Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T08:05:50.104Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Confabulation Resilience of the Developing Brain: A Brief Review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 May 2019

Julie Nyvang Christensen*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen University Hospital, Glostrup, Denmark
Thomas Alrik Sørensen
Affiliation:
Aalborg University & Sino-Danish Center for Education and Research, Aalborg, Denmark
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Julie Nyvang Christensen, Department of Neurology, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen University Hospital, Valdemar Hansens Vej 1-23 DK-2600 Glostrup, Denmark. E-mail [email protected]

Abstract

Objectives: To investigate a possible confabulation resilience of the developing brain. Methods: We performed a literature search on confabulation in PubMed and identified all empirical studies of children and adolescents under the age of 18. Results: The analysis identified only three case studies of confabulation in children under the age of 18 of 286 empirical studies of confabulation. This reveals a striking discrepancy in the number of reported cases caused by brain injury between children and adults. We hypothesize that there may be a resilience toward confabulation in the developing brain and present three tentative explanations regarding the possible underlying mechanisms. Conclusions: Additional awareness on the scarcity of reported cases of confabulation in children could lead to important insights on the nature of confabulation and greater understanding of the resilience and plasticity of the developing brain. (JINS, 2019, 25, 426–431)

Type
Short Review
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society, 2019. 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). DSM-IV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Borzutzky, S., Fujiwara, E., Brand, M., & Markowitsch, H. (2008). Confabulations in Alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Neuropsychologia, 46, 31333143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowman, K. M., & Blau, A. (1940). Psychotic states following head and brain injury in adults and children. In S. Brock (Ed.). Injuries of the skull. brain and spinal cord: Neuro-psychiatric surgical and medico-legal aspects (pp. 309360). London: Ballière Tindal, & Cox.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (2012). Forensic developmental psychology in the courtroom. In D. Faust (Ed.). Coping with psychiatric and psychological testimony. (6th ed., pp. 723–736). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, V., Jellinek, S. P., Stansfield, L., Truong, H., Baseluos, C., & Marshall, J. P. (2011). Cardiac arrest with residual blindness after overdose of Tessalon® (Benzonatate Perles). The Journal of Emergency Medicine, 41(2), 166171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Barba, G. (1993a). Different patterns of confabulation. Cortex, 29(4), 567581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Barba, G. (1993b). Confabulation: Knowledge and recollective experience. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10(1), 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Barba, G., Cipolotti, L., & Denes, G. (1990). Autobiographical memory loss and confabulation in Korsakoff’s syndrome: A case report. Cortex, 26, 525534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalla Barba, G., Brazzarola, M., Marangoni, S., Barbera, C., & Zannoni, I. (2017). A longitudinal study of confabulation. Cortex, 87, 4451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalla Barba, G., Guerin, B., Brazzarola, M., Marangoni, S., Barbera, C., & La Corte, V. (2018). The confabulation battery: Instructions and international data from normal participants. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 21, 112.Google Scholar
Ellison, P. A., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2007). Child neuropsychology. Assessment and intervention for neurodevelopmental disorders. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Garré, M. L., & Cama, A. (2007). Craniopharyngeoma: Modern concepts in pathogenesis and treatment. Current Opinions in Pediatrics, 19(4), 471479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilboa, A. (2010). Strategic retrieval, confabulations and delusions: Theory and data. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15(1), 145180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilboa, A., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience and confabulation: A review and a model. In A. D. Baddeley, M. D. Kopelman, & B. Wilson (Eds.). The handbook of memory disorders (2nd ed., pp. 315342). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Kolb, B., & Fantie, B. D. (2009). Development of the child’s brain and behavior. In C. R. Reynolds & E. Fletcher-Janzen (Eds.). Handbook of Clinical Child Neuropsychology (3rd ed., pp. 1946). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
La Corte, V., Serra, M., Attali, E., Boissé, M. F., & Dalla Barba, G. (2010). Confabulation in Alzheimer’s disease and amnesia: A qualitative account and a new taxonomy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(6), 967974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2006). Brain development in children and adolescents: Insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(6), 718729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liverani, M. C., Manuel, A. L., Nahum, L., Guardabassi, V., Tomasetto, C., & Schnider, A. (2017). Children’s sense of reality: The development of orbitofrontal reality filtering. Child Neuropsychology, 23(4), 408421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maddula, M., Lutton, S., & Keegan, B. (2009). Anton’s syndrome due to cerebrovascular disease: A case report. Journal of Medical Case Reports, 3(1), 9028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meguro, Y., Suzuki, K., Tsukjura, T., Fujii, T., Yamadori, A., & Kudoh, M. (1999). Episodic and semantic memory after brain injury in a child. No To Shinkei, 51(11), 985990.Google Scholar
Nahum, L., Bouzerda-Wahlen, A., Guggisberg, A., Ptak, R., & Schnider, A. (2012). Forms of confabulation: Dissociations and associations. Neuropsychologia, 65, 25242534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnider, A. (2003). Spontaneous confabulation and the adaption of thought to ongoing reality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 662671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnider, A. (2008). The confabulation mind – How the brain creates reality. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnider, A., Nahum, L., & Ptak, R. (2017). What does extinction have to do with confabulation? Cortex, 87, 515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnider, A., Ptak, R., von Däniken, C., & Remonda, L. (2000). Recovery from spontaneous confabulations parallels recovery of temporal confusion in memory. Neurology 55(1), 7483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnider, A., von Daniken, C., & Gutbrod, K. (1996). The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. Brain, 119(4), 13651375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas-Antérion, C., Truche, A., Sciessere, K., & Extier, C. (2012). Spontaneous confabulations and behavioral and cognitive dysexecutive syndrome. Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine. 55(1), 4452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed