INTRODUCTION
Social cognition broadly refers to one’s ability to perceive and understand other people’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors and to respond appropriately. The most researched aspects of social cognition are emotion recognition, theory of mind (ToM) and empathy. Emotion recognition is the ability to identify and discriminate emotional states from verbal and nonverbal cues. Theory of mind refers to the ability to understand what other people are thinking and feeling and to infer complex mental states, such as intention and disposition, in others. Empathy is the ability to understand and respond to the emotional experiences of others and has been shown to contribute to successful social interaction and to promote pro-social actions (Sun et al., Reference Sun, Vuillier, Hui and Kogan2019). The literature typically differentiates between two components of empathy: affective empathy, which entails affective sharing of other people’s emotional states, and cognitive empathy, which refers to the ability to decode and understand other people’s perspective (Singer & Lamm, Reference Singer and Lamm2009).
Social cognition is complex and multifaceted, subserved by an intricate network of interconnected brain regions collectively referred to as the “social brain.” These regions include the amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, temporoparietal junction, anterior cingulate, and insula. Many of these structures are adversely affected in conditions such as schizophrenia (Green et al., Reference Green, Horan and Lee2015), stroke (Hillis, Reference Hillis2014), and neurodegenerative disorders (Christidi et al., Reference Christidi, Migliaccio, Santamaría-García, Santangelo and Trojsi2018), giving rise to the propensity for social cognitive impairment in these groups. Social cognitive impairment is a feature of many developmental, psychiatric, neurological and neurodegenerative conditions (Cotter et al., Reference Cotter, Granger, Backx, Hobbs, Looi and Barnett2018). Not surprising, people with epilepsy also demonstrate impairments in social cognition that negatively impact the quality of life, employability, and other cognitive functions (for instance, Giovagnoli et al., Reference Giovagnoli, Franceschetti, Reati, Parente, Maccagnano, Villani and Spreafico2011; Giovagnoli et al., Reference Giovagnoli, Parente, Villani, Franceschetti and Spreafico2013). Despite the increasing volume of studies investigating social cognition in epilepsy over recent years, research into the predictors of social cognitive impairment in this population remains lacking. Specifically, the extent to which social cognitive difficulties are caused by medication, psychological, and social factors (e.g., fear of seizures, perceived stigma, discrimination, lack of social support), recurrent seizures, or the underlying epileptogenic brain lesion, remains unclear. Thus, identifying the nature and magnitude of social cognitive impairments in people with epilepsy has both theoretical and clinical implications, including the potential to inform guidelines for clinical assessment and psychosocial intervention.
Four prior meta-analyses evaluated theory of mind and emotion recognition in patients with epilepsy. Outcomes differed slightly depending on the groups of patients studied and the outcome measures used. The chief findings of the studies were: (A) theory of mind ability was affected in patients with frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE) and temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), but not from seizure disorders originating outside these areas (extra-TLE/FLE; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa and Lah2016); (B) deficient recognition of fear, followed by sadness and disgust, in both visual and auditory modalities was reported in patients with epilepsy (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Stewart, Palermo and Lah2017; Monti et al., 2015); (C) patients with right TLE exhibited more significant impairments in recognition of fear, disgust, and sadness than patient with left TLE (Bora & Meletti, Reference Bora and Meletti2016); and (D) patients undergoing surgery did not differ in social cognitive outcomes (emotion recognition and theory of mind) compared to those not undergoing surgery (Bora & Meletti, Reference Bora and Meletti2016).
While these previous meta-analytic reviews are a valuable contribution to the literature, it is still unknown how empathy and social behavior are affected by epilepsy. This study is the first to evaluate all four major components of social cognition: emotion recognition, theory of mind, empathy, and social behavior, in people with TLE and FLE. We acknowledge that social cognition is a multidimensional construct and significantly influenced by personality vulnerabilities, mood disturbance and cognitive impairment. However, for the purpose of this review, we examine the evidence for four social cognitive components previously investigated in other clinical populations (Henry et al., Reference Henry, von Hippel, Molenberghs, Lee and Sachdev2016; Kumfor et al., Reference Kumfor, Hazelton, De Winter, de Langavant and Van den Stock2017; Adams et al., Reference Adams, Schweitzer, Molenberghs and Henry2019). Our primary aims were twofold: (A) to determine whether social cognitive impairment is global or specific to one domain of social cognition in people with TLE and FLE, and (B) to investigate whether the severity and patterns of impairment are moderated by the location of the epileptic focus in the temporal vs. the frontal lobe. We conclude this review with a suggested pathway and clinically useful tools to support the screening of social cognition in people with epilepsy when indicators of potential compromise are identified.
METHOD
This systematic review was conducted in line with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman and The2009). The completed PRISMA checklist is presented in the supplementary material.
Identification of Studies
Search strategy
We searched several databases from 1990 to January 2021 including PsycINFO and MEDLINE (via EBSCOhost), Embase, and Web of Science Core Collection (via Clarivate Analytics). We constructed a comprehensive search strategy using variant terms for the focal epilepsy subtypes and social cognition constructs. The search strategy developed for articles is presented below;
(“epilep*” OR “focal epilepsy” OR “temporal lobe epilepsy” OR “frontal lobe epilepsy” OR “seizure” OR ((“amygdala” OR “hippocamp*”) AND “damage”)) AND (“perc*” OR “identif*” OR “recogni*” OR “process*” OR “label*”) AND (“emotion recognition” OR ((“face” OR “facial”) AND (“affect*” OR “emotion*” OR “expression*”)) OR “theory of mind” OR “social cognition” OR “social perception” OR “perspective task*” OR “mentalis*” OR “mind read*” OR “empath*” OR “social competence” OR “social outcome*” OR “social adjustment” OR “social behavio*” OR “social skill*” OR “social interaction*”).
Duplicate results were removed automatically in EndNote. Using the Ancestry Method, reference lists were searched for appropriate reviews and eligible studies to identify additional papers. Then titles and abstracts were screened by two authors (MZ & CA). Full-text of papers were reviewed when it was unclear whether an article fulfilled the eligibility criteria based on the title and abstract alone. Two authors (MZ & CA) independently screened the full text articles against the inclusion criteria and discrepancies were resolved through discussion.
Inclusion criteria
The review included studies that: 1. reported primary research published in English-language peer-reviewed journals; 2. included samples of people diagnosed with temporal lobe (TLE) or frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE) and reported the data separately for each patient group; 3. included a nonclinical comparison group; 4. used validated measures evaluating each of the four components of social cognition (as detailed in Table 1); and 5. included either pediatric or adult patient populations.
Exclusion criteria
The review excluded: 1. case studies, nonclinical outcome studies (e.g., psychometric validation studies), secondary research or special papers (e.g., reviews, editorial, commentaries, or letters); 2. studies that did not include a control group; 3. studies that recruited heterogenous focal epilepsy groups (e.g., multifocal epilepsy from frontal and temporal lobes); 4. studies that used qualitative measures such as interviews or behavioral tasks other than those specified in Table 1; 6. studies with inadequate data to calculate a mean or weighted effect; 7. fMRI studies that investigated one of these domains in TLE or FLE patients but without overt, explicit, behavioral measures during the fMRI task; 8. studies that included patients with unilateral or bilateral amygdala or hippocampal damage, but without seizures; 9. studies that included only postoperative patients, or grouped patients with and without lobectomy together. Studies were not excluded based on other premorbid conditions, cause of epilepsy (e.g., acquired lesions, tumors, congenital structural abnormalities), or medication use.
Definition of Social Cognitive Domains
Definition of each social cognitive domain is extensively discussed in the supplementary materials. In short, in the following we provide a brief description of each domain:
Emotion recognition
we defined emotion recognition as any task that required participants to label, recognize, rate, match or select the emotions expressed within the stimuli. Stimuli could be visual or auditory and may consist of static faces, videos, or sounds. All eligible studies evaluating emotion recognition and their effect sizes are reported in Table 2.
Note: N = number; MTLE = mesial temporal lobe epilepsy; TLE = temporal lobe epilepsy; FLE = frontal lobe epilepsy; AAD = asymmetrical amygdala damage; FCE = fronto-central epilepsy; No. AED = Average of number of antiepileptic drugs; NS = not specified; SR = systematic review; MA = meta-analysis.
Theory of mind
we defined theory of mind as the ability to understand others’ thinking and feeling and included studies that used measures that were commonly used in the social cognitive literature such as False Belief, Faux Pas, or Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test. All eligible studies evaluating theory of mind and their effect sizes are reported in Table 3.
Note: N = number; MTLE = mesial temporal lobe epilepsy; TLE = temporal lobe epilepsy; FLE = frontal lobe epilepsy; IED = interictal epileptiform discharges; ATL = Anterior temporal lobectomy; No. AED = Average of number of antiepileptic drugs; NS = not specified; SR = systematic review; MA = meta-analysis.
Empathy
We included studies that looked at the generalized construct as well as the cognitive and affective sub-components that assesses how individuals perceive others’ emotions and perspective and how they share emotional states with others, respectively. All eligible studies evaluating empathy and their effect sizes are reported in Table 4.
Note: N = number; MTLE = mesial temporal lobe epilepsy; TLE = temporal lobe epilepsy; FLE = frontal lobe epilepsy; IED = interictal epileptiform discharges; No. AED = Average of number of antiepileptic drugs; NS = not specified; SR = systematic review; MA = meta-analysis.
Social behavior
Given that social behavior has been operationalized differently in the literature, we only included studies that employed one or more widely used measures considered relevant to this construct such as Social Responsiveness Scale, Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and Social Adjustment Scale. All eligible studies evaluating social behavior and their effect sizes are reported in Table 4.
Data Extraction and Data Entry
We extracted the available participant characteristics (e.g., mean age, standard deviation, and number of participants) in each clinical and nonclinical control group for each study. For clinical groups, we recorded the mean age of onset, duration of epilepsy, and the number of seizures per month, where available. For each social cognition domain, the mean and standard deviation of items reported (accuracy or errors) for each task was extracted for each clinical and nonclinical control group. Where the mean of errors was reported (Amlerova et al., Reference Amlerova, Cavanna, Bradac, Javurkova, Raudenska and Marusic2014; Cohn et al., Reference Cohn, St-Laurent, Barnett and McAndrews2015; Laurent et al., Reference Laurent, Arzimanoglou, Panagiotakaki, Sfaello, Kahane, Ryvlin and de Schonen2014; Schacher et al., Reference Schacher, Winkler, Grunwald, Kraemer, Kurthen, Reed and Jokeit2006), the effect size calculation was reversed to account for this difference in the direction of effect. Alternatively, for studies where raw values were not available, other statistics such as events rates (Gomez-ibanez et al., Reference Gomez-Ibanez, Urrestarazu and Viteri2014), p-values and sample size were used to calculate effect sizes (Amlerova et al., Reference Amlerova, Cavanna, Bradac, Javurkova, Raudenska and Marusic2014; Batut et al., Reference Batut, Gounot, Namer, Hirsch, Kehrli and Metz-Lutz2006; Meletti et al., Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Rubboli, Cantalupo, Nichelli and Tassinari2003b; Schacher et al., Reference Schacher, Winkler, Grunwald, Kraemer, Kurthen, Reed and Jokeit2006). We contacted authors to request subgroup raw values when focal epilepsy groups or laterality data were combined. Data were made available in several instances (Cohn et al., Reference Cohn, St-Laurent, Barnett and McAndrews2015; Laurent et al., Reference Laurent, Arzimanoglou, Panagiotakaki, Sfaello, Kahane, Ryvlin and de Schonen2014; Tanaka et al., Reference Tanaka, Akamatsu, Yamano, Nakagawa, Kawamura and Tsuji2013; Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Rankin, Huppertz, Kurthen and Jokeit2015a, Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Grunwald, Huppertz, Kurthen, Rankin and Jokeit2015b; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019b; Bujarski et al., Reference Bujarski, Flashman, Li, Tosteson, Jobst, Thadani and Roth2016; Morou et al., Reference Morou, Papaliagkas, Markouli, Karagianni, Nazlidou, Spilioti and Kosmidis2018; Lunn et al., Reference Lunn, Lewis and Sherlock2015). Studies that only included postoperative patients were excluded from the estimation of effect sizes (e.g., Ammerlaan et al., Reference Ammerlaan, Hendriks, Colon and Kessels2008; Brierley et al., Reference Brierley, Medford, Shaw and David2004; Jiang et al., Reference Jiang, Hu, Wang, Zhou, Zhu and Wang2014; Gosselin et al., Reference Gosselin, Peretz, Hasboun, Baulac and Samson2011; Boucher et al., Reference Boucher, Rouleau, Lassonde, Lepore, Bouthillier and Nguyen2015). Data published more than 10 years ago were assumed to be unavailable.
For studies with more than one outcome measure in each social-cognitive domain, effect sizes were pooled to calculate an overall effect for each construct. If studies reported both pre- and postoperative data, only preoperative data were included. For studies in which different intensities of emotional expression were used to evaluate emotion recognition, we included the data for the 100% intensity of facial expression in the meta-analyses and the remaining intensity data were narratively synthesized (Sedda et al., Reference Sedda, Rivolta, Scarpa, Burt, Frigerio, Zanardi and Bottini2013). For studies in which different task instructions were used, the index reported by the authors was included in the analysis (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Lawrence, Bramham, Brierley, Radbourne and David2007). Due to their limited number, studies employing widely used measures of social behavior and empathy are discussed as part of the systematic review but results from meta-analyses are provisional and require replication in future. Second author extracted the data and First author, MZ, checked the accuracy of extracted data independently.
Tables 2–4 present a study-by-study breakdown of effect sizes for each domain of social cognition in patients compared to nonclinical controls.
Quality Rating
The Downs and Black Checklist (Reference Downs and Black1998) was used to evaluate the methodological quality of included studies in the meta-analysis. The checklist includes measures for the psychometric properties of the papers for randomized and non-randomized studies. The checklist shows good test-retest reliability (r = .88), inter-rater reliability (r = .75), and internal consistency (Kruder-Richardson formula 20 = .89 (Downs & Black, Reference Downs and Black1998). The adapted 17-item version of this checklist was used; items related to interventional trials were omitted as studies of this nature were not included for review. This checklist evaluates the quality of reporting (items 1–8), external validity (item 9), internal validity (statistical and methodological bias, items 10–12; selection bias, items 13–16), and power (item 17). All items are scored from 0 (no, or unable to be determined) to 1 (yes), except for item 4, which is scored 0 (no, unable to be determined), 1 (partially), or 2 (yes). Items 7 and 16 were only scored for studies with a longitudinal design. Therefore, scores for cross-sectional studies, ranged from 0–16, while longitudinal studies scores ranged from 0–18. Studies were categorized into three categories with high (0–5 points for cross-sectional and longitudinal studies), average (6–10 points for cross-sectional and 6–11 points for longitudinal), and low (11–16 points for cross-sectional and 12–18 points for longitudinal studies) risk of bias. A similar procedure to that reported by Edwards et al. (Reference Edwards, Stewart, Palermo and Lah2017); Stewart et al. (Reference Stewart, Catroppa and Lah2016) was used for quality rating using this checklist. Two authors (MZ & CA) independently reviewed and scored all papers and discrepancies were resolved through discussion.
Methods of Review
Figure 1 displays the flow diagram describing the process of study selection for the review. The initial search retrieved 6181 articles. After duplicates were removed, 3847 titles and abstracts were screened for the relevance and eligibility of the papers, 108 full texts were assessed for the final eligibility independently by two authors (90% agreement). Forty-four eligible papers remained for the systematic review, 40 of which were included for meta-analysis.
Patient Demographics
Overall studies included 2830 nonclinical control participants and 1595 patients with epilepsy, 113 patients with FLE and 1482 patients with TLE. The age ranged from 10 to 48 years in the nonclinical group and 12 to 52 years in the patient group. For the patient group, the mean age-of-onset of epilepsy was 13 years for the emotion recognition studies, 13 years for theory of mind studies, 17 years for the empathy studies, and 20 years for social behavior studies. The average duration of epilepsy was 12 years for emotion recognition studies, 11 years for theory of mind studies, 16 years for empathy studies, 10 years for social behavior studies.
Statistical Analysis
Data were analyzed using the Comprehensive Meta Analyses Program, Version 3 (Borenstein et al., Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins and Rothstein2013). Effect sizes were calculated as Hedges’ g. We interpreted the importance and strength of our effect in line with the Cohen’s d guidelines, with 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 corresponding to small, medium, and large effects, respectively (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). A negative effect indicates that the epilepsy group performed more poorly than nonclinical controls and positive effect indicates the reverse. Effect size by task type was also compared using the Q-Test, indicating the degree of variability within each effect in question where the larger values indicate larger between-groups variability. In other words, the Q value larger than the number of pooled effects (k-1 degree of freedom) indicates that differences between groups were significant within each task domain.
The analyses examined the overall and specific effects for each social cognitive construct: (1) overall difference in social cognition performance (emotion recognition, theory of mind and empathy) between TLE and FLE compared to nonclinical controls, and between TLE subgroups (side of seizure focus - left versus right); (2) performance for specific emotions (anger, sadness, happiness, disgust, and fear) between TLE and FLE compared to nonclinical controls, and between TLE subgroups (side of seizure focus - left versus right); (3) performance for sub-components of theory of mind (e.g., Faux Pas and False Belief tasks) between TLE and FLE compared to nonclinical controls, and between TLE subgroups (side of seizure focus - left versus right). Due to known variation, random-effect models were used for all analyses to ensure that we captured the heterogeneous effects present in clinical populations. Where permitted, the effects of laterality of the seizure focus on task performance (e.g., visual vs. auditory emotion recognition) were assessed in each social cognitive domain.
Initial analyses included calculation of the weighted mean effect size for each domain of social cognition (e.g., a single effect size for each dependent measure for each independent study). Subsequent analyses focused on left- and right-side seizure focus vs. nonclinical control participants in each domain of social cognitive function. Finally, exploratory analyses were conducted on specific theory of mind tasks for TLE vs. nonclinical controls and specific emotions that are reported at the end.
RESULTS OF SYSTEMATIC REVIEW & META-ANALYSES
Emotion Recognition
Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE)
Facial emotion recognition Twenty-three studies examined facial emotion recognition in TLE patients, including the use of static and dynamic stimuli as well as stimuli displaying facial expressions at varying intensities (Table 1). Among these studies, 17 of 18 reported impaired emotion recognition using a composite score. Of the individual emotions, deficits were most commonly reported in fear recognition (11 of 18 studies), followed by disgust (10 of 17 studies) and then sadness (6 of 19 studies) in all types of stimuli (e.g., dynamic or static). Two of 15 studies found deficits in recognizing angry facial expressions, whereas only one of 18 studies found deficits in recognizing happy facial expressions. Lateralization: Eleven of the above studies reported effects of laterality of the epileptic focus, compared to nonclinical controls [seven included right TLE (RTLE) and six left TLE (LTLE)]. For overall emotional recognition ability, four of five studies found impairments in RTLE patients and three of five studies found impairments in LTLE patients compared to nonclinical controls. In terms of recognizing specific emotions, three of seven studies examining fear demonstrated deficits in RTLE and one of seven studies demonstrated impairments in LTLE. Two of six studies showed that RTLE patients were impaired in their recognition of sadness (Meletti et al., Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Cantalupo, Rubboli, Tassinari and Nichelli2009; Meletti et al., Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Rubboli, Cantalupo, Nichelli and Tassinari2003b) whereas LTLE patients exhibited intact recognition of sadness compared to nonclinical controls (Seven of seven studies). One of six studies measuring happiness found that RTLE patients were impaired in their recognition of happiness (Cohn et al., Reference Cohn, St-Laurent, Barnett and McAndrews2015); however, no deficits emerged for LTLE patients among all seven studies (See Table 6). Seven studies compared the performance between right-side epilepsy patients and controls in recognizing disgust with three of these studies reporting deficits in RTLE patients (Golouboff et al., Reference Golouboff, Fiori, Delalande, Fohlen, Dellatolas and Jambaque2008; Meletti et al., Reference Meletti, Cantalupo, Santoro, Benuzzi, Marliani, Tassinari and Rubboli2014; Meletti et al., Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Rubboli, Cantalupo, Nichelli and Tassinari2003b) whereas none of six reported deficits in LTLE patients. None of the five studies examining anger found impairments in either RTLE or LTLE patients.
Note: TLE = temporal lobe epilepsy; FLE = frontal lobe epilepsy.
Thirteen studies made direct comparisons between LTLE and RTLE in facial emotion recognition. A minority of the studies that reported the composite score found lateralized differences in total emotion recognition (three of 11), with RTLE patients performing worse than LTLE patients (Meletti et al., Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Rubboli, Cantalupo, Nichelli and Tassinari2003b; Meletti et al. Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Cantalupo, Rubboli, Tassinari and Nichelli2009; Sedda et al., Reference Sedda, Rivolta, Scarpa, Burt, Frigerio, Zanardi and Bottini2013). Meletti et al. (Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Cantalupo, Rubboli, Tassinari and Nichelli2009) reported that bilateral TLE (BTLE) patients performed worse than RTLE patients, and RTLE performed worse than LTLE patients. Additionally, Sedda et al. (Reference Sedda, Rivolta, Scarpa, Burt, Frigerio, Zanardi and Bottini2013) found that RTLE patients were more impaired at recognizing emotions at 75% intensity than LTLE patients; however, these differences were nonsignificant when expressions were presented at 100%.
Eight studies examined fear recognition in patients with RTLE and LTLE. Five of these studies found differences according to seizure lateralization. Specifically, four studies found that RTLE patients were more impaired in fear recognition than LTLE patients. Meletti et al. (Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Cantalupo, Rubboli, Tassinari and Nichelli2009) showed that fear recognition was comparable in BTLE and RTLE patients, with both groups performing worse than LTLE patients. In contrast, one study reported that LTLE patients performed worse than RTLE patients in fear recognition (Golouboff et al., Reference Golouboff, Fiori, Delalande, Fohlen, Dellatolas and Jambaque2008). Three studies found a nonsignificant difference (Reynders et al., Reference Reynders, Broks, Dickson, Lee and Turpin2005; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019a; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Lawrence, Bramham, Brierley, Radbourne and David2007).
Finally, six studies inspected the differences in recognizing other emotions (i.e., happiness, anger, sadness and disgust), in addition to fear, according to seizure lateralization. One study reported significant differences between RTLE and LTLE patients in the recognition of anger and sadness (Meletti et al., Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Cantalupo, Rubboli, Tassinari and Nichelli2009). Meletti et al. (Reference Meletti, Benuzzi, Cantalupo, Rubboli, Tassinari and Nichelli2009) found that BTLE and RTLE patients performed equally in recognizing anger and sadness but were worse than LTLE patients. No differences were found according to seizure laterality for happiness or disgust.
Results from the meta-analysis on all included emotion recognition studies in TLE produced a Hedge’s g of 0.69 and a 95% confidence interval (CI) for the effect of between 0.35 and 1.04; Z score = 3.93, p < .001; Table 5). As this range does not include zero, the effects are considered significantly different from zero. Therefore, we can reject the null hypothesis and conclude that TLE patients significantly and substantially performed worse than nonclinical groups on emotion recognition tasks.
Additionally, TLE patients with left versus right-sided seizure lateralization were compared to a nonclinical population. Our meta-analysis suggested that the small mean effect for LTLE (Hedge’s g = .35; 95% CI = –.01–.68; Z score = 2.06, p = .039) and RTLE (Hedge’s g = .42; 95% CI = –.04–.80; Z score = 2.21, p = .027; Table 5) compared to the nonclinical group. Given that the CIs cross zero, the effects are not significant, suggesting that there is no statistical difference in emotion recognition comparing the LTLE and RTLE to nonclinical group.
Frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE)
Overall, three studies have looked at facial emotion recognition in FLE (Farrant et al., Reference Farrant, Morris, Russell, Elwes, Akanuma, Alarcon and Koutroumanidis2005; Golouboff et al., Reference Golouboff, Fiori, Delalande, Fohlen, Dellatolas and Jambaque2008; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016). Two studies evaluated overall emotional recognition ability, one study revealing impairments in FLE (Farrant et al., Reference Farrant, Morris, Russell, Elwes, Akanuma, Alarcon and Koutroumanidis2005) while the other did not (Golouboff et al., Reference Golouboff, Fiori, Delalande, Fohlen, Dellatolas and Jambaque2008; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016). In terms of specific emotions, a greatest number of impairments was found in the recognition of anger, fear and sadness (two of three: Farrant et al., Reference Farrant, Morris, Russell, Elwes, Akanuma, Alarcon and Koutroumanidis2005; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016); However, Golouboff et al. (Reference Golouboff, Fiori, Delalande, Fohlen, Dellatolas and Jambaque2008) did not find impairments in these emotions. Only one of three studies found deficits in the recognition of happiness (Farrant et al., Reference Farrant, Morris, Russell, Elwes, Akanuma, Alarcon and Koutroumanidis2005) and disgust (Hu et al., Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016). Differences between sides were not reported in any of the studies.
At meta-analytic review, a large effect size was achieved for emotion recognition in FLE (Hedge’s g = 1.18; 95% CI = .56–1.80). The Z-value for testing the null hypotheses was 3.71 for emotion recognition (p-value < .001; Table 5), suggesting that performance was poorer in FLE than in nonclinical controls, however, the results need to be considered cautiously due to small number of studies and participants in this category.
Theory of Mind
Temporal lobe epilepsy
Seventeen studies included at least one measure of Theory of Mind with the Faux Pas tTest being the most used task (Table 3). None of the 17 studies that examined Theory of Mind in focal epilepsy provided composite scores; thus, the results of each task will be examined separately. For the Faux Pas task, 12 of 13 studies reported lower performance in TLE patients than in nonclinical controls. Regarding seizure lateralization, four of four studies found that both RTLE and LTLE patients were impaired compared with nonclinical controls, and one study also showed similar performance in BTLE compared to RTLE and LTLE patients (Li et al., Reference Li, Chiu, Yeh, Liou, Cheng and Hua2013). Furthermore, one of four studies found that RTLE patients were more impaired than LTLE patients (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Frings, Huppertz, Grunwald, Kurthen, Kramer and Jokeit2012a), and the remaining studies reported a nonsignificant difference according to seizure lateralization (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b; Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Duhamel, Lopes, Semah, Tyvaert and Szurhaj2015b; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Lawrence, Bramham, Brierley, Radbourne and David2007; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019a; see supplementary table 2 for breakdown of studies).
For the False Beliefs task, two of three studies found that patients with TLE scored lower than nonclinical controls (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Shih, Yu, Yen, Lin, Kwan and Hua2015; Li et al., Reference Li, Chiu, Yeh, Liou, Cheng and Hua2013). One study comparing epileptic seizure lateralization to nonclinical controls reported that RTLE and BTLE patients were impaired but LTLE was not impaired (Li et al., Reference Li, Chiu, Yeh, Liou, Cheng and Hua2013). For the Reading the Mind in the Eye test, one of two studies reported that mesial TLE patients were more impaired than nonclinical controls (Okruszek et al., Reference Okruszek, Bala, Wordecha, Jarkiewicz, Wysokinski, Szczepocka and Marchel2017), and no study identified significant differences according to seizure lateralization (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b).
Of the studies that have used the Strange Stories Test, four studies found that TLE patients were more impaired than controls (Li et al., Reference Li, Chiu, Yeh, Liou, Cheng and Hua2013, Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Lawrence, Radbourne, Bramham, Polkey and David2004, Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019a; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Shih, Yu, Yen, Lin, Kwan and Hua2015) while one study did not find any difference (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Lawrence, Bramham, Brierley, Radbourne and David2007). In the only study comparing TLE subgroups according to seizure lateralization with nonclinical controls, deficits were reported among RTLE patients, but not LTLE or BTLE patients (Li et al., Reference Li, Chiu, Yeh, Liou, Cheng and Hua2013). However, no significant differences emerged between patients with epilepsy grouped by seizure lateralization and controls on this task in two studies (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Lawrence, Bramham, Brierley, Radbourne and David2007; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019a).
All three studies that used the moving triangles task reported that mesial TLE patients performed worse than nonclinical controls (Bala et al., Reference Bala, Okruszek, Piejka, Glebicka, Szewczyk, Bosak and Marchel2018; Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b; Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Koelkebeck, Brion, Tyvaert, Plomhause and Szurhaj2016). Only one study reported that both right and left mesial TLE patients performed worse than nonclinical controls (Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Koelkebeck, Brion, Tyvaert, Plomhause and Szurhaj2016); however, when comparing patients according to seizure lateralization, left and right TLE patients performed comparably (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b; Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Koelkebeck, Brion, Tyvaert, Plomhause and Szurhaj2016). Additionally, Hennion et al. (Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Koelkebeck, Brion, Tyvaert, Plomhause and Szurhaj2016) reported that brain activation in a ToM-interaction condition relative to non-theory of mind interaction condition showed greater activation of inferior and middle occipital gyrus in right mesial TLE than in nonclinical controls. Nonclinical control participants, however, activated inferior and middle occipital gyrus, left temporoparietal junction, fusiform gyri, and superior temporal sulcus more than patients.
Results from meta-analysis suggested that the effect for theory of mind was large (Hedge’s g = .95; 95% CI = .61–1.29; Z score = 5.55, p < .001; Table 5). As the confidence interval does not include zero, the effects were significantly different from zero; hence, we can reject null hypotheses and conclude that on average, TLE patients performed significantly, and substantially, worse than nonclinical groups on theory of mind. Additionally, our results indicated large effect sizes for both right and left lateralized TLE. However, the magnitude of this impairment was larger for patients with RTLE (Hedge’s g = 1.10; 95% CI = .55–1.66; Z score = 3.91, p < .001) compared to LTLE (Hedge’s g = .90; 95% CI = .37–1.42; Z score = 3.36, p = .001). The difference between the two means yields a Q-value of 64.86 for RTLE (p < . 001) and 63.10 for LTLE (p < . 001) (Table 5). We reject the null hypotheses that the means of the two groups are identical and conclude that patients had more severe theory of mind difficulties than nonclinical controls and also that RTLE patients had more deficits than LTLE patients. Table 6 summarizes the results for comparison between LTLE and RTLE in emotion recognition and theory of mind constructs.
Frontal lobe epilepsy
Overall, studies that have evaluated theory of mind in FLE patients reported lower performance on the Faux Pas Test (two of three studies) the RMET (two of two studies), and cartoon vignettes test (only one study) compared to nonclinical controls (Table 3). One study that used the Yoni task found that FLE patients performed worse than controls on the sub-measures of second-order ToM, “fortune of others”, envy, gloating, and identification (Hu et al., Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016). However, FLE patients were not impaired relative to controls on the Strange Stories task (Farrant et al., Reference Farrant, Morris, Russell, Elwes, Akanuma, Alarcon and Koutroumanidis2005). At meta-analytic review, a large effect size was achieved for theory of mind (Hedge’s g = .90; 95% CI = .48–1.31). The Z-value for testing the null hypotheses was 4.27 (p-value < .001), suggesting that patients with FLE performed more poorly than nonclinical controls in ToM. Due to small number of studies and participants, these results need to be interpreted cautiously.
Empathy
Temporal lobe epilepsy
Four studies measured empathy in TLE patients (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b; Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Duhamel, Lopes, Semah, Tyvaert and Szurhaj2015b; Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Rankin, Huppertz, Kurthen and Jokeit2015a; Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Grunwald, Huppertz, Kurthen, Rankin and Jokeit2015b), and each study reported a different combination of the total score and subscale scores (Table 4). Two studies looking at the total empathy score found that TLE patients were unimpaired compared to nonclinical controls (Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Duhamel, Lopes, Semah, Tyvaert and Szurhaj2015b; Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b). Regarding the cognitive component of empathy, one study revealed deficits in TLE patients (Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Duhamel, Lopes, Semah, Tyvaert and Szurhaj2015b) while another did not (Broicher, Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b). For the perspective-taking subscale, deficits were reported in RTLE patients compared with nonclinical controls (Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Grunwald, Huppertz, Kurthen, Rankin and Jokeit2015b). However, no deficits were found in the fantasy subscale (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b). No differences between TLE groups according to seizure lateralization were found on the total score, cognitive component score, or fantasy subscore (Broicher et al. Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b), or perspective-taking subscore (Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Grunwald, Huppertz, Kurthen, Rankin and Jokeit2015b).
In studies examining various aspects of the affective component, no impairments were found in the total affective score (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b; Hennion et al., Reference Hennion, Delbeuck, Duhamel, Lopes, Semah, Tyvaert and Szurhaj2015b). Two of three studies reporting empathic concern found deficits in RTLE but not LTLE patients (Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Rankin, Huppertz, Kurthen and Jokeit2015a; Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Grunwald, Huppertz, Kurthen, Rankin and Jokeit2015b). For the personal distress scale, one of two studies showed that RTLE and LTLE patients had higher scores than nonclinical controls (Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Rankin, Huppertz, Kurthen and Jokeit2015a). Three studies compared RTLE and LTLE patients. RTLE patients were impaired in empathic concern relative to LTLE patients in two of the three studies (Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Rankin, Huppertz, Kurthen and Jokeit2015a; Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Grunwald, Huppertz, Kurthen, Rankin and Jokeit2015b), whereas no differences between sides were reported in personal distress (two of two studies) (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b; Toller et al., Reference Toller, Adhimoolam, Rankin, Huppertz, Kurthen and Jokeit2015a) or in the overall affective component (one of one study) (Broicher et al., Reference Broicher, Kuchukhidze, Grunwald, Kramer, Kurthen and Jokeit2012b). Results from meta-analysis revealed that for the empathy domain the effect size was moderate with a Hedge’s g of .57 (95% CI = .32-.82; Z score = 4.43, p < .001; Table 5). As the confidence interval does not include zero, the effects were significantly different from zero, so, we can reject the null hypotheses and conclude that on average, TLE patients performed significantly, and substantially, worse than nonclinical groups on empathy.
Frontal lobe epilepsy
Two studies measured empathy in FLE patients (Gul & Ahmad, Reference Gul and Ahmad2017; Hu et al., Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016). One study reported the total score from the IRI, which showed that FLE patients were impaired in empathy relative to nonclinical controls (Hu et al., Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016). Both studies showed impairments in cognitive empathy but no impairments in affective empathy. Of the two studies, only Hu et al. (Reference Hu, Jiang, Hu, Ma and Wang2016) reported results of the four cognitive subscales (i.e., perspective taking, fantasy, empathic concern, and personal distress). Deficits were found on the perspective taking subscale but not the fantasy subscale, and no impairments were found on either empathic concern or personal distress. Differences between FLE patients grouped according to seizure lateralization were not reported in any study (Table 4).
Due to the low number of studies in the analysis, statistical measures need to be interpreted cautiously. Overall, we found high Hedge’s g of .96 (95% CI = .27–1.65; Z score = 2.75, p < .001; Table 5) suggesting that FLE patients had lower empathic response than nonclinical population.
Social Behavior
Temporal lobe epilepsy
Studies included in this systematic review employed only three scales: Social Responsiveness Scale, Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), and Social Adjustment Scale. Four studies compared TLE patients with nonclinical controls on different aspects of social behavior (Gois et al., Reference Gois, Valente, Vicentiis, Moschetta, Kuczynski, Fiore and Fuentes2011; Gascoigne et al., Reference Gascoigne, Smith, Barton, Webster, Gill and Lah2019; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019a; Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019b). One study investigated social adjustment in adults with TLE compared to nonclinical controls (Gois et al., Reference Gois, Valente, Vicentiis, Moschetta, Kuczynski, Fiore and Fuentes2011) and found impairments in overall social adjustment as well as subdomains pertaining to work and leisure. However, the remaining subdomains (i.e., family relationship, marital relationship, relationship with children, domestic life, and financial situation) were unaffected in patients with TLE (Table 4).
The remaining three studies investigated social behavior in children and adolescents. Two studies assessed participants on the Social Competence Scale from the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL), in which one reported impairment (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019b) whereas the other did not (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019a). Another study assessed and reported significantly elevated scores on the Social Problems and Aggressive behaviors subscales but not on the Rule-Breaking subscale from the CBCL (Gascoigne et al., Reference Gascoigne, Smith, Barton, Webster, Gill and Lah2019). Lastly, one study administered the Social Responsiveness Scale and found deficits on the social communication subscale but not the prosocial behavior subscale (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa, Gonzalez, Gill, Webster, Lawson and Lah2019a).
Results from meta-analysis revealed that for social behavior the effect in TLE was low with a Hedge’s g of .13 (95% CI = −1.26-.98; Z score = −.24, p = .80; Table 5). As this range does include zero, the effects were not significantly different from zero, so we cannot reject null hypotheses. Thus, on average, patients were not significantly different from nonclinical control groups in social behavior. However, due to the small number of studies included, the results need to be interpreted cautiously for this construct. The effect was also not significant for patients with frontal epilepsy (Hedge’s g of .47 (95% CI = –.44–1.40; Z score = 1.01, p = .31).
Specific Emotions: Patients vs. Nonclinical Control
We included studies that specifically measured and reported performance for specific emotions, anger, sadness, fear, surprise, happiness, disgust. All patients performed poorly relative to nonclinical group evident by the Hedge’s g and Z values (Table 5). Based on the effect size values, disgust (Hedge’s g = .42; 95% CI = .21–.59; Z score = 3.78, p < .001), fear (Hedge’s g = .40; 95% CI = .17–.59; Z score = 3.32, p = .001), anger (Hedge’s g = .36; 95% CI = .04–.61; Z score = 2.23, p = .025), and sadness (Hedge’s g = .38; 95% CI = .16–.56; Z score = 3.28, p = .001) showed large effect sizes, meaning that patients significantly and substantially recognized these emotions more poorly than the nonclinical group.
Specific Theory of Mind Task: Patients vs. Nonclinical Control
For the specific theory of mind tasks, we included Faux Pas, False Belief, and Reading the Mind in the Eye tests as common tasks used in the literature (Table 5). Overall, patients showed poorer performance in all these measures relative to the nonclinical group, with large effect sizes. However, in the Faux Pas Test, more studies (14 studies) were included (Hedge’s g = 1.15; 95% CI = .57–1.72; Z score = 3.94, p < .001) and False Belief (Hedge’s g = 1.84; 95% CI = .47–3.21; Z score = 2.64, p < .001) and RMET (Hedge’s g = .86; 95% CI = .54–1.18; Z score = 5.33, p < .001) only included two and six studies, respectively.
Quality Ratings
The results of quality rating using the 17-item Downs and Black checklist revealed that each study included in the meta-analytic review was deemed to be of low methodological bias. Score for cross sectional studies ranged from 11–16 out of 16 and longitudinal studies’ scores ranged from 12–16 out of 18 points. Overall, the studies reported in the emotion recognition domain were assessed as exhibiting the lowest quality when compared to the other social cognitive domains, albeit still within the range considered to be ‘low risk’. Most studies failed to report the statistical metrics required such as reporting actual probability (item 8) or estimation of random variability in the data (item 6). Only few studies included confounding variables as covariates in the analyses, but all studies scored perfectly for the methodological biases and all studies had sufficient power. The ratings of each study are included in the supplementary tables 4–6.
CONCLUSION
We examined the four major domains of social cognition: emotion recognition, theory of mind, empathy, and social behavior, among patients with temporal and frontal lobe epilepsy. Both narrative synthesis and meta-analyses were conducted to compare patients with temporal lobe and frontal lobe epilepsy (TLE and FLE, respectively) with nonclinical controls as well as comparing between left and right-side of lesion among TLE patients. Next, we discuss the theoretical and clinical implications of these findings alongside suggestions to support routine clinical assessment of social cognition for people with epilepsy.
MAGNITUDE OF SOCIAL COGNITIVE DEFICITS FOLLOWING EPILEPSY
This is the first review, to our knowledge, to comprehensively examine functioning across the four major domains of social cognition in people with TLE and FLE. Compared to nonclinical controls, these focal epilepsy groups exhibited moderate to large deficits across each of the four social cognitive domains. These deficits were most notable within the domains of emotion recognition and theory of mind, which are in line with previous meta-analyses in this area suggesting deficits in the emotion recognition and theory of mind in focal epilepsy patients (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Catroppa and Lah2016; Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Stewart, Palermo and Lah2017). Our findings are also in accordance with the broader literature on social cognitive deficits among many clinical populations (Cotter et al., Reference Cotter, Granger, Backx, Hobbs, Looi and Barnett2018). Additionally, our results converge with previous meta-analyses suggesting the magnitude of social cognitive impairment varies according to focal epilepsy syndromes. Specifically, FLE patients exhibit significantly greater impairment in emotion recognition compared to TLE patients while the effects were comparable for theory of mind.
More specifically, the summary effect size for emotion perception was larger in patients with FLE compared to nonclinical controls than those with TLE compared to nonclinical controls. This difference is likely to reflect differential disruption to the underlying neural substrates subserving emotion perception according to the location of the epileptogenic focus. Converging evidence suggests importance of the prefrontal cortex and limbic areas during processing emotional facial expression (Todorov et al., Reference Todorov, Mende-Siedlecki and Dotsch2013; Lopatina et al., Reference Lopatina, Komleva, Gorina, Higashida and Salmina2018). While the importance of limbic areas has been established in the literature for processing social cues (Pessoa Reference Pessoa2010; Adolphs, Reference Adolphs2008; Adolphs Reference Adolphs2010), damage to prefrontal cortex, such as the case of Phineas Gage, has adverse impact on the processing of social and emotional cues (Perry et al., Reference Perry, Saunders, Stiso, Dewar, Lubell, Meling and Knight2017; Martins et al., Reference Martins, Faísca, Esteves, Simão, Justo, Muresan and Reis2012). Our results are in line with this notion that prefrontal areas as critical as limbic areas in processing and regulation of social and emotional cues (Buhle et al., Reference Buhle, Silvers, Wager, Lopez, Onyemekwu, Kober and Ochsner2014) and perhaps insults to this area could result in more adverse impact on the performance among patients with FLE relative to TLE. Importantly however, our methodology did not permit direct comparison of FLE and TLE patients across the four domains of social cognition. Hence, further direct comparison is warranted. Additionally, the precision of the findings extracted from studies with FLE patients is uncertain given the small number of eligible studies to date. Further studies are needed to investigate social cognitive functions in FLE and confirm the current findings.
It is worth mentioning that the age of disease onset was early among patients with FLE, relative to the TLE group. The ability to understand emotions may facilitate the acquisition of emotional skills as the child continues to develop (Conte et al., Reference Conte, Ornaghi, Grazzani, Pepe and Cavioni2019). Thus, insults to these areas could impact the ability to perceive social and emotional cues in later life more severely (Korkmaz, Reference Korkmaz2011). It remains an open question for further investigation, however, to clarify the phenomenology and social cognitive performance, and specifically emotion recognition, among patients with epilepsy at different stages of disease’ development.
LATERALITY OF TEMPORAL LOBE EPILEPSY
Our results indicate that patients with RTLE have greater difficulties in the theory of mind domain compared to those with LTLE. Previous studies on the laterality of social cognitive functions indicate that the right hemisphere may play a more important role in theory of mind than the left hemisphere (Giovagnoli et al., Reference Giovagnoli, Franceschetti, Reati, Parente, Maccagnano, Villani and Spreafico2011; Bora & Meletti, Reference Bora and Meletti2016). A recent meta-analysis on stroke patients also indicated that the magnitude of social cognitive impairment was greater following right than left hemisphere insults (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Schweitzer, Molenberghs and Henry2019). These hemispheric differences are in keeping with studies showing that the right temporo-parietal junction is particularly important for theory of mind processing (Saxe & Wexler, Reference Saxe and Wexler2005).
RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN TASK FEATURES AND TASK PERFORMANCE
A recent meta-analysis suggests that the temporo-parietal junction, medial prefrontal cortex, posterior superior temporal sulcus, and inferior parietal lobe are critical in theory of mind (Schurz et al., Reference Schurz, Radua, Aichhorn, Richlan and Perner2014). Given that these areas are disrupted by focal epilepsies such as TLE and FLE, it is not surprising that patients with epilepsy might show difficulties in the performance of theory of mind tasks. Interestingly, our results suggest distinctions in the types of tasks within the construct of theory of mind that are affected by epilepsy. While studies found large effects for Faux Pas, False Belief and Reading the Mind in the Eye tests, the Faux Pas Test is more affected in these patients than the other two tasks. It is possible that this difference simply reflects difference in the demands of each task on cognitive and executive functions which is in line with Stewart et al.’s findings (Reference Stewart, Catroppa and Lah2016). For example, the Faux Pas Test relies more heavily on language understanding and skills than the Reading the Mind in the Eye test. While there is an association between language and theory of mind skills (Milligan et al., Reference Milligan, Astington and Dack2007), this explanation does not seem to be supported by the difference in performance between LTLE and RTLE patents. RTLE patients performed worse than LTLE patients in our analyses, however, none of the studies reported the relationships between executive functioning, and theory of mind skills. A potential avenue for future research is to consider how visual and verbal processing could account for social cognitive performance in this population. See also supplementary table 8 for summary of previous studies and inclusion of executive functioning measures. Additionally, we highlightthe lack of systematic research establishing the psychometric properties of social cognitive measures for use with people with epilepsy. Future studies confirming the psychometric rigor of these measures for this population will enhance the confidence in interpreting the clinical and theoretical significance of results.
CLINICAL RECOMMENDATION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS
Accumulating evidence emphasizes that, in addition to the influence of disease characteristics and cognitive or psychiatric comorbidities, changes in social cognition impacts psychosocial outcomes in people with epilepsy (Yogarajah & Mula, 2019). Clinically, deficits in social cognition remain under-recognized, likely due to oversight of this domain during clinical evaluation (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Baxendale, Barr, Hamed, Langfitt, Samson and Smith2015). Deficient social cognition has wide-ranging functional implications across multiple psychosocial domains (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Shih, Yu, Yen, Lin, Kwan and Hua2015). Therefore, disclosure of psychosocial challenges ought to trigger consideration of social cognitive impairment as a possible etiological factor and prompt more targeted clinical and psychometric enquiry. While difficulties in other cognitive (e.g., language or visual processing) or psychiatric domains (e.g., depression) may be the root of dysfunctional social behavior, the modularity of social cognition means that impairment may also occur in isolation (McDonald & Cassel, Reference Mcdonald, Cassel, Wilson, Winegardner, van Heugten and Ownsworth2017). Further, the integrity of social cognition cannot be determined from performances across other higher cognitive domains alone, necessitating the inclusion of targeted measures of social cognition within comprehensive psychometric batteries.
In terms of clinical practice recommendations, we encourage clinicians to remain vigilant to indicators of social cognitive dysfunction in patients with TLE and FLE. To guide clinic-based screening of problematic social behavior, we provide some example probes that clinicians can use to guide their clinical interview (see Table 7). This list is not exhaustive but provides a starting point for clinicians to explore suspected concerns about an individual’s social behavior. Collateral information is vital because deficits in social cognition may be subtle or self-appraisal limited, for example by insight/awareness, emotional state, feelings of stigma, and capacity to understand and articulate responses to questions about abstract social cognitive constructs.
* Questions can be phrased in the second person when interviewing the patient (e.g., “what does your main support network look like?”) or in the third person when interviewing parent/family/friends (e.g., “describe (patients name)’s support network”).
♯ Prompt for specific examples if themes are endorsed (e.g., “can you give me some examples of when this has happened? What happened?”).
If clinic-based screening further substantiates concerns about dysfunctional social behavior multidisciplinary psychiatric, psychological, and neuropsychological assessment should be requested to comprehensively investigate the etiological factors at play. Multidisciplinary evaluation is recommended to optimize accurate differential diagnosis given the overlap of personality vulnerabilities, psychopathology and cognitive impairment as drivers of impaired social behavior. While input from multiple specialties is desirable, we recognize that this may not be feasible in some services due to staffing and/or time constraints. Therefore, in practice, referrals to available specialists may be initiated sequentially according to the balance of evidence obtained during initial screening (e.g., evidence of co-morbid psychopathology may indicate initial referral to psychiatry whereas evidence of co-occuring cognitive difficulties may indicate initial referral to clinical psychology or neuropsychology for psychometric testing). To assist clinical psychologists and neuropsychologists in test selection, we provide tentative suggestions, pending confirmation via systematic review, of measures to evaluate each of the four social cognitive domains in people with epilepsy (see supplementary table 7). While we based our suggestions on the most frequently used measures within this review, a systematic review of the psychometric properties of social cognitive measures pertintent to people with epilepsy is a priority for future research.
Multiple data sources are required to appropriately determine whether social difficulties are primary manifestations of impaired social cognition or secondary to one or more psychosocial comorbidities. Interpretation and formulation of findings from multiple psychometric tests is a core capability of clinical psychologists and neuropsychologists; disciplines which are well placed to contribute to the differential diagnosis of the basis of impaired social behavior.
CONCLUSION
This study is the first to investigate all four domains of social cognition in patients with epilepsy using both systematic and statistical meta-analysis reports. We investigated performance of people with TLE and FLE on emotion recognition, theory of mind, empathy, and social behavior. Our results from meta-analysis revealed that patients with FLE demonstrated poorer performance compared to those with TLE, mainly in emotion recognition tasks. While our results highlight the variability of social cognitive functions in the population of epilepsy patients, it also demonstrates the importance of the frontal lobe in social cognitive performance. Our results provide future directions for clinical and experimental research with the emphasis on the inclusion of social cognitive tasks in the clinical setting. We also highlight a gap in the literature in studies focussing on the domains of empathy and social behavior in patients with focal epilepsy and psychometric properties of these measures among people with epilepsy. Furthermore, additional experimental research is needed to explore the overlapping cognitive and mental disorders related to social cognition in patients with focal epilepsy. It is of utmost importance for clinicians to consider social cognitive deficits as a potential contributory factor in their assessment of impaired social behavior and which could help to move toward a more holistic treatment plans for this population.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617722000066
Acknowledgments
Authors would like to thank Priscilla Tjokrowijoto for help with literature search.
Financial Support
MZ was funded by the UQ Development Fellowship (Research and Teaching) at the time of conducting this review. Data were collected using the Royal Brisbane and Women’s Hospital Foundation grant money.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors have nothing to disclose.
Ethical Standards
This study was approved by regional Ethics committee.