Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T21:42:43.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Calibrate to innovate: Community age vectors and the real time incrementation of language change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 October 2020

Sophie Holmes-Elliott*
Affiliation:
University of Southampton, UK
*
Address for correspondence: Department of Linguistics, Queen Mary, University of London, E1 4NS, United Kingdom[email protected]

Abstract

This study investigates children's real time incrementation of language change as it is impacted by community-wide patterns of linguistic variability. The investigation combines apparent time analyses across an age-stratified sample of adult speakers, with real time analyses across a panel of speakers spanning childhood to adolescence. Three variables are analysed: GOOSE-fronting, a socially unmarked change; TH-fronting, a socially stigmatised, rapidly expanding change; and T-glottaling, a socially stigmatised, steadily shifting change. Variables are selected based on their social and generational profiles which present learners with more or less challenging community patterns to extract. Real time analyses confirm that community variance impacts on speakers’ ability to increment change in real time. Findings provide support for the momentum-based model of language change and builds on Labov's (2012:267) theory of the ‘outward orientation’ of children, which views learners as capable of extracting age vectors from generational differences. (Language change, incrementation, real time, GOOSE-fronting, TH-fronting, T-glottaling)*

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by the British Academy (grant no. SG-162287). Many thanks to James Turner, the project research assistant. I am grateful to the editors of Language in Society and two anonymous reviewers who helped to improve this manuscript. I would also like to thank Jennifer Smith, Erez Levon, and the audience at UK LVC 2019 for their feedback on an earlier version of the work. Any remaining shortcomings are my own. Special thanks to my speakers from Hastings who made this research possible. I would like to acknowledge that this research was conducted at the Department of Modern Languages and Linguistics, University of Southampton. However, my current affiliation is Queen Mary University of London.

References

Aitchison, Jean (1981). Language change: Progress or decay? London: Fontana.Google Scholar
Altendorf, Ulrike, & Watt, Dominic (2008). The dialects in the south of England: Phonology. In Kortmann, Bernd & Upton, Clive (eds.), Varieties of English, vol. 1: The British Isles, 194222. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Andrésen, Bjørn Stålhane (1968). Pre-glottalization in English standard pronunciation. Oslo: Norwegian Universities Press.Google Scholar
Baranowski, Maciej (2017). Class matters: The sociolinguistics of goose and goat in Manchester English. Language Variation and Change 29(3):301–39.10.1017/S0954394517000217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baranowski, Maciej, & Turton, Danielle (2015). Manchester English. In Hickey, Raymond (ed.), Researching Northern Englishes, 293316. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/veaw.g55.13barCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas; Maechler, Martin; Bolker, Ben; & Walker, Steve (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1):148.10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2020). The initiation and incrementation of sound change: Community-oriented momentum-sensitive learning. In James Kirby, Lauren Hall-Lew, & Patrick Honeybone (eds.), Individuals, communities, and sound change. Special collection in Glossa, to appear.Google Scholar
Boudahmane, Karim; Manta, Mathieu; Antoine, Fabien; Galliano, Sylvain; & Barras, Claude (2008). Transcriber [Software]. Online: http://trans.sourceforge.net.Google Scholar
Catford, John. C. (1988). A practical introduction to phonetics. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny L.; Kerswill, Paul; Fox;, Sue & Torgersen, Eivind (2011). Contact the feature pool and the speech community: The emergence of Multicultural London English. Journal of Sociolinguistics 15(2):151–96.10.1111/j.1467-9841.2011.00478.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chevrot, Jean-Pierre; Beaud, Laurence; & Varga, Renata (2000). Developmental data on a French sociolinguistic variable: Post-consonantal word-final /R/. Language Variation and Change 12(3):295319.10.1017/S095439450012304XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Lynn, & Trousdale, Graeme (2009). Exploring the role of token frequency in phonological change: Evidence from TH-fronting in east-central Scotland. English Language and Linguistics 13(1):3355.10.1017/S1360674308002852CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, Sandra; Ford, Elms; & Amani, Youssef (1995). The third dialect of English: Some Canadian evidence. Language Variation and Change 7(2):209–28.10.1017/S0954394500000995CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, Felicity (1999). Vowel change in Australian English. Phonetica 56(1):127.10.1159/000028438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Díaz-Campos, Manuel (2005). The emergence of adult-like command of sociolinguistic variables: A study of consonant weakening in Spanish-speaking children. In Eddington, David (ed.), Studies in the acquisition of the Hispanic languages: Papers from the 6th Conference on the Acquisition of Spanish and Portuguese as First and Second Languages, 5665. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, Manuel (2011). Becoming a member of the speech community: Learning socio-phonetic variation in child language. In Díaz-Campos, Manuel (ed.), The handbook of Hispanic sociolinguistics, 263–83. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.10.1002/9781444393446.ch13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Docherty, Gerard. J., & Foulkes, Paul (1999). Instrumental phonetics and phonological variation: Case studies from Newcastle upon Tyne and Derby. In Foulkes & Docherty, 47–71.Google Scholar
Drummond, Rob (2011). Glottal variation in /t/ in non-native English speech: Patterns of acquisition. English World-Wide 32(3):280308.10.1075/eww.32.3.02druCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, Anita, & Bauer, Laurie (2000). An acoustic study of the vowels of New Zealand English. Australian Journal of Linguistics 20:93117.10.1080/07268600020006021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope (2000). Linguistic variation as social practice. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope (2011). Language and power in the preadolescent heterosexual market. American Speech 86:8597.10.1215/00031283-1277528CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, Susan (1972). Sociolinguistic rules: Alteration and co-occurrence. In Gumperz, John & Hymes, Dell (eds.), Directions in sociolinguistics, 213–50. Cambridge: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Fabricius, Anne (2002). Ongoing change in modern RP: Evidence for the disappearing stigma of t-glottalling. English World-Wide 23(1):115–36.10.1075/eww.23.1.06fabCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, Nicholas (2012). A sociophonetic study of Nottingham speakers. York: University of York PhD dissertation.Google Scholar
Fought, Carmen (1999). A majority sound change in a minority community: /u/-fronting in Chicano English. Journal of Sociolinguistics 3(1):523.10.1111/1467-9481.t01-1-00060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foulkes, Paul, & Docherty, Gerard (eds.) (1999). Urban voices: Accent studies in the British Isles. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Foulkes, Paul; Docherty, Gerard; & Watt, Dominic (2005). Phonological variation in child directed speech. Language 81:177206.10.1353/lan.2005.0018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, Valerie (2008). Patterns of /uw/, /ʊ/, and /ow/ fronting in Reno, Nevada. American Speech 83(4):432–54.10.1215/00031283-2008-030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haddican, Bill; Foulkes, Paul; Hughes, Vincent; & Richards, Hazel (2013). Interaction of social and linguistic constraints on two vowel changes in northern England. Language Variation and Change 25(3):371403.10.1017/S0954394513000197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall-Lew, Lauren (2011). The completion of a sound change in California English. In Lee, Wai Sum & Zee, Eric (eds.), Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 17–21 August 2011, Hong Kong. Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Harrington, Jonathan; Kleber, Felicitas; & Reubold, Ulrich (2008). Compensation for coarticulation, /u/-fronting, and sound change in Standard Southern British: An acoustic and perceptual study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123(5):2825–35.10.1121/1.2897042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes-Elliott, Sophie (2016). Ladies first? Adolescent peaks in a male-led change: TH-fronting in southeast England. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 22(2), article 10. Online: https://repository.upenn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1930&context=pwpl.Google Scholar
Holmes-Elliott, Sophie, & Turner, James (2019). The emergence of gendered production between childhood and adolescence: A real time analysis of /s/ in Southern British English. In Calhoun, Sasha, Escudero, Paola, Tabain, Marija, & Warren, Paul (eds.), Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia 2019, 2670–74. Canberra: Australasian Speech Science and Technology Association.Google Scholar
Kassab, Werdan (2008). A linguistic change in a newly-formed town. Lancaster: Lancaster University MA dissertation.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul (2003). Dialect levelling and geographical diffusion in British English. In Britain, David & Cheshire, Jenny (eds.), Social dialectology, In honour of Peter Trudgill, 223–44. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/impact.16.16kerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, Paul (2010). Sociolinguistic approaches to language change: Phonology. In Wodak, Ruth, Johnstone, Barbara, & Kerswill, Paul (eds.), The SAGE handbook of sociolinguistics, 219–35. London: SAGE.Google Scholar
Kerswill, Paul, & Williams, Anne (2000). Creating a New Town koine: Children and language change in Milton Keynes. Language in Society 29(1):65115.10.1017/S0047404500001020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkham, Sam, & Moore, Emma (2013). Adolescence. In Chambers, Jack, Schilling-Estes, Natalie, & Trudgill, Peter (eds.), The handbook of language variation and change, 277–96. Oxford: Blackwell.10.1002/9781118335598.ch13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohn, Mary, & Farrington, Charlie (2012). Evaluating acoustic speaker normalization algorithms: Evidence from longitudinal child data. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 131(3):2237–48.10.1121/1.3682061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, Alexandra; Brockhoff;, Per Bruun & Bojesen Christensen, Rune Haubo (2016). LmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package). Online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest.Google Scholar
Labov, William (1984). Field methods of the project on linguistic change and variation. In Baugh, John & Sherzer, Joel (eds.), Language in use: Readings in sociolinguistics, 2853. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Labov, William (1994). Principles of linguistic change, vol. 1: Internal factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William (2001). Principles of linguistic change, vol. 2: External factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Labov, William (2007). Transmission and diffusion. Language 83:344–87.10.1353/lan.2007.0082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William (2012). What is to be learned: The community as the focus of social cognition. Review of Cognitive Linguistics 10:265–93.10.1075/rcl.10.2.02labCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenth, Russell (2017). Using lsmeans. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lsmeans/vignettes/using-lsmeans.pdf.Google Scholar
Llamas, Carmen (1998). Language variation and innovation in Middlesbrough. Leeds Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics 6:9115.Google Scholar
Lobanov, Boris M. (1971). Classification of Russian vowels spoken by different listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 49:606608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mathisen, Anne G. (1999). Sandwell, West Midlands: Ambiguous perspectives on gender patterns and models of change. In Foulkes & Docherty, 107–23.Google Scholar
Mees, Inger M., & Collins, Beverley (1999). Cardiff: A real-time study of glottalisation. In Foulkes & Docherty, 185–202.Google Scholar
Mesthrie, Rajend (2010). Sociophonetics and social change: Deracialisation of the GOOSE vowel in South African English. Journal of Sociolinguistics 14(1):333.10.1111/j.1467-9841.2009.00433.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milroy, James (2003). When is a sound change? On the role of external factors in language change. In Britain, David & Cheshire, Jenny (eds.), Social dialectology: In honour of Peter Trudgill, 209–22. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/impact.16.15milCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milroy, James, Milroy, Lesley; Hartley;, Sue & Walshaw, David (1994). Glottal stops and Tyneside glottalization: Competing patterns of variation and change in British English. Language Variation and Change 6:327–57.10.1017/S095439450000171XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, Jimmi (2010). Lexical frequency effects in the spread of TH-fronting in Glaswegian: A cue to the origins of sound change? Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh MS dissertation.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Online: http://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Raymond, William D.; Brown;, Esther L. & Healy, Alice F. (2016). Cumulative context effects and variant lexical representations: Word use and English final t/d deletion. Language Variation and Change 28:175202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Julie (1994). Acquisition of variable rules: (-t,d) deletion and ing production in preschool children. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania PhD dissertation.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie (1997). Hitting a moving target: Acquisition of sound change in progress by Philadelphia children. Language Variation & Change 9:249–66.10.1017/S0954394500001897CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Julie (1999). Going younger to do difference: Acquisition of dialect features by preschool children. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 6(2), article 10. Online: https://repository.upenn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1659&context=pwpl.Google Scholar
Roberts, Julie, & Labov, Wiliam (1995). Learning to talk Philadelphian: Acquisition of short a by preschool children. Language Variation & Change 7:101–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne (1984). The language of children and adolescents; The acquisition of communicative competence. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rosenfelder, Ingrid; Fruehwald, Josef; Keelan, Evanini; Scott, Seyfarth; Kyle, Gorman; Prichard, Hilary; & Yuan, Jiahong (2014). FAVE (Forced Alignment and Vowel Extraction) Program Suite v1.2.2. Online: https://github.com/JoFrhwld/FAVE.Google Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian (2018). Language change across the lifespan. Annual Review of Linguistics 4(1):297316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian (2019). Language change across the lifespan: Three trajectory types. Language 95(2):197229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schleef, Erik (2013). Glottal replacement of /t/ in two British capitals: Effects of word frequency and morphological compositionality. Language Variation and Change 25(2):201–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schleef, Erik, & Ramsammy, Michael (2013). Labio-dental fronting of /θ/ in London and Edinburgh: A cross-dialectal study. English Language and Linguistics 17(1):2554.10.1017/S1360674312000317CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Jennifer, Durham, Mercedes, & Fortune, Liane (2007). ‘Mam, ma troosers is fa'in doon!’: Community, caregiver and child in the acquisition of variation in Scottish dialect. Language Variation and Change 19(1):6399.10.1017/S0954394507070044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Jennifer, & Holmes-Elliott, Sophie (2017). The unstoppable glottal: Tracking rapid change in an iconic British variable. English Language and Linguistics 22(3):323–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockwell, Robert, & Minkova, Donka (1997). On drifts and shifts. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia: An International Review of English Studies 31:283303.Google Scholar
Stuart-Smith, Jane (1999). Glottals past and present: A study of T-glottalling in Glaswegian. Leeds Studies in English, 181–204. Online: https://ludos.leeds.ac.uk:443/R/-?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=124883&silo_library=GEN01.Google Scholar
Stuart-Smith, Jane, & Timmins, Claire (2006). ‘Tell her to shut her moof’: The role of the lexicon in TH-fronting in Glaswegian. In Caie, Graham, Hough, Carole, & Wotherspoon, Irené (eds.), The power of words: Essays in lexicography, lexicology and semantics in honour of Christian J. Kay, 171–83. Amsterdam: Rodolphi.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A., & D'Arcy, Alexandra (2009). Peaks beyond phonology: Adolescence, incrementation and language change. Language 85:58108.10.1353/lan.0.0084CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tollfree, Laura (1999). South East London English: Discrete versus continuous modelling of consonantal reduction. In Foulkes & Docherty, 163–84.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter (1999). Norwich: Endogenous and exogenous linguistic changes. In Foulkes & Docherty, 124–40.Google Scholar
Watt, Dominic, & Milroy, Lesley (1999). Variation in three Tyneside vowels: Is this dialect levelling? In Foulkes & Docherty, 25–46.Google Scholar
Wells, John. C. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar