Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T17:38:09.314Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Temporal processing deficits in letter-by-letter reading by Ingles and Eskes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2006

H. BRANCH COSLETT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy

Extract

Based on his seminal contributions in 1891 and 1892, Dejerine is widely credited for initiating the investigation of acquired dyslexia. In the first contribution (1891) he described a patient who developed an inability to read and write after suffering an infarction of the left hemisphere. Dejerine designated the disorder “Alexia with Agraphia”; he attributed it to a loss of the “optical images” of words that were presumed to be supported by the angular gyrus. In 1892, Dejerine described a patient who was able to write but could not read what he (and others) had written (Dejerine, 1892). This disorder, variously known as Alexia without Agraphia, Pure Alexia or Agnosic Alexia, has been reported with regularity since the original account. Although subsequent reports have noted aspects of the disorder not emphasized in Dejerine's index case—pure alexia may be associated with surface (Patterson & Kay, 1982) or deep (Buxbaum & Coslett, 1996) dyslexia—the essential features of the disorder were well documented by Dejerine and the syndrome remains quite consistent with respect to its core phenomenology. Similarly, the pathologic substrate of the disorder has proven to be remarkably constant: the vast majority of patients with pure alexia have lesions involving the occipital lobe of the dominant hemisphere and either the forceps major or, less commonly, the splenium of the corpus callosum. The latter component of the lesion interrupts the white matter tracts connecting the visual association cortices of the right and left hemispheres.

Type
NEUROBEHAVIORAL GRAND ROUNDS—INTRODUCTION
Copyright
© 2007 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Arguin, M., Bub, D., & Bowers, J. (1998). Extent and limits of covert lexical activation in letter-by-letter reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 5392.Google Scholar
Behrmann, M., Nelson, J., & Sekuler, E.B. (1998). Visual complexity in letter-by-letter reading: “Pure” alexia is not pure. Neuropsychologia, 36, 11151132.Google Scholar
Behrmann, M. & Shallice, T. (1995). Pure alexia: A nonspatial visual disorder affecting letter activation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 409454.Google Scholar
Buxbaum, L. & Coslett, H.B. (1996). Deep dyslexic phenomenon in a letter-by-letter reader. Brain and Language, 54, 136167.Google Scholar
Coslett, H.B. & Saffran, E.M. (1989). Evidence for preserved reading in “pure alexia.” Brain, 112, 327359.Google Scholar
Coslett, H.B., Saffran, E.M., Greenbaum, S., & Schwartz, H. (1993). Preserved reading in pure alexia: The effect of strategy. Brain, 116, 2137.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. & Damasio, H. (1983). The anatomic basis of pure alexia. Neurology, 33, 15731583.Google Scholar
Fiset, D., Arguin, M., Bub, D., Humphreys, G.W., & Riddoch, M.J. (2005). How to make the word-length effect disappear in letter-by-letter dyslexi. Psychological Science, 16, 535.Google Scholar
Dejerine, J. (1891). C. R. Sur un cas de cecite verbale avec agraphie, suivi d'autopsie. Societe du Biologie 43, 197201.Google Scholar
Dejerine, J. (1892). Contribution a l'etude anatomoclinique et clinique des differentes varietes de cecite verbale.
Farah, M.J. & Wallace, M. (1991). Pure alexia as a visual impairment: A reconsideration. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 313334.Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnection syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237294.Google Scholar
Greenblatt, S.H. (1973). Alexia without agraphia. Brain, 96, 307316.Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. & Warrington, E.K. (1962). A disorder of simultaneous form perception. Brain, 85, 461486.Google Scholar
Landis, T., Regard, M., & Serrat, A. (1980). Iconic reading in a case of alexia without agraphia caused by a brain tumor: A tachistoscopic study. Brain and Language, 11, 4553.Google Scholar
Patterson, K.E. & Kay, J. (1982). Letter-by-letter reading: Psychological descriptions of a neurological syndrome. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 411441.Google Scholar
Raymond, J.E., Shapiro, K.L., & Arnell, K.M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 849860.Google Scholar
Saffran, E.M. & Coslett, H.B. (1998). “Implicit vs. Letter-by-letter reading in pure alexia: A tale of two systems.” Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 141166.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. & Saffran, E. (1986). Lexical processing in the absence of explicit word identification: Evidence from a letter-by-letter reader. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 429458.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. & Warrington, E.K. (1980). Single and multiple component central dyslexic syndromes. In M. Coltheart, K.E. Patterson, & J.C. Marshall (Eds.), Deep Dyslexia. London: Routledge.
Warrington, E.K. & Langdon, D. (1994). Spelling dyslexia: A deficit of the visual word-form. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 57, 211216.Google Scholar