Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T13:24:33.792Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of maternal psychosocial perceptions in parent-training programs: a preliminary randomized controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 June 2019

Rebecca M. ALPER*
Affiliation:
Temple University, USA
Richard R. HURTIG
Affiliation:
University of Iowa, USA
Karla K. MCGREGOR
Affiliation:
University of Iowa, USA Boys Town National Research Hospital, USA
*
*Corresponding Author. Weiss Hall Rm. 110, 1701 N. 13th St., Temple University, Philadelphia, PA19122, USA. E-mail: [email protected]; tel: 215-204-8537

Abstract

Parent–child interaction is critical for early language and literacy development. Parent training programs have proliferated to support early interactions. However, many environmental and psychosocial factors can impact the quality of parent–child language and literacy interactions as well as training program outcomes. This preliminary randomized controlled trial examined maternal perceived self-efficacy and locus of control during a language and literacy parent training program. Thirty mother–child dyads (mother age 21–40; children 2;6–4;0) were assigned in parallel to the training or control group. The training was efficacious for mothers and children – training-group dyads made significantly greater gains in maternal strategy use, responsivity, and child print awareness than the control group. Gains were maintained one month post-training. Children whose mothers had more external baseline control perceptions identified significantly fewer print targets at baseline and made greater gains than those with more internal control perceptions. Future directions and implications are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alper, R. M. (2015). Determining factors related to success in parent-implemented emergent language and literacy intervention (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from Iowa Research Online < https://doi.org/10.17077/etd.q8kud88d > .CrossRef+.>Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baxendale, J., & Hesketh, A. (2003). Comparison of the effectiveness of the Hanen Parent Programme and traditional clinic therapy. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38(4), 397415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedel, E. F. (2012). An examination of locus of control, epistemological beliefs and metacognitive awareness in preservice early childhood teachers. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, 12(4), 3051–60.Google Scholar
Benzies, K. M., Trute, B., & Worthington, C. (2013). Maternal self-efficacy and family adjustment in households with children with serious disability. Journal of Family Studies, 19(1), 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bibok, M. B., Carpendale, J. I. M., & Müller, U. (2009). Parental scaffolding and the development of executive function. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2009(123), 1734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 867–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartmill, E. A., Armstrong, B. F., Gleitman, L. R., Goldin-Meadow, S., Medina, T. N., & Trueswell, J. C. (2013). Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(28), 11278–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Coleman, P. K., & Karraker, K. H. (2003). Maternal self-efficacy beliefs, competence in parenting, and toddlers’ behavior and developmental status. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24(2), 126–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Development, 77(1), 4458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeSocio, J., Kitzman, H., & Cole, R. (2003). Testing the relationship between self-agency and enactment of health behaviors. Research in Nursing & Health, 26(1), 20–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D. K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2010). Speaking out for language: why language is central to reading development. Educational Researcher, 39(4), 305–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth Edition. Circle Pines, MN: Pearson.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: a guide for clinicians and researchers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40(5), 532–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, V., & Masur, E. F. (2007). Characteristics of maternal verbal style: responsiveness and directiveness in two natural contexts. Journal of Child Language, 34(3), 519–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagekull, B., Bohlin, G., & Hammarberg, A. (2001). The role of parental perceived control in child development: a longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25(5), 429–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassall, R., Rose, J., & McDonald, J. (2005). Parenting stress in mothers of children with an intellectual disability: the effects of parental cognitions in relation to child characteristics and family support. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49(6), 405–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, C. R., Teti, D. M., & Hussey-Gardner, B. (2004). Self-efficacy and parenting of high-risk infants: the moderating role of parent knowledge of infant development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 423–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., … Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children's language success. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1071–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houck, G. M., Booth, C. L., & Barnard, K. E. (1991). Maternal depression and locus of control orientation as predictors of dyadic play behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 12(4), 347–60.3.0.CO;2-9>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, T. L., & Prinz, R. J. (2005). Potential roles of parental self-efficacy in parent and child adjustment: a review. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(3), 341–63.Google ScholarPubMed
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2000). Enhancing children's print and word awareness through home-based parent intervention. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 9(3), 257–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Justice, L. M., & Ezell, H. K. (2002). Use of storybook reading to increase print awareness in at-risk children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(1), 1729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Justice, L. M., & Sofka, A. E. (2010). Engaging children with print: building early literacy skills through quality read-alouds. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Justice, L. M., Weber, S. E., Ezell, H. K., & Bakeman, R. (2002). A sequential analysis of children's responsiveness to parental print references during shared book-reading interactions. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11(1), 3040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, A. P., Hemmeter, M. L., Ostrosky, M. M., Fischer, R., Yoder, P., & Keefer, M. (1996). The effects of teaching parents to use responsive interaction strategies. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 16(3), 375406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children's development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Developmental Psychology, 37(3), 387403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leckman-Westin, E., Cohen, P. R., & Stueve, A. (2009). Maternal depression and mother–child interaction patterns: association with toddler problems and continuity of effects to late childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(9), 1176–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, T., & Hastings, R. P. (2009). Parental locus of control and psychological well-being in mothers of children with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 34(2), 104–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonigan, C. J., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1998). Relative efficacy of parent and teacher involvement in a shared-reading intervention for preschool children from low-income backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13(2), 263–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovelace, S., & Stewart, S. R. (2007). Increasing print awareness in preschoolers with language impairment using non-evocative print referencing. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38(1), 1630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. F., Flynn, V., & Eichorst, D. L. (2005). Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 32(1), 6391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J., & Chapman, R. (2006). SALT: Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) [Computer software]: Language Analysis Laboratory (Version 9). Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin-Madison.Google Scholar
Piasta, S. B., Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2012). Increasing young children's contact with print during shared reading: longitudinal effects on literacy achievement. Child Development, 83(3), 810–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramírez-Esparza, N., García-Sierra, A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2014). Look who's talking: speech style and social context in language input to infants are linked to concurrent and future speech development. Developmental Science, 17(6), 880–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, E., Sparks, A., & Leyva, D. (2010). A review of parent interventions for preschool children's language and emergent literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10(1), 97117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, M. Y., & Kaiser, A. P. (2011). The effectiveness of parent-implemented language interventions: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20(3), 180–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2018). Beyond the 30-million-word gap: children's conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29(5), 700–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(1), 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1). 185205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L., Leech, K. A., & Cabrera, N. (2017). Going beyond input quantity: wh-questions matter for toddlers’ language and cognitive development. Cognitive Science, 41(S1), 162–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherbaum, C. A., Cohen-Charash, Y., & Kern, M. J. (2006). Measuring general self-efficacy: a comparison of three measures using item response theory. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(6), 1047–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schulz, K. F., Altman, D. G., & Moher, D. (2010). CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ, 340, c332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, S. S., & Wilk, M. B. (1965). An Analysis of Variance test for normality (Complete Samples). Biometrika, 52(3/4), 591611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherer, M., & Adams, C. H. (1983). Construct validation of the Self-Efficacy Scale. Psychological Reports, 53(3), 899902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherer, M., Maddux, J. E., Mercandante, B., Prentice-Dunn, S., Jacobs, B., & Rogers, R. W. (1982). The Self-Efficacy Scale: construction and validation. Psychological Reports, 51(2), 663–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, K. E., Landry, S. H., & Swank, P. R. (2006). The role of early maternal responsiveness in supporting school-aged cognitive development for children who vary in birth status. Pediatrics, 117(5), 1608–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, A., Malmberg, L.-E., Sylva, K., Barnes, J., Leach, P., & the FCCC team (2008). The influence of maternal depression, caregiving, and socioeconomic status in the post-natal year on children's language development. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(5), 603–12.Google ScholarPubMed
Suskind, D. L., Leung, C. Y. Y., Webber, R. J., Hundertmark, A. C., Leffel, K. R., Suskind, E., … Graf, E. (2018). Development of the Survey of Parent/Provider Expectations and Knowledge (SPEAK). First Language, 38(3), 312–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., & Song, L. (2014). Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 121–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1980). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, C. M., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2008). Parenting self-efficacy and problem behavior in children at high risk for early conduct problems: the mediating role of maternal depression. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(4), 594605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wei, M., Russell, D. W., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, social self-efficacy, self-disclosure, loneliness, and subsequent depression for freshman college students: a longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(4), 602–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, D. S., Zosh, J. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2013). Talking it up: play, language development, and the role of adult support. American Journal of Play, 6(1), 3954.Google Scholar
Whitehead, A. L., Julious, S. A., Cooper, C. L., & Campbell, M. J. (2016). Estimating the sample size for a pilot randomised trial to minimise the overall trial sample size for the external pilot and main trial for a continuous outcome variable. Statistical Methods in Medical Research, 25(3), 1057–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test-Second Edition. Circle Pines, MN: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wittenburg, P., Brugman, H., Russell, A., Klassman, A., & Sloetjes, H. (2006). ELAN: a professional framework for multimodality research. In Proceedings of LREC 2006, Fifth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. Online < https://pure.mpg.de/pubman/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_60436 > .+.>Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Self-Efficacy: an essential motive to learn. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 8291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Alper et al. supplementary material

Alper et al. supplementary material 1

Download Alper et al. supplementary material(File)
File 90.1 KB