Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T10:21:24.292Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Traumatic brain injury in young children: Postacute effects on cognitive and school readiness skills

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 September 2008

H. GERRY TAYLOR*
Affiliation:
Division of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics and Pediatric Psychology, Department of Pediatrics, Case Western Reserve University and University Hospitals Case Medical Center, Cleveland, Ohio
MAEGAN D. SWARTWOUT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
KEITH OWEN YEATES
Affiliation:
Division of Psychology, Department of Pediatrics, The Ohio State University & Center for Biobehavioral Health, The Research Institute at Nationwide Children's Hospital, Columbus, Ohio
NICOLAY CHERTKOFF WALZ
Affiliation:
Division of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Department of Pediatrics, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center and University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio
TERRY STANCIN
Affiliation:
Division of Pediatric Psychology, Department of Pediatrics, MetroHealth Medical Center and Case Western Reserve University School of Medicine, Cleveland, Ohio
SHARI L. WADE
Affiliation:
Department of Rehabilitation, Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center and University of Cincinnati College of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: H. Gerry Taylor, Division of Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics and Psychology, D.O. Walker Building, Suite 3150, 10524 Euclid Avenue, Cleveland, OH. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Previous studies have documented weaknesses in cognitive ability and early academic readiness in young children with traumatic brain injury (TBI). However, few of these studies have rigorously controlled for demographic characteristics, examined the effects of TBI severity on a wide range of skills, or explored moderating influences of environmental factors on outcomes. To meet these objectives, each of three groups of children with TBI (20 with severe, 64 with moderate, and 15 with mild) were compared with a group of 117 children with orthopedic injuries (OI group). The children were hospitalized for their injuries between 3 and 6 years of age and were assessed an average of 1½ months post injury. Analysis revealed generalized weaknesses in cognitive and school readiness skills in the severe TBI group and less pervasive effects of moderate TBI. Indices of TBI severity predicted outcomes within the TBI sample and environmental factors moderated the effects of TBI on some measures. The findings document adverse effects of TBI in early childhood on postacute cognitive and school readiness skills and indicate that these effects are related to both injury severity and the family environment. (JINS, 2008, 14, 734–745.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

INTRODUCTION

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is one of the most common causes of death and long-term disability in the pediatric age range (Gotschall, Reference Gotschall and Eichelberger1993; Kraus, Reference Kraus, Broman and Michell1995). According to a report on emergency department (ED) visits, hospitalizations, and deaths in the United States for the years 1995–2001 (Langlois et al., Reference Langlois, Rutland-Brown and Thomas2006), nearly half a million children 0–14 years of age had TBI each year during this period. Among survivors, the consequences of TBI in children include physical conditions (e.g., neuromotor impairment, seizures, trauma-related orthopedic injuries), lowered cognitive and academic skills relative to age expectations or preinjury estimates, and problems in school performance, behavior, socialization, and adaptive functioning (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Barnes, Fletcher, Levin, Swank and Song2004a; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Taylor, Drotar, Yeates, Wade and Stancin2003; Stancin et al., Reference Stancin, Drotar, Taylor, Yeates, Wade and Minich2002; Yeates, Reference Yeates, Yeates, Ris and Taylor2000; Yeates & Taylor, Reference Yeates and Taylor1997). Although TBI in school-age children is associated with global cognitive deficits (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer and DeLeon2004b; Fay et al., Reference Fay, Jaffe, Polissar, Liao, Rivara and Martin1994; Levin et al., Reference Levin, Ewing-Cobbs, Eisenberg, Broman and Michel1995; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Klein and Stancin1999; Yeates, Reference Yeates, Yeates, Ris and Taylor2000), impairments are especially pronounced on measures of memory, perceptual abilities and psychomotor speed, attention and executive function, and discourse processing (Anderson & Catroppa, Reference Anderson and Catroppa2005; Bawden et al., Reference Bawden, Knights and Winogron1985; Chadwick et al., Reference Chadwick, Rutter, Brown, Shaffer and Traub1981; Dennis & Barnes, Reference Dennis and Barnes2001; Donders, Reference Donders2001; Donders & Giroux, Reference Donders and Giroux2005; Ewing-Cobbs & Barnes, Reference Ewing-Cobbs and Barnes2002; Levin & Hanten, Reference Levin and Hanten2005; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Klein and Stancin1999).

More negative outcomes are predicted by increasing TBI severity and less advantaged family environments (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Ewing-Cobbs, Francis, Levin, Broman and Michel1995; Schwartz et al., Reference Schwartz, Taylor, Drotar, Yeates, Wade and Stancin2003; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Klein and Stancin1999, Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Stancin and Minich2002). Whereas cognitive deficits are well documented in school-age children with moderate to severe TBI (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Morse, Catroppa, Haritou and Rosenfeld2004, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005b; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Klein and Stancin1999), studies of children with mild TBI have yielded inconsistent findings. Some of these studies have demonstrated only transient cognitive deficits if any and others have suggested emerging consequences over time after injury (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2001; Gronwall et al., Reference Gronwall, Wrightson and McGinn1997; Keenan et al., Reference Keenan, Hooper, Wetherington, Nocera and Runyan2007; Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Willmott, Rothwell, Cameron, Ayton, Nelms, Curran and Ng1999).

Younger age at injury is another predictor of worse outcomes. Specifically, children aged 2 to 7 years at the time of injury are more susceptible to deficits in expressive language, attention, and academic achievement compared with children injured at later ages (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005a; Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Dennis and Wilkinson1999; Dennis et al., Reference Dennis, Wilkinson, Koski, Humphreys, Broman and Michel1995; Ewing-Cobbs & Barnes, Reference Ewing-Cobbs and Barnes2002; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Miner, Fletcher and Levin1989, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997; Morse et al., Reference Morse, Haritou, Ong, Anderson, Catroppa and Rosenfeld1999; Verger et al., Reference Verger, Junque, Jurado, Tresserras, Bartumeus, Nogues and Poch2000). Researchers have speculated that the poorer outcomes in younger children may reflect a greater susceptibility to diffuse brain insult or abnormalities in neurogenesis, or a greater effect of injury on postinjury skill development (Anderson & Moore, Reference Anderson and Moore1995; Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Dennis and Wilkinson1999; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer and DeLeon2004b; Taylor & Alden, Reference Taylor and Alden1997; Wetherington & Hooper, Reference Wetherington and Hooper2006).

Unfortunately, questions remain regarding the nature of the effects on TBI in young children on cognition and achievement. Previous research demonstrates both postacute and persisting effects of TBI in this age group on a wide range of ability measures (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse and Haritou1999, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2000a,Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Rosenfeld, Haritou and Morseb, Reference Anderson, Morse, Catroppa, Haritou and Rosenfeld2004, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Miner, Fletcher and Levin1989, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer and DeLeon2004b; Gronwall et al., Reference Gronwall, Wrightson and McGinn1997). However, methodological limitations make it difficult to interpret these findings, as past studies have either failed to include a comparison group of children without TBI or have used uninjured children as controls. Inclusion of controls without TBI is needed to assess the effects of varying degrees of TBI severity relative to expectations for children without brain insult. But comparison with uninjured controls is also problematic. These children may have fewer preinjury developmental problems and come from more advantaged family backgrounds than children with TBI, raising questions as to whether group differences were present before TBI (Goldstrohm & Arffa, Reference Goldstrohm and Arffa2005; Keenan et al., Reference Keenan, Hooper, Wetherington, Nocera and Runyan2007).

Determining the postacute effects of TBI on cognitive and school readiness skills is especially critical given the need for early identification of children who have been adversely affected by injury. Awareness of postacute deficits would be useful in gauging children's needs for interventions as they are transitioning to school entry or beginning to acquire basic academic competences. Data suggesting persisting or even later-emerging impairments in this age group (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse and Haritou1999, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Rosenfeld, Haritou and Morse2000b, Reference Anderson, Morse, Catroppa, Haritou and Rosenfeld2004, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Miner, Fletcher and Levin1989, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997; Gronwall et al., Reference Gronwall, Wrightson and McGinn1997) further reinforce the importance of an awareness of postacute sequelae. A better understanding of the effects of TBI severity and environmental factors on outcomes would also be useful in identifying children at high risk for sequelae.

The primary objective of the present study was thus to investigate the effects of TBI in young children on postacute cognitive and achievement outcomes using methods that rigorously control for noninjury influences on outcomes. To provide an estimate of the effects of TBI that took into account preinjury risk exposure as well as the experience of hospitalization for injury, children admitted to hospitals for orthopedic injuries but without TBI were recruited as a comparison group. Outcomes were assessed using comprehensive measures of cognitive and early academic skills that were applicable across all or at least a major portion of the 3- to 6-year-old age range. Previous findings suggesting that children's self-regulatory or executive functions may be vulnerable to TBI and may play an important role in children's ongoing development (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005b; Blair, Reference Blair2002; Bronson, Reference Bronson2000; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer and DeLeon2004b) prompted inclusion of several experimental measures of this skill domain. Finally, group comparisons were made controlling for sociodemographic factors.

We hypothesized that young children hospitalized for TBI would have deficits in cognitive and school readiness skills relative to children with orthopedic injuries only, and that these deficits would be most pervasive in children with severe TBI. Given the failure of many previous studies to investigate a wide range of outcomes in young children with TBI, we anticipated wide-ranging deficits and did not have expectations with respect to which skills would be more or less affected. We also hypothesized that outcomes would be worse for children from more disadvantaged environments (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006), and we explored the possibility that such environments might even exacerbate the negative consequences of TBI (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Klein and Stancin1999, Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Stancin and Minich2002; Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Drotar, Wade, Klein, Stancin and Schatschneider1997).

METHODS

Sample

Children were recruited from consecutive inpatient admissions from 2003 to 2006 of children with TBI or with OI at three tertiary care children's hospitals and a general hospital, all of which had Level 1 trauma centers. The study was approved by the ethics boards of all participating hospitals and informed consent was obtained before participation. Eligibility criteria included age at injury between 3 years, 0 months and 6 years, 11 months, no documentation in the medical chart or in parent interview of child abuse as a cause of the injury, and English as the primary spoken language in the home. Children with a previous history of autism, mental retardation, or a neurological disorder were excluded. Eligibility for the TBI group included a TBI due to blunt trauma requiring overnight admission to the hospital and either a Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS; Teasdale & Jennett, Reference Teasdale and Jennett1974) score <15, suggesting altered neurological status, or evidence for TBI-related brain abnormalities from computed tomography (CT) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).

Consistent with previous investigations (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Ewing-Cobbs, Miner, Levin and Eisenberg1990; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Klein and Stancin1999), severe TBI was defined as one resulting in a GCS score of 8 or less. Moderate TBI was defined as a GCS score of 9–12 or a higher GCS score with abnormal neuroimaging. A final group of children with mild TBI comprised those participants with GCS scores of 13–14 without neuroimaging abnormalities. To insure that evidence for TBI was based on direct physical examination and not on history alone, children with GCS scores of 15 and normal neuroimaging were not recruited. The GCS score assigned to the child was the lowest one recorded. Inclusion in the OI group required a documented bone fracture in an area of the body other than the head that required an overnight hospital stay, and the absence of any evidence of loss of consciousness or other findings suggestive of brain injury.

A total of 221 children (102 with TBI and 119 with OI) and their caregivers were enrolled in the study. Recruitment rates for families contacted were somewhat higher for the combined TBI sample than for the OI group (53% vs. 35%). Examination of pre-enrollment screening data indicated that refusal rates were lower for children with the highest GCS scores. However, comparison of participants with nonparticipants on census-based neighborhood income failed to reveal differences for the sample as a whole or for subsets of children with GCS scores <9, 9–12, or 13–14. At least a portion of the test battery was administered to 216 children (98%) at the postacute assessment. The final sample comprised 99 children with TBI (20 severe, 64 moderate, and 15 mild) and 117 with OI. Reasons for failure to test children included injuries that precluded testing (2 with severe TBI) and difficulties in arranging for travel for the assessment (1 with severe TBI and 2 with OI). Untested children did not differ from those assessed in parental marital status, neighborhood income, race, or sex.

As shown in Table 1, the groups did not differ statistically in age at assessment, neighborhood income, distributions of sex, race, and maternal education levels, or parent resources and stressors as measured by the Life Stressors and Social Resources Inventory—Adult Version (LISRES-A; Moos & Moos, Reference Moos and Moos1994). Data collected in parent interview also failed to suggest group differences in preinjury developmental status as assessed by special education services or prior concerns about the child's development, behavior, or learning. The educational classification of special services was not requested and in view of the young age of the sample we did not inquire about attention deficits or learning disorders.

Table 1. Sample demographic characteristics

Note

TBI = traumatic brain injury; OI = orthopedic injury; SD = standard deviation; GED = General Education Diploma. All group differences nonsignificant. LISRES-A = Life Stressors and Social Resources Inventory—Adult Version.

Table 2 lists injury and medical characteristics for each of the groups. The time between injury and assessment was shorter for the OI group than for the TBI groups (significant for mild and moderate TBI groups, nonsignificant trend for severe TBI group). This difference was likely related to our willingness to extend recruitment somewhat beyond our initial recruitment window (3 months after injury) in an effort to enroll as many children with TBI as possible. The groups also differed in their mean New Injury Severity Score (NISS; Osler et al., Reference Osler, Bakker and Long1997), defined as the sum of the squares of the Abbreviated Injury Scale (AIS) scores for each child's three most severely injured body regions. Post hoc tests indicated higher NISS for the severe and moderate TBI groups compared with the mild TBI and OI groups. The groups also differed in mean “non–head-injury” NISS, computed as the NISS minus the AIS for the head region. By virtue of selection criteria for children with OI, the severity of injuries to regions other than the head was higher in this group than in the TBI groups. The distribution of causes of injury for the TBI group were consistent with national trends for young children, with a substantial proportion of both the TBI and OI group sustaining injuries due to falls (Langlois et al., Reference Langlois, Rutland-Brown and Thomas2006). A significant group difference in cause of injury reflected higher rates of transportation-related injuries in the TBI groups compared with the OI group. Neuroimaging abnormalities were classified based on findings reported by radiologists (records available for all but two children). The four categories of abnormality were: no lesion; mild abnormalities, defined as a single subdural, subarachnoid, or epidural hemorrhage, or a single intraparenchymal lesion, contusion or hemorrhage; moderate abnormalities, defined as multifocal lesions without diffuse abnormality (i.e., no edema, mass effect, swelling, midline shift, volume loss, or diffuse axonal injury); and severe abnormalities, defined as any diffuse abnormality, with or without focal lesions. The categorization of neuroimaging was based on previous research relating outcomes of TBI to the presence versus absence and type of brain lesions (Bowen et al., Reference Bowen, Clark, Bigler, Gardner, Nilsson, Gooch and Pompa1997; Levin, Reference Levin1995; Levin et al., Reference Levin, Aldrich, Saydjari, Eisenberg, Foulkes, Bellefleur, Luerssen, Jane, Marmarou, Marshall and Young1992; Prasad et al., Reference Prasad, Ewing-Cobbs, Swank and Kramer2002). At the time of the postacute assessment, 4 children with severe TBI and 3 with moderate TBI were receiving anti-epilepsy drugs, one with mild TBI was receiving Adderall, and none of the children in the OI group was taking a prescription medication.

Table 2. Injury and medical characteristics

Note

TBI = traumatic brain injury; OI = orthopedic injury; SD = standard deviation; NISS = New Injury Severity Score; GCS = Glasgow Coma Scale.

* Significant difference between groups at p < .05.

a Injuries due to “other” causes included those related to sports and recreation, rough-housing, and falling objects.

b See text for definition of severity of neuroimaging abnormality. Abnormality was absent in the mild TBI and OI groups by definition.

Assessment Procedures

Child and family assessments were conducted in tandem as part of a more comprehensive evaluation of the child and family that also included parent interviews, ratings of child behavior, and video-taped parent-child interactions. Administered by parent interview, the LISRES-A has satisfactory internal consistency and was used to assess interpersonal supports and stressors experienced by the caregiver in a variety of social domains (e.g., with family members, friends, coworkers). Child tests were administered in a fixed order, with three separate but overlapping test batteries given to children in the following age ranges: 3 years, 0 months to 3 years, 5 months; 3 years, 6 months to 5 years, 11 months; and 6 years, 0 months to 6 years, 11 months.

Table 3 lists the child assessment procedures, the age ranges of administration, and the scores used in analysis. To obtain a measure of general cognitive ability, we administered the core subtests needed to compute the General Conceptual Ability (GCA) score of the Differential Ability Scales (DAS; Elliott, Reference Elliott1990). Other standardized tests were used to assess performance in the domains of language, memory, spatial reasoning, and school readiness skills. Nonstandardized tests of executive function included the Delay of Gratification Task (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Murray and Harlan2000), Delayed Alternation (DA; Espy et al., Reference Espy, Kaufman, McDiarmid and Glishy1999), and Shape School (Espy, Reference Espy1997). Delay of Gratification requires the child to inhibit opening an attractive gift, with performance defined in terms of contact versus no contact with the gift. In DA, the child is asked to retrieve a reward (e.g., an M&M or a Cheerio) hidden under one of two cups placed side by side. The contingency is then reversed with the reward hidden under the other cup. The child is not allowed to see where the reward is placed, but can learn to anticipate placement because the placement side is reversed after each correct response. Performance was defined in terms of number of consecutively correct alternations. Shape School is a Stroop-like measure of self regulatory abilities in young children. In this task, the child is first taught to name cartoon “pupils” by their shapes or colors. The child is then asked to name the color of some pupils but not others. This test measures the ability to inhibit prepotent responses and the mental flexibility to switch between color and shape names according to learned rules. Conditions include Simple Naming, Inhibition, Switching, and Both (the latter referring to a condition in which both inhibition and set switching are required). An efficiency score was computed for each condition by dividing the number correct by completion time.

Table 3. Neuropsychological test battery

Note

DAS = Differential Abilities Scales; GCA = General Conceptual Ability; CASL = Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language; NEPSY = A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; WJ-III = Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement, 3rd Ed.; SCR = School Readiness Composite. Variations in sample size were related in large part to the applicability of some of the measures to only a subset of the 3- to 6-year-old age range. Less marked variations in sample size were also related to the inability of some children to complete select tests. Reasons for variation in sample size for the different Shape School conditions is related to the applicability of the Switch and Both conditions only to children age 4 years and older and the inability of some age-eligible children to grasp task demands.

Data Analysis

Before analysis, raw scores on the outcome measures were converted to age-standardized scores using published norms. Because published norms were unavailable for DA and Shape School, age-expected scores on these tests were generated based on regression analysis of data from the OI group. Age-adjusted z scores were computed for each measure by dividing the differences between each observed and age-predicted score by the standard error of the estimate. Potential influences of extreme scores on results were limited by truncating, or windsorizing, standard scores to within 3 standard deviations of the mean score (Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell1989). Examination of the scores revealed acceptable distributions for all continuous measures.

Preliminary analysis revealed that primary caregiver education level and median census tract income were positively correlated with each other and with most neuropsychological outcomes. Socioeconomic status (SES) was thus defined as the mean of the sample z-scores for these two variables. Additional sociodemographic variables (e.g., parent marital status and occupation) were examined but were excluded after initial analyses failed to reveal associations with outcomes independent of parent education and census income.

Group comparisons on the continuous measures of outcome were made using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). Group effects were defined by preplanned contrasts of each TBI group with the OI group. Covariates included SES, sex, and race (white/nonwhite), as justified by evidence for associations of these factors with cognitive abilities in children (McDermott, Reference McDermott1995). Time since injury was not considered as analysis failed to reveal associations with the performance of the TBI sample. Covariate-adjusted logistic analysis was used to examine group differences in odds of contact versus no contact on the Delayed Gratification Task, with age at assessment entered as an additional covariate in analysis of this measure.

To assess the dose-response relationship of TBI presence and severity with outcome, a set of secondary analyses examined the linear trend between degree of TBI (none or 0 = OI; 1 = mild TBI, 2 = moderate TBI, and 3 = severe TBI) and covariate-adjusted test scores. To further investigate the relation of injury severity to outcomes for the children with TBI, we conducted regression analyses of data from the three TBI groups combined. In these analyses, the GCS score and coma duration (none, <24 hr, ≥24 hr) were entered (separately) into a hierarchical regression following entry of SES, race, and sex.

Regression analysis was also used to examine moderating effects of SES, LISRES-A stressors and resources scores, and age at injury on the group differences. To test for moderation, each of these factors was entered separately into a regression along with the TBI-OI group contrast terms and the interaction of each contrast with that factor. Models that included the covariates were used to subsequently examine the moderating effects of LISRES-A resources and stressors and age at injury. Moderating effects of race and sex were not examined due to small cell sizes for the severe and mild TBI groups. Logistic regression was conducted to investigate moderating effects on the Delayed Gratification Task, with age at assessment again included as an additional covariate.

The Holm's modification of Bonferroni procedure (Jaccard & Guilamo-Ramos, Reference Jaccard and Guilamo-Ramos2002) was used to adjust for multiple comparisons within each test domain, with domain-wise alpha set at .05. Because of the more experimental nature of measures in the executive function domain and the lack of published age standards, this adjustment was not applied to the measures in that domain. However, effect sizes were computed for all group contrasts, and only significant differences of at least medium effect size (d = .5) were considered in interpreting results from the tests of executive function. Statistical power computations indicated that the study was adequate for detection of a medium effect size for the moderate TBI-OI group contrasts but under-powered for detection of an effect of this magnitude for the severe TBI-OI and mild TBI-OI group contrasts. These results suggest that only relatively large effects of severe or mild TBI were likely to reach levels of statistical significance.

RESULTS

Group Differences

As shown in Table 4, the severe TBI group had lower scores than the OI group on the GCA, all four memory tests, Pattern Construction, Copying Designs, Early Number Concepts, and SRC. The moderate TBI group had lower scores than the OI group on Sentence Memory, Recall of Digits, and the Shape School Both condition. Many of the effect sizes for the severe TBI-OI group contrasts were of medium magnitude (Cohen's d = .5–.8), whereas most effects for the moderate TBI-OI group contrasts were small (Cohen's d = .2–.5). Effect sizes for several of the mild TBI-OI group contrasts were in the medium range and in the hypothesized direction despite a lack of statistical significance, with nonsignificant trends (unadjusted ps < .1) for Verbal Fluency, Recall of Digits, and the Shape School Simple Naming and Switch conditions.

Table 4. Group comparisons on neuropsychological and school readiness measures

Note

Group sizes vary across tests depending on whether the procedures were appropriate for all or a portion of the 3- to 6-year-old age range. Abbreviations: TBI = traumatic brain injury; OI = orthopedic injury; Adj. M = covariate-adjusted mean; se = standard error; ES = effect size; DAS = Differential Abilities Scales; GCA = General Conceptual Ability; CASL = Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language; NEPSY = A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment; WJ-III = Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement, 3rd Ed.; SCR = School Readiness Composite; Bracken SRC = Bracken Basic Concept Scale School Readiness Composite.

a Means are adjusted for effects of SES, sex, and race.

b Effect sizes defined by Cohen's d: difference between estimated (covariate-adjusted) means/estimate of pooled within-group SD.

c Standard score.

d T score

e Scaled score.

f Age-standardized Z score.

* Significant difference, severe TBI versus OI.

Significant difference, moderate TBI versus OI.

Other Factors Related to Outcome

According to results from the ANCOVAs, all three covariates accounted for unique variance in at least some outcomes. Higher SES was associated with better performance on all tests except for the Delayed Gratification Task, DA, and the Shape School Both condition. Whites scored higher than nonwhites on Verbal Comprehension, Naming Vocabulary, and the Shape School Both condition. Females outperformed males on the GCA, Pragmatic Judgment, Verbal Comprehension, Recognition of Pictures, Recall of Digits, Story Recall, Shape School Simple Naming and Inhibition conditions, and Spelling.

In regressions examining the effect of group along a continuum from OI to severe TBI, significant linear trend effects were found for GCA, Sentence Memory, Story Recall, Recall of Digits, Pattern Construction, Shape School Switch and Both conditions, Early Number Concepts, and SRC (all adjusted ps < .05). For each of these outcomes, scores decreased with increasing TBI severity. Regressions conducted on data from the children with TBI indicated that a lower GCS score predicted worse outcomes on GCA, Pattern Construction, Copying Designs Early Number Concepts, and SRC; and longer coma duration predicted worse outcomes on GCS, Pattern Construction, Copying Designs, and Early Number Concepts (all adjusted ps < .05).

Moderators of TBI Effects

Lower SES was associated with more adverse effects of severe TBI on Naming Vocabulary, B = 9.46 (3.39), adjusted p < .05; and increasing parent stressors were associated with more negative consequences of moderate TBI on Spelling, B = −0.78 (0.29), adjusted p < .05. Results failed to reveal moderating effects of parent resources or age at injury on any of the outcomes.

DISCUSSION

Effects of TBI in Young Children

Consistent with findings from past research on young children, the participants in this study with moderate to severe TBI performed more poorly than the OI group on a wide range of neuropsychological and achievement tests (Anderson & Catroppa, Reference Anderson and Catroppa2005; Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Morse, Klug, Catroppa, Haritou, Rosenfeld and Pentland1997, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse and Haritou1999, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Miner, Fletcher and Levin1989, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997, Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Barnes, Fletcher, Levin, Swank and Song2004a,Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Prasad, Landry, Kramer and DeLeonb). Compared with the OI group, children with severe TBI had poorer general cognitive ability as measured by the GCA and lower scores on tests of memory, spatial reasoning, executive function, and school readiness skills. The moderate TBI group performed more poorly than the OI group on tests of memory and executive function, but not in general ability. The moderate TBI-OI group contrasts had smaller effect sizes than the severe TBI-OI group contrasts, suggesting that children with moderate TBI group had more selective and less pronounced deficits than those with severe TBI. Specifically, visual-perceptual and memory skills were most clearly affected, and there was no evidence for effects of moderate TBI on what might be regarded as established language skills. The deficits observed in this group are consistent with past evidence of selective impairments in children with TBI and with expectations based on the brain regions most susceptible to insult in TBI (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Morse, Klug, Catroppa, Haritou, Rosenfeld and Pentland1997, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005b, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006; Donders & Giroux, Reference Donders and Giroux2005; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997; Levin & Hanten, Reference Levin and Hanten2005; Yeates, Reference Yeates, Yeates, Ris and Taylor2000). The present findings do not, however, contraindicate effects of TBI on language functions not tapped by our test battery, such as discourse processing and inferencing (Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Levin, Wanek, Weyrauch and Kufera1998; Dennis & Barnes, Reference Dennis and Barnes2001; Dennis et al., Reference Dennis, Purvis, Barnes, Wilkinson and Winner2001; Ewing-Cobbs & Barnes, Reference Ewing-Cobbs and Barnes2002; Morse et al., Reference Morse, Haritou, Ong, Anderson, Catroppa and Rosenfeld1999).

Although none of the mild TBI-OI contrasts was significant, the lower mean scores of the mild TBI group corresponded to medium effects sizes for several measures (Verbal Fluency, Recall of Digits, and the Shape School Simple Naming and Switch conditions). Given the limited statistical power for detection of these effects, results are interpreted as offering tentative support for postacute cognitive effects of mild TBI. Previous studies have yielded inconsistent effects of mild TBI, with some studies demonstrating adverse consequences for cognition or achievement (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2001; Dennis & Barnes, Reference Dennis and Barnes2001; Gronwall et al., Reference Gronwall, Wrightson and McGinn1997; Jaffe et al., Reference Jaffe, Fay, Polissar, Martin, Shurtleff, Rivera and Winn1992; McKinlay et al., Reference McKinlay, Dalrymple-Alford, Horwood and Fergusson2002; Ponsford et al., Reference Ponsford, Willmott, Rothwell, Cameron, Ayton, Nelms, Curran and Ng1999) and others not (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse and Haritou1999, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005b, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006). In a study comparing children with mild to moderate TBI with children hospitalized for other injuries, Goldstrohm and Arffa (Reference Goldstrohm and Arffa2005) found differences similar to ours, but they did not isolate the effects mild TBI. The consequences of such injuries may vary depending on when after TBI children are assessed and the criteria used to define mild injury (Bijur & Haslum, Reference Bijur, Haslum, Broman and Michel1995; Satz et al., Reference Satz, Zaucha, McCleary, Light, Asarnow and Becker1997). We recruited children who were hospitalized for at least 1 day and had some impairment in consciousness as defined by a GCS score of 13 or 14. For this reason, our mild TBI group may have had more significant trauma than children discharged home from emergency departments or with GCS scores of 15.

Additional Evidence for Effects of Injury Severity

As further evidence for a relationship between TBI severity and outcomes, scores on many of the same tests that discriminated the severe TBI and OI groups were linearly related to the degree of TBI. Tests of the linear trend across groups—from OI to mild, moderate, and severe TBI—demonstrated that TBI severity was associated with the Switch and Both conditions of Shape School, offering further evidence for negative consequences of TBI on executive function in young children. Analyses of the effects of GCS score and coma duration on outcomes within the TBI sample provided additional support for injury severity as a predictor of the cognitive and achievement outcomes of TBI in young children (Bowen et al., Reference Bowen, Clark, Bigler, Gardner, Nilsson, Gooch and Pompa1997; Foreman et al., Reference Foreman, Caesar, Parks, Madden, Gentilello, Shafi, Carlile, Harper and Diaz-Arrastia2007; Levin, Reference Levin1995; Prasad et al., Reference Prasad, Ewing-Cobbs, Swank and Kramer2002).

Noninjury Factors Related to Outcomes of TBI

Consistent with another study of young children (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006), better performance on nearly all measures was associated with higher SES, and these associations did not vary by group. The finding of higher scores in girls than boys for the majority of the measures was unexpected given the general absence of sex differences in on our previous research on a school-age TBI cohort (Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Wade, Drotar, Stancin and Minich2002). Gender differences in rates of developmental disorders or early language skills may help to explain this finding (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Caruso and Ellerbeck2003). Alternatively, a male predilection for preinjury developmental problems may have been exacerbated in our sample if these disorders contributed more to risk of injury in boys than in girls.

The findings also documented moderating influences of noninjury factors on some of the group differences. In keeping with results of our previous study of older children with TBI (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Stancin and Minich2002; Yeates et al., Reference Yeates, Taylor, Drotar, Wade, Klein, Stancin and Schatschneider1997), weaknesses in Naming Vocabulary in the severe TBI group were evident only at lower levels of SES, and weaknesses in Spelling in the moderate TBI group were found only at higher levels of parent stressors. Potential explanations for these findings are that these skills were not as well established in children from less advantaged backgrounds and thus more easily disrupted by TBI, or that fewer resources were available after injury to support postinjury recovery (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Yeates, Wade, Drotar, Stancin and Minich2002). The fact that both measures entail knowledge-based skills is consistent with these interpretations.

Results did not reveal moderating effects of age at injury on outcomes of TBI. The lack of evidence for worse outcomes in younger children contrasts with results from previous studies that have examined TBI across wider age spans (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2005a; Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Dennis and Wilkinson1999; Dennis et al., Reference Dennis, Wilkinson, Koski, Humphreys, Broman and Michel1995; Ewing-Cobbs & Barnes, Reference Ewing-Cobbs and Barnes2002; Levin et al., Reference Levin, Ewing-Cobbs, Eisenberg, Broman and Michel1995; Verger et al., Reference Verger, Junque, Jurado, Tresserras, Bartumeus, Nogues and Poch2000) and suggests that outcomes may be less strongly related to age at injury during early childhood (Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Fletcher, Levin, Francis, Davidson and Miner1997). Alternatively, the impact of younger age at injury may become more pronounced with increasing time since injury (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2000a,Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Rosenfeld, Haritou and Morseb; Ewing-Cobbs et al., Reference Ewing-Cobbs, Barnes, Fletcher, Levin, Swank and Song2004a).

Limitations

Despite measuring a wide range of skills and assessing outcomes of TBI relative to those of an orthopedic injury group, the study was limited in several respects. First, only clinical neuroimaging, in most instances CT scans, were available to determine the presence and nature of the brain insults in the TBI sample. Imaging methods with greater sensitivity to white matter damage and focal lesions would likely have provided information useful in enhancing prediction of variations in outcomes of TBI (Scheibel & Levin, Reference Scheibel, Levin, Drasnegor, Lyon and Goldman-Rakic1997; Wilde et al., Reference Wilde, Chu, Bigler, Hunter, Fearing, Hanten, Newsome, Scheibel, Li and Levin2006). Second, the mild and severe TBI groups were relatively small despite recruitment from multiple hospitals, and larger group sizes would have increased statistical power for detection of effects. Third, the lowest GCS score was used to define injury severity. This measure likely over-estimated TBI severity in some of the children who were sedated and intubated, though past studies showed that efforts to remove this confound yielded little improvement in predictive validity (Foreman et al., Reference Foreman, Caesar, Parks, Madden, Gentilello, Shafi, Carlile, Harper and Diaz-Arrastia2007; Zafonte et al., Reference Zafonte, Hammond, Mann, Wood, Black and Millis1996). Finally, we failed to assess some important cognitive outcomes, such as speed of processing (Catroppa & Anderson, Reference Catroppa and Anderson2005), and we examined individual test scores rather than multiple-indicator latent constructs. The latter approach would have allowed us to more clearly distinguish the skills most and least affected by TBI and reduced the probability of Type I error.

CONCLUSIONS AND IMPLICATIONS

Study findings suggest several conclusions with respect to the postacute effects of TBI on young children, and provide a basis for formulating more specific hypotheses about how TBI affects young children:

  1. 1. Severe TBI sustained during early childhood can result in generalized cognitive impairment and deficits in school readiness skills. Furthermore, memory, spatial reasoning, and executive function may be more affected than some language skills.

  2. 2. Children with moderate TBI have more specific and less pronounced impairments than those with severe TBI.

  3. 3. Indices of TBI severity, including the GCS score and coma duration, are related to cognitive and school readiness skills.

  4. 4. Although age at injury is not related to most postacute effects of TBI on cognitive and school readiness skills, at least within the 3- to 6-year-old age range, further follow-up is needed to determine whether age-at-injury effects emerge with increasing time since injury.

  5. 5. Measures of environmental disadvantage predict lower scores on most tests in children with and without TBI but also amplify the effects of TBI on some tests.

The primary clinical implication of these findings is that young children with TBI are at risk for postacute deficits in cognition and school readiness skills and that measures used in this study, or similar ones, are sensitive to these deficits. Although generalized cognitive deficits may be present in more severely injured children, even those with less severe TBI may be at risk for weaknesses in memory and executive function. As early academic readiness skills appear to be compromised, children who have sustained a recent TBI may be vulnerable to learning difficulties as they begin formal academic instruction. Monitoring of cognitive and readiness skills is thus recommended relatively soon after hospital discharge. Based on research showing slow recovery and emerging deficits in this age group (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2000a,Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Rosenfeld, Haritou and Morseb, Reference Anderson, Morse, Catroppa, Haritou and Rosenfeld2004, Reference Anderson, Catroppa, Dudgeon, Morse, Haritou and Rosenfeld2006), ongoing follow-up is additionally advised. The pattern of deficits observed in the present TBI sample also provides clues as to potentially useful educational interventions. In view of weaknesses in nonverbal abilities and executive function, children who are experiencing new-onset learning problems after TBI may benefit from structured teaching methods. Similarly, memory deficits suggest that it may be useful to provide these children with memory cues, repeated presentations of materials, and opportunities to practice newly taught skills.

Another clinical implication of the findings is the need to consider both injury-related and environmental factors in assessing risks for adverse outcomes of TBI. Lower SES and higher TBI severity contributed independently to poorer performance on many of the tests, but lower SES and parent stressors also potentiated the negative effects of TBI on some outcomes. These results underscore the complexity of influences on the recovery process (Taylor, Reference Taylor2004).

Further research is required to better understand the consequences of the full range of TBI severity on young children's brain development, and how brain pathology maps onto neurobehavioral outcomes. Larger scale studies of outcome will also be required, especially in examining the consequences of mild TBI. Further research on this subset of children might explore factors contributing to the decision to hospitalize these children and track acute changes in cognitive functioning. Assuming that mild TBI results in residual deficits in only a minority of children with mild TBI (Bigler, Reference Bigler2008; Kirkwood et al., Reference Kirkwood, Yeates, Taylor, Randolph, McCrea and Andersonin press), the identification of ways to distinguish affected from unaffected individuals may be more helpful than studies of group differences. The development of age-appropriate tests that clarify the effects of TBI and study of the factors that contribute to skill development following injury will be essential in improving our capacity to identify and treat the negative consequences of TBI in young children. We are currently following the present cohort to determine the nature of changes in cognitive and achievement outcomes across time post injury, evaluate behavioral sequelae, and investigate the effects of family factors on outcomes.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Supported by grant R01 HD42729 to Dr. Wade from NICHD, in part by USPHS NIH Grant no. M01 RR 08084, and by Trauma Research grants from the State of Ohio Emergency Medical Services. The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of Christine Abraham, Andrea Beebe, Lori Bernard, Anne Birnbaum, Beth Bishop, Tammy Matecun, Karen Oberjohn, Elizabeth Roth, and Elizabeth Shaver in data collection and coding. We also thank Nori Minich for her assistance in data analysis. The Cincinnati Children's Medical Center Trauma Registry, Rainbow Pediatric Trauma Center, Rainbow Babies & Children's Hospital, Columbus Children's Hospital Trauma Program, and MetroHealth Center Department of Pediatrics and Trauma Registry provided assistance with recruitment.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, V. & Catroppa, C. (2005). Recovery of executive skills following paediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI): A 2-year follow-up. Brain Injury, 19, 459470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Catroppa, C., Dudgeon, P., Morse, S.A., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J.V. (2006). Understanding predictors of functional recovery and outcome 30 months following early childhood head injury. Neuropsychology, 20, 4257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Catroppa, C., Morse, S.A., & Haritou, F. (1999). Functional memory skills following traumatic brain injury in young children. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 3, 159166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. (2000a). Recovery of intellectual ability following traumatic brain injury in childhood: Impact of injury severity and age at injury. Pediatric Neurosurgery, 32, 282290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. (2001). Outcome from mild head injury in young children: A prospective study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 705717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. (2005a). Functional plasticity or vulnerability after early brain injury? Pediatrics, 116, 13741382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Catroppa, C., Morse, S., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J. (2005b). Attentional and processing skills following traumatic brain injury in early childhood. Brain Injury, 19, 699710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Catroppa, C., Rosenfeld, J., Haritou, F., & Morse, S.A. (2000b). Recovery of memory function following traumatic brain injury in pre-school children. Brain Injury, 14, 679692.Google ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V. & Moore, C. (1995). Age at injury as a predictor of outcome following pediatric head injury: A longitudinal perspective. Child Neuropsychology, 1, 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, V.A., Morse, S.A., Catroppa, C., Haritou, F., & Rosenfeld, J.V. (2004). Thirty month outcome from early childhood head injury: A prospective analysis of neurobehavioural recovery. Brain, 127, 26082620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V.A., Morse, S.A., Klug, G., Catroppa, C., Haritou, F., Rosenfeld, J.V., & Pentland, L. (1997). Predicting recovery from head injury in young children: A prospective analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 568580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, M.A., Dennis, M., & Wilkinson, M. (1999). Reading after closed head injury in childhood: Effects on accuracy, fluency, and comprehension. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15, 124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bawden, H.G., Knights, R.M., & Winogron, H.W. (1985). Speeded performance following head injury in children. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 3954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigler, E.D. (2008). Neuropsychology and clinical neuroscience of persistent post-concussive syndrome. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bijur, P.E. & Haslum, M. (1995). Cognitive, behavioral, and motoric sequelae of mild head injury in a national birth cohort. In Broman, S. & Michel, M.E. (Eds.), Traumatic head injury in children (pp. 147164). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowen, J.M., Clark, E., Bigler, E.D., Gardner, M., Nilsson, D., Gooch, J., & Pompa, J. (1997). Childhood traumatic brain injury: Neuropsychological status at the time of hospital discharge. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 39, 1725.Google ScholarPubMed
Bracken, B.A. (1998). Bracken Basic Concept Scale-Revised Examiner's Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bronson, M. (2000). Self-Regulation in Early Childhood: Nature and Nurture. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (2000). Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Catroppa, C. & Anderson, V. (2005). A prospective study of the recovery of attention from acute to 2 years following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 8498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chadwick, O., Rutter, M., Brown, G., Shaffer, D., & Traub, M. (1981). A prospective study of children with head injuries: II. Cognitive sequelae. Psychological Medicine, 11, 4961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, S.B., Levin, H.S., Wanek, A., Weyrauch, J., & Kufera, J. (1998). Discourse after closed head injury in young children. Brain and Language, 61, 420449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M. & Barnes, M.A. (2001). Comparison of literal, inferential, and intentional text comprehension in children with mild or severe closed head injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 16, 456468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M., Purvis, K., Barnes, M.A., Wilkinson, M., & Winner, E. (2001). Understanding of literal truth, ironic criticism, and deceptive praise following childhood head injury. Brain and Language, 78, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M., Wilkinson, M., Koski, L., & Humphreys, R.P. (1995). Attention deficits in the long term after childhood head injury. In Broman, S. & Michel, M.E. (Eds.), Traumatic Head Injury in Children (pp. 165187). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Donders, J. (2001). Using a short form of the WISC-III: Sinful or smart? Child Neuropsychology, 7, 99103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donders, J. & Giroux, A. (2005). Discrepancies between the California Verbal Learning Test: Children's Version and the Children's Category Test after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 386391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, C. (1990). Differential Ability Scales: Introductory and Technical Handbook. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Espy, K.A. (1997). The Shape School: Assessing executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 495499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Espy, K.A., Kaufman, P.M., McDiarmid, M.D., & Glishy, M.L. (1999). Executive functioning in preschool children: Performance on the A-Not-B and other delayed response format tasks. Brain and Cognition, 41, 178199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing-Cobbs, L. & Barnes, M. (2002). Linguistic outcomes following traumatic brain injury in children. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 9, 209217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Barnes, M., Fletcher, J.M., Levin, H.S., Swank, P.R., & Song, J. (2004a). Modeling of longitudinal academic achievement scores after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 107133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Fletcher, J.M., Levin, H.S., Francis, D.J., Davidson, K., & Miner, M.E. (1997). Longitudinal neuropsychological outcomes in infants and preschoolers with traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 581591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Miner, M.E., Fletcher, J.M., & Levin, H.S. (1989). Intellectual, motor, and language sequelae following closed head injury in infants and preschoolers. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 14, 531547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ewing-Cobbs, L., Prasad, M.R., Landry, S.H., Kramer, L., & DeLeon, R. (2004b). Executive functions following traumatic brain injury in young children: A preliminary analysis. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 487512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fay, G.C., Jaffe, K.M., Polissar, M.L., Liao, S., Rivara, J.B., & Martin, K.M. (1994). Outcome of pediatric traumatic brain injury at three years: A cohort study. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 75, 733741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, J.M., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Francis, D.J., & Levin, H.S. (1995). Variability in outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children: A developmental perspective. In Broman, S.H. & Michel, M.E. (Eds.), Traumatic Head Injury in Children (pp. 321). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J.M., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Miner, M., Levin, H., & Eisenberg, H. (1990). Behavioral changes after closed head injury in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 9398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foreman, B.P., Caesar, R.R., Parks, J., Madden, C., Gentilello, L.M., Shafi, S., Carlile, M.C., Harper, C.R., & Diaz-Arrastia, R.R. (2007). Usefulness of the Abbreviated Injury Score and the Injury Severity Score in comparison to the Glasgow Coma Scale in predicting outcome after traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Trauma, 62, 946950.Google Scholar
Goldstrohm, S.L. & Arffa, S. (2005). Preschool children with mild to moderate traumatic brain injury: An exploration of immediate and post-acute morbidity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 675695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotschall, C.S. (1993). Epidemiology of childhood injury. In Eichelberger, M.R. (Ed.), Pediatric Trauma: Prevention, Acute Care, and Rehabilitation (pp. 1619). St. Louis, MO: Mosby Yearbooks.Google Scholar
Gronwall, D., Wrightson, P., & McGinn, V. (1997). Effect of mild head injury during the preschool years. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 592597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaccard, J. & Guilamo-Ramos, V. (2002). Analysis of variance frameworks in clinical child and adolescent psychology: Issues and recommendations. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 130146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaffe, K.M, Fay, G.C., Polissar, N.L., Martin, K.M., Shurtleff, H., Rivera, J.B., & Winn, H.R. (1992). Severity of pediatric traumatic brain injury and early neurobehavioral outcome: A cohort study. American Academy of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 73, 540547.Google ScholarPubMed
Keenan, H.T., Hooper, S.R., Wetherington, C.E., Nocera, M., & Runyan, D. (2007). Neurodevelopmental consequences of early traumatic brain injury in 3-year-old children. Pediatrics, 119, e616e623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkwood, M.W., Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Randolph, C, McCrea, M., & Anderson, V.A. (in press). Management of pediatric mild traumatic brain injury: A neuropsychological review from injury through recovery. The Clinical Neuropsychologist.Google Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K.T., & Harlan, E.T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36, 220232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY–A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Kraus, J.F. (1995). Epidemiological features of brain injury in children: Occurrence, children at risk, causes and manner of injury, severity, and outcomes. In Broman, S.H. & Michell, M.E. (Eds.), Traumatic Head Injury in Children (pp. 2239). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Langlois, J.A., Rutland-Brown, W., & Thomas, K.E. (2006). Traumatic Brain Injury in the United States: Emergency Department Visits, Hospitalizations, and Deaths. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control.Google Scholar
Levin, H.S. (1995). Prediction of recovery from traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 12, 913922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, H.S., Aldrich, E.F., Saydjari, C., Eisenberg, H.M., Foulkes, M.A., Bellefleur, M., Luerssen, T.G., Jane, J.A., Marmarou, A., Marshall, L.F., & Young, H.F. (1992). Severe head injury in children: Experience of the Traumatic Coma Data Bank. Neurosurgery, 31, 435444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, H.S., Ewing-Cobbs, L., & Eisenberg, H.M. (1995). Neurobehavioral outcome of pediatric head injury. In Broman, S. & Michel, M.E. (Eds.), Traumatic Head Injury in Children (pp. 7094). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levin, H.S. & Hanten, G. (2005). Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatric Neurology, 33, 7993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, P.A. (1995). Sex, race, class, and other demographics as explanations for children's ability and adjustment: A national appraisal. Journal of School Psychology, 33, 7591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKinlay, A., Dalrymple-Alford, J.C., Horwood, L.J., & Fergusson, D.M. (2002). Long term psychosocial outcomes after mild head injury in early childhood. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 73, 281288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moos, R.H. & Moos, B.S. (1994). Life Stressors and Social Resources Inventory: Adult Form Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Morse, S., Haritou, F., Ong, K., Anderson, V., Catroppa, C., & Rosenfeld, J. (1999). Early effects of traumatic brain injury on young children's language performance: A preliminary linguistic analysis. Pediatric Rehabilitation, 3, 139148.Google Scholar
Osler, T., Bakker, S.P., & Long, W.A. (1997). A modification of the injury severity score that both improves accuracy and simplifies scoring. Journal of Trauma, 43, 922925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ponsford, J., Willmott, C., Rothwell, A., Cameron, P., Ayton, G., Nelms, R., Curran, G., & Ng, K.T. (1999). Cognitive and behavioral outcome following mild traumatic brain injury in children. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 14, 360372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prasad, M.R., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Swank, P.R., & Kramer, L. (2002). Predictors of outcome following traumatic brain injury in young children. Pediatric Neurosurgery, 36, 6474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satz, P., Zaucha, K., McCleary, C., Light, R., Asarnow, R., & Becker, D. (1997). Mild head injury in children and adolescents: A review of studies (1970–1995). Psychological Bulletin, 122, 107131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheibel, R.S. & Levin, H.S. (1997). Frontal lobe dysfunction following closed head injury in children: Findings from neuropsychology and brain imaging. In Drasnegor, N.A., Lyon, G.R., & Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (Eds.), Development of Prefrontal Cortex: Evolution, Neurobiology, and Behavior (pp. 241263). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Schwartz, L., Taylor, H.G., Drotar, D., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., & Stancin, T. (2003). Long-term behavior problems after pediatric traumatic brain injury: Prevalence, predictors, and correlates. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28, 251264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stancin, T., Drotar, D., Taylor, H.G., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., & Minich, N.M. (2002). Health-related quality of life of children and adolescents after traumatic brain injury. Pediatrics, 109, E34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabachnick, B.G. & Fidell, L.S. (1989). Using Multivariate Statistics (2nded.). New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G. (2004). Research on outcomes of pediatric traumatic brain injury: Current advances and future directions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 199225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H.G. & Alden, J. (1997). Age-related differences in outcomes following childhood brain insults: An introduction and overview. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H.G., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., Drotar, D., Klein, S.K., & Stancin, T. (1999). Influences on first-year recovery from traumatic brain injury in children. Neuropsychology, 13, 7689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, H.G., Yeates, K.O., Wade, S.L., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2002). A prospective study of short- and long-term outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children: Behavior and achievement. Neuropsychology, 16, 1527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teasdale, G. & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: A practical scale. Lancet, 2, 8184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, T., Caruso, M., & Ellerbeck, K. (2003). Sex matters in autism and other developmental disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 7, 345362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verger, K., Junque, C., Jurado, M.A., Tresserras, P., Bartumeus, F., Nogues, P, & Poch, J.M. (2000). Age effects on long-term neuropsychological outcome in paediatric traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 14, 495503.Google ScholarPubMed
Wetherington, C.E. & Hooper, S.R. (2006). Preschool traumatic brain injury: A review for the early childhood special educator. Exceptionality, 12, 155170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilde, E.A., Chu, Z., Bigler, E.D., Hunter, J.V., Fearing, M.A., Hanten, G., Newsome, M.R., Scheibel, R.S., Li, X., & Levin, H.S. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging in the corpus callosum in children after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 23, 14121426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R.W., McGrew, K.S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar
Yeates, K.O. (2000). Pediatric closed-head injury. In Yeates, K.O., Ris, M.D., & Taylor, H.G. (Eds.), Pediatric Neuropsychology: Research, Theory, and Practice (pp. 92116). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Yeates, K.O. & Taylor, H.G. (1997). Predicting premorbid neuropsychological functioning following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 825837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Drotar, D., Wade, S.L., Klein, S., Stancin, T., & Schatschneider, C. (1997). Pre-injury family environment as a determinant of recovery from traumatic brain injuries in school-age children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 617630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, K.O., Taylor, H.G., Wade, S.L., Drotar, D., Stancin, T., & Minich, N. (2002). A prospective study of short- and long-term neuropsychological outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children. Neuropsychology, 16, 514523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zafonte, R.D., Hammond, F.M., Mann, N.R., Wood, D.L., Black, K.L., & Millis, S.R. (1996). Relationship between Glasgow Coma Scale and functional outcome. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 75, 364369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample demographic characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2. Injury and medical characteristics

Figure 2

Table 3. Neuropsychological test battery

Figure 3

Table 4. Group comparisons on neuropsychological and school readiness measures