is well below 256 Hz, these were analyzed as sampled. The MATLAB 2019a plugin, EEGlab (version 2019_0), was used to process the data. The EEG of all 18 recording sensors was re-referenced to the mastoid sensors (M1 and M2); filtered from 1 to 36 Hz using the function (pop_ eegfiltnew); and segmented from the start of the first to the end of the last test trial. We used the runamica15() function to run Independent Component Analysis to identify eye blink and movement components. The eye components were removed automatically using the pop_icflag() function. A 90% similarity threshold was used, that is, only components with > 90% similarity to eye components were removed. Next, two 1 5 s EEG segments centered, respectively, at the onset of the Stop-Signal and mouse click were separately extracted from each trial. Any segment with EEG > ±70 µV detected in any of the sensors was excluded from analyses using the function (pop_eegthresh). Two consecutive 1 s Hanning windows, overlapping by 0·5 s, were applied to each 1 5 s segment, delivering two 1 s epochs. The Hanning window is a cosine function that reduces the weighting of the edge data within an epoch. (Note that the overlapping and windowing procedures delivered, in the next step, spectral activity centered on the 0·5 s after the onset of the Stop-signal and mouse click respectively.) The fft() function was used to extract spectral amplitude from each 1 s epoch; squared to derive spectral power; and log transformed to normalize the power distribution. The normalized spectral power for 4–7 Hz was then averaged to yield a single value to index theta activity at each period of interest. Below, we described further steps for deriving the “conflict” and “error” contrasts.

GO trial activity was used as a baseline to control for GO response-related theta. In the SST, all STOP trials began in the same way as a GO trial until the onset of the stop signal, which initiated processing of STOP, superimposed on existing GO processes. So, the STOP-GO difference extracts STOP-specific theta activity removing other general and GO-related processes. For both conflict and error, theta power in the GO trial preceding a STOP trial was subtracted from that STOP trial (power from the following GO trial was subtracted if the preceding trial was also a STOP trial). As the sequence of STOP and GO trials was pre-determined and fixed across participants, the GO trials used for the computation of STOP-GO activity was pre-identified. Post-experiment, an event mark was inserted into these GO trials according to the SSDs of the STOP trial it was paired with. For example, if the SSD for the STOP trial was 20 ms, a unique event mark was inserted 20 ms from the onset of the arrow in the paired GO trial. Spectral power was then extracted from this “Stop-Signal” event mark as per the procedures for STOP trials.

The conflict contrast was computed by averaging these STOP-GO trial differences for early and late SSDs and subtracting this average from the average of trials with medium SSDs. The residual theta power (centered on 500 ms from the onset of the stop signal) indexed inhibition-related reactivity specific to conflict, which should peak in the medium SSD trials because response inhibition was relatively easy or hard in the early- and late-delay trials, respectively, while stopping and going were in balanced conflict in the medium case. As we were interested in trait-like neural responses that are relatively stable over time, we extracted a minimum of 5 trials per SSD trial type from each of the 3 blocks of SST trials. Participants with too few trials from any of the blocks (due to EEG noise) were excluded from the analyses.

The error contrast was computed centered on the 500 ms from the onset of the mouse click (in both STOP and GO trials); and only STOP trials with mouse clicks (failed inhibition) were averaged as were GO trials with mouse clicks. Thus, clicking was consistent and trials only differed in terms of whether the response was incorrect (STOP trial) or correct (GO trial). Participants with fewer than 15 trials in the averages were excluded.

1.5. Participant exclusion

Out of 218 participants, 47 participants were excluded due to excessive EEG artifacts. Compared to our previous work that did not focus on externalizing, a relatively larger percentage of participants were excluded. More of our participants fidgeted throughout the task, generating EEG artifacts, likely due to externalizing (e.g., ADHD) symptoms. This interacted with the requirement here for a minimum number of trials from each block of SST trials for analysis, leading to a larger number of exclusions. A further 25 participants were excluded due to inconsistent or deviant responding, detected with the MMPI-3 validity scales (Ben-Porath & Tellegen, Reference Ben-Porath and Tellegen2020).

1.6. General procedures

Participants took part in the experiment as part of a larger study; they completed a battery of questionnaires that included the ones reported here as sub-sets. While they completed the questionnaires, the experimenter prepared the participants for EEG recording by applying electro-gel and reducing the impedance of the recording sensors attached to the EEG cap. The participants were then tested (∼25 min) on the stop-signal task (Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, Glue, McIntosh and McNaughton2015), followed by the gold bar/lemon task (Neo et al., Reference Neo, McNaughton and Sellbom2021) (∼30 min; data not reported here). The participants were then administered the structured clinical interviews. The experimental session was conducted by a trained research assistant under supervision by a registered clinical psychologist. At the end of the experiment, participants were paid $50 in petrol or supermarket vouchers, and cash winnings ($6) from the gold bar/lemon task. The whole experimental session lasted 3 5 – 4 h.

1.7. Data analysis

To facilitate comparison with published SST studies, we computed the descriptive statistics for the behavioral measures commonly reported in the SST (Table 1). Our data came from an archival database and so participants with internalizing problems were not specifically targeted during recruitment. However, externalizing and internalizing problems are moderately correlated (Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2006). To ascertain that our sample, recruited for overweighting in externalizing symptoms, showed the appropriate range in scores for both externalizing and internalizing, we calculated the descriptive statistics of the self-report measures (Table 2).

To assess if midfrontal (Fz) conflict and error were associated positively/negatively with dimensional measures of internalizing/externalizing and personality traits implicated in psychopathology, we calculated the Pearson product-moment correlations for each of our theta measures with EID/BXD and BIS/BAS scales respectively (see Table 2). To index midfrontal activity, we used Fz and the mean of 4–7 Hz theta activity. These choices, including the spectral power extraction methods, were made so as to replicate the methods used in previous SST work (McNaughton et al., Reference McNaughton, Swart, Neo, Bates and Glue2013; Neo et al., Reference Neo, Thurlow and McNaughton2011; Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, Glue, McIntosh and McNaughton2015) on goal conflict, defined by the theoretical BIS (Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000). Although other sensors, frequencies and spectral power extraction methods could potentially generate stronger results, keeping these unchanged here facilitates comparison with the previous work. More importantly, the usual way of identifying optimal sensors and frequencies with a spatial map of mean spectral power across electrodes was problematic for our current purpose. Our aim was to identify neural activity that varies with individual differences in psychopathology dimensions. Large mean activity in sensors/frequencies is not equivalent to variability across individuals. Therefore, we anchored our methods of analysis to previous SST versions set within the theoretical framework of BIS which is compatible with the dimensional perspective of psychopathology (for details on how BIS fits in with the dimensional perspective, see Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton and Corr2008). Our hypotheses on the relationships between theta, internalizing, and externalizing are also anchored to the wider literature on midfrontal theta activity and its contributions to different form of psychopathology.

To assess if midfrontal theta effects were distributed across other sensors, the following steps were repeated for both conflict and error to create an index of distributed theta activity. Firstly, 4–7 Hz STOP-GO theta differences of all 18 active electrodes were entered into a Principal Component Analysis (PCA) to extract a single major component. Next, the eigenvalues of the full model were compared to outputs from a parallel analysis to help us determine the inherent structure of the data, that is, the optimal number of components to extract. Regardless of the results of the parallel analysis, individual regression-weighted scores of the first component in the solution were used to index variation in theta activity distributed across the 18 electrodes. (For a solution with more than 1 factor, the oblique Promax rotation to simple structure was used.) We then calculated the Pearson correlations between the individual factor scores and the self-report measurements listed in our hypotheses (see component 1 results in Table 2). The component loadings of each of the 18 electrodes for conflict and error are shown in Fig. 3.

To assess shared versus unique variances, we entered EID and BXD; and in a separate regression model, BIS and BAS; as predictors of midfrontal theta activity to extract the shared and unique variances. To assess if age could account for any of the results above, we also extracted Pearson correlations between age and each of our dimensional measures of internalizing and externalizing.

To facilitate comparisons with previous work focused on group averages, we plotted the spectral power, averaged across all participants, for conflict and error (see Fig. 3). However, note that it was not appropriate to test the group means for statistical differences as the means were computed from different contrasts reflecting fundamentally different cognitive processes. Our aim here was to help the reader, used to analyses of group averages, gain qualitative insights into the distribution of individual theta contrasts across the two conditions. Conversely, those used to individual self-report data would be interested in the reliability of the measures (Hajcak et al., Reference Hajcak, Meyer and Kotov2017). These are difficult to calculate in this case (see Clayson et al., Reference Clayson, Baldwin and Larson2021, on reliability of difference scores) as they involve complex contrasts (e.g., linear x quadratic over a set of 6 trials). Assessing our type of measure like a standard single, conventional, score has additional problems since: 1) the score varies systematically across trials (Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, Ando, McIntosh, Lodhia, Russell and McNaughton2021) – which would add to the nominal error of a conventional reliability score; and, 2) is not meant to be test-retest reliable (Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, McIntosh and McNaughton2020).

2. Results

The common behavioral measures published in SST studies are shown in Table 1. The current sample showed slower reaction times, and the percentage of successful STOP trials was also lower across all types of trials compared to previous work (Aron & Poldrack, Reference Aron and Poldrack2006; Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, Glue, McIntosh and McNaughton2015). Notably, seven participants failed to inhibit their responses throughout the task. That explains the zero values in Table 1 (see min. values for successful inhibition, row 1–3). With over 80% correct responses in GO trials, these participants did not appear to be ignoring the task requirements. Also, the computation of our contrasts was not dependent on correctly inhibited trials. So, even though these participants had zero trials available for correctly inhibited SST trials, they were included in our EEG analyses.

Table 1. Descriptive values in the current study of behavioral measures commonly reported in the SST

Row 1–3: percentage of successfully inhibited mouse clicks for early, medium and late SSD trials respectively; row 4–6: length of SSD for each trial type; row 7: reaction times in GO trials; row 8: Stop-Signal Reaction Times (mean SSD for medium trials subtracted from median GO reaction times); row 9: percentage of correct responses in GO trials; row 10–11: reaction times and length of SSD for failed STOP trials.

The descriptive statistics of the self-report measures are shown in Table 2. It shows that our sample showed average to above average internalizing scores (see EID) and with even greater variability than is typical of a community sample (Ben-Porath & Tellegen, Reference Ben-Porath and Tellegen2020). It also shows that, as expected given our recruitment strategy, there is a high variability in externalizing scores (see BXD). Table 2 also shows that error theta, whether indexed by Fz or component 1, was positively correlated with EID. Although error theta at Fz showed a positive correlation with BIS, this was not significant after Bonferroni correction for multiple testing. (Given that each unique theta index was tested 4 times, we used a correction factor of 4). Both Fz and component 1 conflict theta were negatively correlated with BXD. Similar negative correlations were observed between conflict theta and BAS. The conflict:error correlation for Fz was r(144)  = +0·145, NS, and their relative involvement in BXD and EID is discussed below (see also Fig. 5).

Table 2. (left)Trait scores and (right) correlations for STOP-GO theta (4–7 Hz average) power against trait measures at midline (Fz) and component 1 at two different time points in the SST (conflict, error)

* Correlation is significant at the 0·05 level (1-tailed).

** Correlation is significant at the 0·01 level (1-tailed).

The head maps in Fig. 2 show how theta activity from each of the 18 electrodes loaded onto component 1 in conflict and error respectively. Results from the parallel analyses suggest that the data structure for conflict and error consisted of one and two components respectively. Therefore, the contribution of each electrode to component 1 for conflict (Fig. 2a) was extracted from a component matrix (unrotated solution) and that for error (Fig. 2b) was extracted from a pattern matrix (rotated solution). Figure 2a shows that component 1 for conflict has the highest loadings in a central frontal cluster of electrodes consisting of F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz and C4. Figure 2b shows that component 1 for error has the highest loadings in a frontal cluster of electrodes consisting of Fp1, Fpz, Fp2, F3, Fz, F4.

Figure 2. Relative contributions of STOP-GO theta activity from each electrode to component 1 at two different time points in the SST (conflict, error). (a) Electrode loadings for conflict from its component matrix. (b) Electrode loadings for error from its pattern matrix. Warmer colors indicate relatively higher loadings. Electrode positions, from left to right: 1st row, Fp1, Fpz, Fp2; 2nd row, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8; 3rd row, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8; 4th row, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8.

Figure 3. Mean spectral power of STOP-GO theta activity (4–7 Hz) at two different time points in the SST and using two different contrasts (conflict, error).

Consistent with previous work (Krueger & Markon, Reference Krueger and Markon2006), EID and BXD were positively correlated r(146) = 0·44, p < 0·001. Age did not show any consistent pattern of correlations with either internalizing or externalizing measures in the current sample. Our regression analyses showed that the significant correlations (after correction for multiple testing) observed in our study (see Table 2) were due to unique variances (see Table 3).

Table 3. R square variances of regressions with trait measures as predictors of STOP-GO theta (4–7 Hz average) power at midline (Fz) and component 1 at two different time points in the SST (conflict, error). A. Regressions with EID and BXD as predictors. B. Regressions with BIS and BAS as predictors

* Variance of regression model or unique contribution is significant at the 0·05 level (1-tailed).

In Fig. 3, we also show the means of conflict and error at the midfrontal site Fz. The mean spectral theta power measures of conflict and error showed distinct characteristics. Conflict activity was of the same order of magnitude as reported previously (Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, Ando, McIntosh, Lodhia, Russell and McNaughton2021, Reference Shadli, Glue, McIntosh and McNaughton2015) with a positive mean (µ = 0·17 log10 μV2). Error activity was close to zero (µ = 0·007 log10 μV2). Notably, standard error for error was much larger compared to conflict (SEM = 0·68). As the contrast used in the two cases is different, statistical comparison of variances is not appropriate. As mentioned in our data analysis section, in contrast to analyses of group averages, our aim was to identify neural activity that varies with individual differences in psychopathology dimensions. So, the interpretation of a small mean in group analyses as a lack of significant activity is less relevant here. These differences in means and variance are also clear in the plots of the overall regression and partial internalizing and externalizing components shown for conflict and error in Fig. 4.

Figure 4. Scatter plots (with regression lines) for overall regression model and internalizing and externalizing partial components for conflict and error. Note that the partial correlation for error with internalizing is positive, while the partial correlation for conflict with externalizing is negative.

To further explore the relation between conflict and error, we entered them as predictors in separate regressions for externalizing and internalizing. In both cases the overall regression was significant (p = 0·033, p = 0·036, respectively). For externalizing, this was primarily due to a unique negative contribution from conflict (part correlation = −0·195) with an apparent slight positive contribution from error (part correlation = 0·064. For internalizing, in contrast, this was primarily due to a unique positive contribution from error (part correlation = 0·202) with an apparent slight negative contribution from conflict (part correlation = −0·036). Scatter plots for these relationships are shown in Fig. 5.

Figure 5. Scatter plots (with regression lines) for overall regression and conflict and error partial components for externalizing and internalizing. Note that the partial correlation for conflict with externalizing is negative while the partial correlation for error with internalizing is positive.

3. Discussion

Midfrontal theta activity reflecting action errors was positively associated with internalizing psychopathology. Its association with the BIS personality scale was also positive but the relationship did not survive correction for multiple testing. Midfrontal theta activity reflecting conflict was negatively associated with both externalizing psychopathology and the BAS personality scale. When STOP-GO theta activity distributed across the frontal, central and posterior regions of the scalp were reduced to extract a major theta component, a central frontal cluster of sites loaded highly on the component for conflict; and a frontal cluster loaded highly on the component for error. Both components showed the same associations as their respective midfrontal theta at Fz displayed with the self-report measures of psychopathology and personality.

These results are consistent with the idea that theta activity indexes important psychiatric vulnerabilities (Cavanagh & Shackman, Reference Cavanagh and Shackman2015; Corr & McNaughton, Reference Corr, McNaughton, Beauchaine and Hinshaw2014; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Gyurkovics, Palmer and Makeig2021). More specifically, given that EID and BXD operationalize higher-order domains that capture common variance cutting across traditional mental disorders, our results suggest that conflict and error theta activities reflect higher-order mechanisms that underly psychopathology. These heterogeneous theta activities appear to share the capacity to support higher-order cognitive functioning that is generally required when internal or external goals are changing or in conflict, and require attention and monitoring (Cohen, Reference Cohen2014; Gray & McNaughton, Reference Gray and McNaughton2000; McLoughlin et al., Reference McLoughlin, Gyurkovics, Palmer and Makeig2021). Additionally, both conflict and error theta activity were quantitatively related to the symptoms, behaviors and traits of psychopathology, extending into the healthy range. This supports the dimensional model of psychopathology (Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2022; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger and Watson2018; Krueger et al., Reference Krueger, Kotov, Watson, Forbes, Eaton and Bach2018; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021), which suggests that there are no clear breaks between health and disorder.

We did not find support for our prediction that a single theta process would contribute to both the internalizing and externalizing domains. This finding is theoretically possible, since the same process could interact (in opposite directions) with a distinct set of co-factors in the development of internalizing and externalizing, respectively; or result as a symptom.

For example, conflict activity in anxiety biased samples has been linked to facets of internalizing (Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, Ando, McIntosh, Lodhia, Russell and McNaughton2021). But, here, each of our theta measures was correlated (via unique variances) with either internalizing or externalizing but not both. Conversely, each of internalizing was correlated with either error or conflict, but not both. This finding could be a result of sampling biases. Unlike the previous study by Shadli et al. (Reference Shadli, Ando, McIntosh, Lodhia, Russell and McNaughton2021) that targeted patients with anxiety symptoms, the current study oversampled for externalizing behaviors and traits. Our participants showed slower Go trial reaction times and lower percentages of successful inhibition than previously (Aron & Poldrack, Reference Aron and Poldrack2006; Shadli et al., Reference Shadli, Glue, McIntosh and McNaughton2015). This result could be due to poor attention span, since half of our participants had diagnosable externalizing problems, with almost a third being diagnosed with ADHD. Although internalizing scores are high in our sample, these could be a consequence of externalizing problems and therefore have inherently different biological origins than a sample with internalizing symptoms as the primary signs of psychopathology. This highlights the importance of multi-modal assessments in psychopathology in understanding their causes. To this end, midfrontal theta activity appears to be a good candidate for indexing psychiatric vulnerabilities.

We must also comment on the lack of relation between externalizing and error theta. As noted in the introduction, midfrontal theta activity has been shown to be less reactive (linked to poor monitoring) in externalizing (Burwell et al., Reference Burwell, Malone, Bernat and Iacono2014; Hall et al., Reference Hall, Bernat and Patrick2007; Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Malone, Burwell, Bernat and Iacono2013). Hall et al. used a flanker task with students with extreme scores on the Externalizing Spectrum Inventory, the other two studies used oddball tasks with groups with “lifetime” externalizing diagnoses. It is most likely that our lack of similar findings reflects the more motoric nature of our task; but, in any case, all three of these studies obtained their clearest results at fronto-central and posterior sites with small or null results in the region of our frontal (Fp1, Fpz, Fp2, F3, Fz, F4) cluster.

In particular, we show here that reaction to response errors (error); and insufficient sensitivity to anticipatory goal conflict (conflict) are higher-order dimensional dysfunctions in psychopathology that are indexed by neurally and functionally distinct theta components. We also show that there are no clear breaks between functional and dysfunctional theta activity. So, theta-related vulnerability factors to psychiatric illnesses apply to the general population as well as patients with sub-threshold diagnoses.

Acknowledgements

None.

Financial support

This research was funded by the University of Minnesota Press (awarded to Martin Sellbom) and the Marsden Fund, Royal Society of New Zealand Te Apārangi (awarded to Martin Sellbom and Neil McNaughton, 19-UOO-249).

Competing interests

Martin Sellbom is a paid consultant to the University of Minnesota Press, publisher of the MMPI-3, which was included in this research. No other authors declare any conflicts.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 Google Scholar
Aron, A. R., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: Role of the subthalamic nucleus. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 24242433. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Tellegen, A. (2020). MMPI-3: Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-3 technical manual. Minneapolis, MNUniversity of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Burwell, S. J., Malone, S. M., Bernat, E. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2014). Does electroencephalogram phase variability account for reduced P3 brain potential in externalizing disorders? Clinical Neurophysiology, 125, 20072015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2014.02.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319333. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavanagh, J. F., & Shackman, A. J. (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: Meta-analytic evidence. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 109, 315. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavanagh, J. F., Zambrano-Vazquez, L., & Allen, J. J. (2012). Theta lingua franca: A common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. Psychophysiology, 49, 220238. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01293.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayson, P. E., Baldwin, S. A., & Larson, M. J. (2021). Evaluating the internal consistency of subtraction-based and residualized difference scores: Considerations for psychometric reliability analyses of event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 58, e13762. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13762 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. X. (2011). Error-related medial frontal theta activity predicts cingulate-related structural connectivity. Neuroimage, 55, 13731383. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.072 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. X. (2014). A neural microcircuit for cognitive conflict detection and signaling. Trends in Neuroscience, 37, 480490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.06.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. X., & Donner, T. H. (2013). Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 27522763. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00479.2013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corr, P. J. & McNaughton, N. (2008). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. In Corr, P. J. (Ed.), The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality (pp. 155187). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511819384.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J., & McNaughton, N. (2014). Neural mechanisms of low trait anxiety and risk for externalizing behaviour. In Beauchaine, T. & Hinshaw, S. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of externalizing spectrum disorders: A developmental psychopathology perspective (pp. 220238). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199324675.013.1 Google Scholar
Cuthbert, B. N. (2022). Research domain criteria: Toward future psychiatric nosologies. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 17, 8997. https://doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2015.17.1/bcuthbert CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, J. S., Watts, A. T. M., Schmidt, N., & Bernat, E. M. (2018). Anxiety and feedback processing in a gambling task: Contributions of time-frequency theta and delta. Biological Psychology, 136, 112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.05.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
First, M., Williams, J., Karg, R., & Spitzer, R. (2015). Structured clinical interview for DSM-5—Research version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, research version; SCID-5-RV) . Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
First, M. B., Williams, J. B., Benjamin, L. S., & Spitzer, R. L. (2016). SCID-5-PD: Structured clinical interview for DSM-5® personality disorders. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.Google Scholar
Gray, J., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hajcak, G., Klawohn, J., & Meyer, A. (2019). The utility of event-related potentials in clinical psychology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 7195. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095457 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G., Meyer, A., & Kotov, R. (2017). Psychometrics and the neuroscience of individual differences: Internal consistency limits between-subjects effects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 823834. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000274 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J. R., Bernat, E. M., & Patrick, C. J. (2007). Externalizing psychopathology and the error-related negativity. Psychological science, 18, 326333. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01899.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harkness, A. R., Finn, J. A., McNulty, J. L., & Shields, S. M. (2012). The Personality Psychopathology-Five (PSY-5): Recent constructive replication and assessment literature review. Psychological Assessment, 24, 432443. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025830 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harper, J., Malone, S. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2017). Theta- and delta-band EEG network dynamics during a novelty oddball task. Psychophysiology, 54, 15901605. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12906 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jean-Richard-Dit-Bressel, P., Killcross, S., & McNally, G. P. (2018). Behavioral and neurobiological mechanisms of punishment: Implications for psychiatric disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 43, 16391650. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0047-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Ormel, J., Petukhova, M., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Russo, L. J., … Alonso, J. (2011). Development of lifetime comorbidity in the World Health Organization world mental health surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 90100. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.180 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2018). A paradigm shift in psychiatric classification: The Hierarchical Taxonomy Of Psychopathology (HiTOP). World Psychiatry, 17, 2425. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20478 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., … Zimmerman, M. (2017). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 454477. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000258 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 921926. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.921 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., Hobbs, K. A., Conway, C. C., Dick, D. M., Dretsch, M. N., Eaton, N. R., … Latzman, R. D. (2021). Validity and utility of hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): II. Externalizing superspectrum. World Psychiatry, 20, 171193. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20844 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., Kotov, R., Watson, D., Forbes, M. K., Eaton, N. R., … Bach, B. (2018). Progress in achieving quantitative classification of psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 17, 282293. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20566 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2006). Reinterpreting comorbidity: A model-based approach to understanding and classifying psychopathology. Annual Review Clinical Psychology, 2, 111133. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095213 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2011). A dimensional-spectrum model of psychopathology: Progress and opportunities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 1011. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.188 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., Patrick, C. J., Benning, S. D., & Kramer, M. D. (2007). Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: An integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 645666. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.645 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krupić, D., Corr, P. J., Ručević, S., Križanić, V., & Gračanin, A. (2016). Five reinforcement sensitivity theory (RST) of personality questionnaires: Comparison, validity and generalization. Personality and Individual Differences, 97, 1924. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., & Davis, K. A. (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction-time responses – a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 10, 276291. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.276 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLoughlin, G., Gyurkovics, M., Palmer, J., & Makeig, S. (2021). Midfrontal theta activity in psychiatric illness: An index of cognitive vulnerabilities across disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.08.020 Google ScholarPubMed
McNaughton, N., Swart, C., Neo, P., Bates, V., & Glue, P. (2013). Anti-anxiety drugs reduce conflict-specific “theta”—a possible human anxiety-specific biomarker. Journal of Affective Disordors, 148, 104111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.057 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G., Palumbo, I. M., DeYoung, C. G., Latzman, R. D., & Kotov, R. (2021). Linking RDoC and HiTOP: A new interface for advancing psychiatric nosology and neuroscience. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naragon-Gainey, K., McMahon, T. P., & Park, J. (2018). The contributions of affective traits and emotion regulation to internalizing disorders: Current state of the literature and measurement challenges. American Psychologist, 73, 11751186. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000371 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neo, P., Thurlow, J. K., & McNaughton, N. (2011). Stopping, goal-conflict, trait anxiety and frontal rhythmic power in the stop-signal task. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 485493. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-011-0046-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neo, S.-H.P, McNaughton, N., & Sellbom, M. (2021). Early and late signals of unexpected reward contribute to low extraversion and high disinhibition respectively. Personality Neuroscience, 4, e5. https://doi.org/10.1017/pen.2021.4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pasion, R., & Barbosa, F. (2019). ERN as a transdiagnostic marker of the internalizing-externalizing spectrum: A dissociable meta-analytic effect. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 103, 133149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinner, J. F. L., & Cavanagh, J. F. (2017). Frontal theta accounts for individual differences in the cost of conflict on decision making. Brain Research, 1672, 7380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2017.07.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sellbom, M., Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). On the hierarchical structure of mood and anxiety disorders: Confirmatory evidence and elaboration of a model of temperament markers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 576590. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012536 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sellbom, M., Kremyar, A. J., & Wygant, D. B. (2021). Mapping MMPI-3 scales onto the hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology. Psychological Assessment, 33, 11531168. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0001049 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shadli, S. M., Ando, L. C., McIntosh, J., Lodhia, V., Russell, B. R., … McNaughton, N. (2021). Right frontal anxiolytic-sensitive EEG ‘theta’ rhythm in the stop-signal task is a theory-based anxiety disorder biomarker. Scientific Reports, 11, 19746. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-99374-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shadli, S. M., Glue, P., McIntosh, J., & McNaughton, N. (2015). An improved human anxiety process biomarker: Characterization of frequency band, personality and pharmacology. Translational Psychiatry, 5, e699. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2015.188 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shadli, S. M., McIntosh, J., & McNaughton, N. (2020). Anxiety process “theta” biomarker in the stop signal task eliminated by a preceding relaxation test. Behavioral Neuroscience, 134, 556561 https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000328 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simms, L. J., Wright, A. G. C., Cicero, D., Kotov, R., Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., Sellbom, M., … Zimmermann, J. (2021). Development of measures for the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A collaborative scale development project. Assessment, 29, 316. https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911211015309 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. F., Stern, E. R., & Gehring, W. J. (2007). Neural systems for error monitoring: Recent findings and theoretical perspectives. Neuroscientist, 13, 160172. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858406298184 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeung, N., Bogacz, R., Holroyd, C. B., Nieuwenhuis, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2007). Theta phase resetting and the error-related negativity. Psychophysiology, 44, 3949. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00482.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review, 111, 931959. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.111.4.931 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoon, H. H., Malone, S. M., Burwell, S. J., Bernat, E. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2013). Association between P3 event-related potential amplitude and externalizing disorders: A time-domain and time-frequency investigation of 29-year-old adults. Psychophysiology, 50, 595609. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zavala, B., Tan, H., Ashkan, K., Foltynie, T., Limousin, P., Zrinzo, L.Brown, P. (2016). Human subthalamic nucleus-medial frontal cortex theta phase coherence is involved in conflict and error related cortical monitoring. Neuroimage, 137, 178187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.031 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Sequence of events in a trial. Onset of the tone from the time of the arrow presentation (SSDs) in a Stop trial are variable. A Go trial follows the same event sequence but without the onset of the tone. A smiley/frowny face is presented for a successful/unsuccessful withholding of a mouse click in a Stop trial. In a Go trial, a smiley/frowny face is presented for correct/incorrect responses. ms: milliseconds. ITI: 500 ms to 4000 ms; SSDs: see Table 1; A FI: Go Correct = 1000 ms; Stop Fail = 1500 ms; Stop Correct = 1700 ms.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive values in the current study of behavioral measures commonly reported in the SST

Figure 2

Table 2. (left)Trait scores and (right) correlations for STOP-GO theta (4–7 Hz average) power against trait measures at midline (Fz) and component 1 at two different time points in the SST (conflict, error)

Figure 3

Figure 2. Relative contributions of STOP-GO theta activity from each electrode to component 1 at two different time points in the SST (conflict, error). (a) Electrode loadings for conflict from its component matrix. (b) Electrode loadings for error from its pattern matrix. Warmer colors indicate relatively higher loadings. Electrode positions, from left to right: 1st row, Fp1, Fpz, Fp2; 2nd row, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8; 3rd row, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8; 4th row, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8.

Figure 4

Figure 3. Mean spectral power of STOP-GO theta activity (4–7 Hz) at two different time points in the SST and using two different contrasts (conflict, error).

Figure 5

Table 3. R square variances of regressions with trait measures as predictors of STOP-GO theta (4–7 Hz average) power at midline (Fz) and component 1 at two different time points in the SST (conflict, error). A. Regressions with EID and BXD as predictors. B. Regressions with BIS and BAS as predictors

Figure 6

Figure 4. Scatter plots (with regression lines) for overall regression model and internalizing and externalizing partial components for conflict and error. Note that the partial correlation for error with internalizing is positive, while the partial correlation for conflict with externalizing is negative.

Figure 7

Figure 5. Scatter plots (with regression lines) for overall regression and conflict and error partial components for externalizing and internalizing. Note that the partial correlation for conflict with externalizing is negative while the partial correlation for error with internalizing is positive.