Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T03:18:06.223Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Getting away from the point: the emergence of ostensive gestures and their functions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2019

Ana MORENO-NÚÑEZ*
Affiliation:
Facultad de Psicología, Departamento Interfacultativo de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Cintia RODRÍGUEZ
Affiliation:
Facultad de Psicología, Departamento Interfacultativo de Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
Edgardo MIRANDA-ZAPATA
Affiliation:
Núcleo de Ciencias Sociales, Laboratorio de Investigación en Ciencias Sociales Aplicadas, Universidad de la Frontera, Temuco, Chile
*
*Corresponding author: Calle Ivan Pavlov, 6, Campus de Cantoblanco – 28049, Madrid, Spain. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Within developmental psychology, pointing gestures have received a great deal of attention, while ostensive gestures have been overlooked in terms of their emergence and intentionality. In a longitudinal and micro-genetic study with six children at 9, 11, and 13 months of age, we codified gesture production of children within second-by-second data frames. We identified 480 instances of gestures and categorised whether they were of ostensive, ostensive–indexical, or indexical nature. We specified the communicative function of each gesture by analysing the object involved and their circumstances of production. Data analysis include frequencies, binomial tests, proportion comparisons, and repeated measures ANOVA. We identified a phatic function in other-directed gestures, as well as exploratory and private functions in self-directed gestures. This has important implications for child development since ostensive gestures are easier to produce and to understand than pointing. The consideration of objects would be essential for defining the communicative function of gestures.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alessandroni, N., Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C., & del Olmo, M. J. (2019). Musical dynamics in early triadic interactions: a case study. Psychological Research. doi:10.1007/s00426-019-01168-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrén, M. (2010). Children's gestures from 18 to 30 months. Lund University.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21(3), 205–26.Google Scholar
Begus, K., & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science, 15(5), 611–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science, 8(6), 492–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1998). Origins of joint visual attention in infancy. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 63, 144–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron-Faulkner, T., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The relationship between infant holdout and gives, and pointing. Infancy, 20(5), 576–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V. (2005). From action to language through gesture: a longitudinal perspective. Gesture, 5(1), 155–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., & Volterra, V. (2008). Gesture and speech: the emergence and development of a strong and changing partnership. Gesture, 8(1), 2244.Google Scholar
Carpendale, J., & Carpendale, A. B. (2010). The development of pointing: from personal directedness to interpersonal direction. Human Development, 53, 110–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christie, P., Newson, E., Prevezer, W., & Chandler, S. (2009). First steps in intervention with your child with autism. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Clark, H. (2003). Pointing and placing. In Kita, S. (Ed.). Pointing: where language, culture and cognition meet (pp. 243–68). Mahwah, NJ/London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cochet, H., & Vauclair, J. (2010). Pointing gestures produced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: different functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 431–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conover, W. J., & Iman, R. L. (1981). Rank transformations as a bridge between parametric and nonparametric statistics. American Statistician, 35(3), 124–9.Google Scholar
Crais, E. R., Douglas, D. D., & Campbell, C. C. (2004). The intersection of the development of gestures and intentionality. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(3), 678–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delgado, B., Gómez, J. C., & Sarriá, E. (2011). Pointing gestures as a cognitive tool in young children: experimental evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 299312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimitrova, N., & Moro, C. (2013). Common ground on object use associates with caregivers’ gesturese. Infant Behavior and Development, 36, 618–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dupertuis, V., & Moro, C. (2016). Self-directed ostensions and mediations of the adult at the age of 8-, 12- and 16 months. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 50(4), 621–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eco, U. (1976). A theory of semiotics. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Levine, S., Zinchenko, E., Yip, T. K.-Y., Hemani, N., & Factor, L. (2012). Doing gesture promotes learning a mental transformation task better than seeing gesture. Developmental Science, 15(6), 876–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gómez, J. C. (2010). The emergence of eye contact as an intersubjective signal in an infant gorilla: implications for models of early social cognition. Acción Psicológica, 7(2), 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gräfenhain, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). One-year-olds’ understanding of nonverbal gestures directed to a third person. Cognitive Development, 24, 2333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosse, G., Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2010). 21-month-olds understand the cooperative logic of requests. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 3377–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M., de Bot, K., & Volterra, V. (2010). Gestures and some key issues in the study of language development. In Gullberg, M. & de Bot, K. (Eds.), Gestures in language development (pp. 333). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Language, 28, 164–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1988). Obras selectas. Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: visible action as utterance. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lausberg, H., & Sloetjes, H. (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, 41(3), 841–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leavens, D., Hopkins, W., & Bard, K. (2008). The heterochronic origins of explicit reference. In Zlatev, J., Racine, T., Sinha, C., & Itkonen, E. (Eds.), The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity, (pp. 187214). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Child Language, 7(2), 173–87.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007a). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. Journal of Child Language, 34, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007b). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10(2), F1F7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cognitive Development, 26, 1629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masataka, N. (2003). From index-finger extension to index-finger pointing: ontogenesis of pointing in preverbal infants. In Kita, S. (Ed.), Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 6984). Mahwah, NJ/London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Moreno-Núñez, A. (2014). Ostensive gestures in triadic interactions: from rhythmic ostensive gestures of the adult to children's gestures at the end of the first year of life. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. Online <http://hdl.handle.net/10486/664115>..' href=https://scholar.google.com/scholar?q=Moreno-Núñez,+A.+(2014).+Ostensive+gestures+in+triadic+interactions:+from+rhythmic+ostensive+gestures+of+the+adult+to+children's+gestures+at+the+end+of+the+first+year+of+life.+Unpublished+doctoral+thesis,+Universidad+Autónoma+de+Madrid.+Online+.>Google Scholar
Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C., & del Olmo, M. J. (2015). The rhythmic, sonorous and melodic components of adult–child–object interactions between age 2 and 6 months. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 49(4), 737–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C., & del Olmo, M. J. (2017). Rhythmic ostensive gestures: how adults facilitate babies’ entrance into early triadic interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 49, 168–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moro, C., Dupertuis, V., Fardel, S., & Piguet, O. (2015). Investigation of consciousness through ostensions to oneself from the onset of the use-of-object to first language. Cognitive Development, 36, 150–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moro, C., & Rodríguez, C. (1991). ¿Por qué el niño tiende el objeto hacia el adulto? La construcción social de la significación de los objetos. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 53, 99118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mount, A. (2008). Intentions, gestures, and salience in ordinary and deferred demonstrative reference. Mind & Language, 23(2), 145–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parise, E., & Csibra, G. (2013). Neural responses to multimodal ostensive signals in 5-month-old infants. Plos One, 8(8), 19. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072360CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, C. S. (1987). Obra lógico-semiótica. Madrid: Taurus.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936/2007). El nacimiento de la inteligencia en el niño. Barcelona: Ares y Mares.Google Scholar
Racine, T., & Carpendale, J. (2007). The role of shared practice in joint attention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez, C. (2009). The ‘circumstances’ of gestures: proto-interrogatives and private gestures. New Ideas in Psychology, 27, 288303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez, C., Moreno-Núñez, A., Basilio, M., & Sosa, N. (2015). Ostensive gestures come first: their role in the beginning of shared reference. Cognitive Development, 36, 142–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (1999). El mágico número tres. Cuando los niños aún no hablan. Barcelona: Paidós.Google Scholar
Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (2008). Coming to agreement: object use by infants and adults. In Zlatev, J., Racine, T., Sinha, C., & Itkonen, E. (Eds.), The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 89114). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodríguez, C., & Palacios, P. (2007). Do private gestures have a self-regulatory function? A case study. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(2), 180–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez, F. G. (2012). El hermano menor de la palabra. Panorámica de los estudios sobre el gesto. Psiencia, Revista Latinoamericana de Ciencia Psicológica, 4(1), 4356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Southgate, V., Van Maanen, C., & Csibra, G. (2007). Infant pointing: Communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Development, 78(3), 735–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: an organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Wilkins, D. (2003). Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal (in sociocultural and semiotic terms) In Kita, S. (Ed.). Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 171215). Mahwah, NJ/London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar