Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T17:13:53.276Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Action (verb) fluency: Test–retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2005

STEVEN PAUL WOODS
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California
J. COBB SCOTT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego State University and University of California at San Diego, San Diego, California
DANIELLE A. SIRES
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California
IGOR GRANT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California
ROBERT K. HEATON
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California
ALEXANDER I. TRÖSTER
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North Carolina
THE HIV NEUROBEHAVIORAL RESEARCH CENTER (HNRC) GROUP
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California

Abstract

Action (verb) fluency is a newly developed verbal fluency task that requires the examinee to rapidly generate as many verbs (i.e., “things that people do”) as possible within 1 min. Existing literature indicates that action fluency may be more sensitive to frontal–basal ganglia loop pathophysiology than traditional noun fluency tasks (e.g., animal fluency), which is consistent with the hypothesized neural dissociation between noun and verb retrieval. In the current study, a series of analyses were undertaken to examine the psychometric properties of action fluency in a sample of 174 younger healthy participants. The first set of analyses describes the development of demographically adjusted normative data for action fluency. Next, a group of hypothesis-driven correlational analyses reveals significant associations between action fluency and putative tests of executive functions, verbal working memory, verbal fluency, and information processing speed, but not between action fluency and tests of learning or constructional praxis. The final set of analyses demonstrates the test–retest stability of the action fluency test and provides standards for determining statistically reliable changes in performance. In sum, this study enhances the potential clinical applicability of action fluency by providing demographically adjusted normative data and demonstrating evidence for its reliability and construct validity. (JINS, 2005, 11, 408–415.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alexander, M.P. & Stuss, D.T. (2000). Disorders of frontal lobe functioning. Seminars in Neurology, 20, 427437.Google Scholar
Basso, M.R., Bornstein, R.A., & Lang, J.M. (1999). Practice effects on commonly used measures of executive function across twelve months. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13, 283292.Google Scholar
Benedict, R.H.B. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Benton, A.L., Hamsher, K., & Sivan, A.B. (1994). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates.
Brandt, J. & Benedict, R.H.B. (2001). Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised. Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Buckner, R.L., Raichle, M.E., & Petersen, S.E. (1995). Dissociation of human prefrontal cortical areas across different speech production tasks and gender groups. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 21632173.Google Scholar
Butters, N., Wolfe, J., Granholm, E., & Martone, M. (1986). An assessment of verbal recall, recognition, and fluency abilities in patients with Huntington's disease. Cortex, 22, 1132.Google Scholar
Cappa, S.F., Sandrini, M., Rossini, P.M., Sosta, K., & Miniussi, C. (2002). The role of the left frontal lobe in action naming: rTMS evidence. Neurology, 59, 720723.Google Scholar
Chelune, G.J., Naugle, R.I., Lüders, H., Sedlack, J., & Awad, I.A. (1993). Individual change after epilepsy surgery: Practice effects and base-rate information. Neuropsychology, 7, 4152.Google Scholar
Damasio, A.R. & Tranel, D. (1993). Nouns and verbs are retrieved with differently distributed neural systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90, 49574960.Google Scholar
Diehr, M.C., Heaton, R.K., Miller, W., & Grant, I. (1998). The Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT): Norms for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment, 5, 375387.Google Scholar
Fastenau, P.S. & Adams, K.M. (1996). Heaton, Grant, and Matthews' comprehensive norms: An overzealous attempt. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 444448.Google Scholar
Fastenau, P.S. (1998). Validity of regression-based norms: An empirical test of the comprehensive norms with older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 906916.Google Scholar
Gladsjo, J.A., Schuman, C.C., Evans, J.D., Peavy, G.M., Miller, S.W., & Heaton, R.K. (1999). Norms for letter and category fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment, 6, 147178.Google Scholar
Golden, C.J. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test. Chicago: Stoelting.
Heaton, R.K., Miller, S.W., Taylor, M.J., & Grant, I. (2004). Revised Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Kemmerer, D. & Tranel, D. (2000). Verb retrieval in brain-damaged subjects: 1. Analysis of stimulus, lexical, and conceptual factors. Brain and Language, 73, 347392.Google Scholar
Kløve, H. (1963). Clinical neuropsychology. In F.M. Forster (Ed.), The medical clinics of North America. New York: Saunders.
Kongs, S.K., Thompson, L.L., Iverson, G.L., & Heaton, R.K. (2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–64 card computerized version. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Perani, D., Cappa., S.F., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Collina., S., Rosa., M.M., & Fazio, F. (1999). The neural correlates of verb and noun processing: A PET study. Brain, 122, 23372344.Google Scholar
Piatt, A.L., Fields, J.A., Paolo, A.M., & Tröster A.I. (1999a). Action (verb naming) fluency as an executive function measure: Convergent and divergent evidence of validity. Neuropsychologia, 37, 14991503.Google Scholar
Piatt, A.L., Fields, J.A., Paolo, A.M., Koller, W.C., & Tröster, A.I. (1999b). Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 435443.Google Scholar
Piatt, A.L., Fields, J.A., Paolo, A.M., & Tröster A.I. (2004). Action verbal fluency normative data for the elderly. Brain and Language, 89, 580583.Google Scholar
Pihlajamaki, M., Tanila, H., Hanninen, T., Kononen, M., Laakso, M., Partanen, K., Soininen, H., & Aronen, H.J. (2000). Verbal fluency activates the left medial temporal lobe: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Annals of Neurology, 47, 470476.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Psychological Corporation. (1997). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third edition. San Antonio, TX: Author.
Reitan, R.M. & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.
Richter, S., Kaiser, O., Hein-Kropp, C., Dimitrova, A., Gizewski, E., Beck, A., Aurich, V., Ziegler, W., & Timmann, D. (2004). Preserved verb generation in patients with cerebellar atrophy. Neuropsychologia, 42, 12351246.Google Scholar
Royston, P. & Altman, D.G. (1994). Regression using fractional polynomials of continuous covariates: Parsimonious parametric modeling. Applied Statistics, 43, 429467.Google Scholar
Sach, M., Seitz, R.J., & Indefrey, P. (2004). Unified inflectional processing of regular and irregular verbs: A PET study. Neuroreport, 15, 533537.Google Scholar
Suhr, J.A. & Jones, R.D. (1998). Letter and semantic fluency in Alzheimer's, Huntington's, and Parkinson's dementias. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 13, 447454.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (2001). A neural basis for the retrieval of words for actions. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18, 655670.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Kemmerer, D., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A.R. (2003). Neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 409432.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Martin, C., Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., & Hichwa, R. (2005). Effects of noun–verb homonymy on the neural correlates of naming concrete entities and actions. Brain and Language, 92, 288299.Google Scholar
Warburton, E., Wise, R.J., Price, C.J., Weiller, C., Hadar, U., Ramsay, S., & Frackowiak, R.S. (1996). Noun and verb retrieval by normal subjects. Studies with PET. Brain, 119, 159179.Google Scholar
Williamson, D.J.G., Adair, J.C., Raymer, A.M., & Heilman, K.M. (1998). Object and action naming in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex, 34, 601610.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G.S. (1993). Wide Range Achievement Test administration manual (3rd ed.). Wilmington, DE: Wide Range, Inc.
Woods, S.P., Carey, C.L., Tröster, A.I., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group (2005). Action (verb) generation in HIV–1 infection. Neuropsychologia, 43, 11441151.Google Scholar
Woods, S.P., Conover, E., Rippeth, J.D., Carey, C., Gonzalez, R., Marcotte, T.D., Heaton, R.K., Grant, I., & The HNRC Group (2004). Qualitative aspects of verbal fluency performance in HIV-associated dementia: A deficit in rule-guided lexical–semantic search processes? Neuropsychologia, 42, 801809.Google Scholar
Woods, S.P., Scott, J.C., Conover, E., Grant, I., Heaton, R.K., & The HNRC Group. (2005). Test–retest reliability of component process variables within the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised. Assessment, 12, 96100.Google Scholar