Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T03:45:41.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dynamic interaction patterns of monolingual and bilingual infants with their parents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 December 2019

Anja GAMPE*
Affiliation:
University of Zurich, Switzerland
Leonie HARTMANN
Affiliation:
University of Konstanz, Germany
Moritz M. DAUM
Affiliation:
University of Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author: University of Zurich – Department of Psychology, Binzmühlestrasse 14/21, Zurich8050, Switzerland. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Bilingual children show a number of advantages in the domain of communication. The aim of the current study was to investigate whether differences in interactions are present before productive language skills emerge. For a duration of 5 minutes, 64 parents and their 14-month-old infants explored a decorated room together. The coordination of their behaviors in the modalities of action, language, and gesture was coded. The results showed no differences in interactions across different language statuses. In two additional analyses, we first compared monolinguals and bilinguals with caregivers who shared the same language and culture. Results showed the same pattern of non-difference. Second, we compared bilinguals with caregivers from different cultures. The rate and duration of coordination differed across infants with different cultural backgrounds. The findings suggest that exposure to two languages is not sufficient to explain the previously identified beneficial effects in the communicative interactions of bilingual children.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 207–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: a critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 699714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., García, M., & Cortez, C. (2005). Conceptual versus monolingual scoring: When does it make a difference? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36(3), 188200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Tal, J., Ludemann, P., Toda, S., Rahn, C. W., … Vardi, D. (1992). Maternal responsiveness to infants in three societies: the United States, France, and Japan. Child Development, 63(4), 808–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and Speech, 46(2/3), 217–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Ganiban, J. M., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., Shaw, D. S., Reiss, D., & Leve, L. D. (2018). Contributions of mothers’ and fathers’ parenting to children's self-regulation: evidence from an adoption study. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brito, N., & Barr, R. (2012). Influence of bilingualism on memory generalization during infancy. Developmental Science, 15(6), 812–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byers-Heinlein, K., Fennell, C. T., & Werker, J. F. (2013). The development of associative word learning in monolingual and bilingual infants. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 198205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabrera, N. J., Volling, B. L., & Barr, R. (2018). Fathers are parents, too! Widening the lens on parenting for children's development. Child Development Perspectives, 12(3), 152–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattani, A., Abbot-Smith, K., Farag, R., Krott, A., Arreckx, F., Dennis, I., & Floccia, C. (2014). How much exposure to English is necessary for a bilingual toddler to perform like a monolingual peer in language tests? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(6), 649–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, R. F. A., van der Steen, S., Guevara, M., de Jonge-Hoekstra, L., & van Dijk, M. (2016). Chromatic and anisotropic cross-recurrence quantification analysis of interpersonal behavior. In Webber, C. L., Ioana, C., & Marwan, N. (Eds.), Recurrence plots and their quantifications: expanding horizons (Vol. 180, pp. 209225). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29922-8_11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2006). Unraveling the dyad: using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Language Learning, 56(3), 391430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D., Jergovic, D., Imami, Z., & Theodos, V. (1997). Monolingual and bilingual children's use of the mutual exclusivity constraint. Journal of Child Language, 24(1), 324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Jonge-Hoekstra, L., Van der Steen, S., Van Geert, P., & Cox, R. F. A. (2016). Asymmetric dynamic attunement of speech and gestures in the construction of children's understanding. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00473CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmen, R. A. G., Malda, M., Mesman, J., Ekmekci, H., & IJzendoorn, M. H. van (2012). Sensitive parenting as a cross-cultural ideal: sensitivity beliefs of Dutch, Moroccan, and Turkish mothers in the Netherlands. Attachment & Human Development, 14(6), 601–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: a magnetoencephalography study. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fouts, H. N., Roopnarine, J. L., Lamb, M. E., & Evans, M. (2012). Infant social interactions with multiple caregivers: the importance of ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(2), 328–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fusaroli, R., Konvalinka, I., & Wallot, S. (2014). Analyzing social interactions: the promises and challenges of using cross recurrence quantification analysis. In Marwan, N., Riley, M., Giuliani, A., & Webber, C. L., (Eds.), Translational recurrences (Vol. 103, pp. 137155). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-09531-8_9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gampe, A., Wermelinger, S., & Daum, M. M. (2019). Bilingual children adapt to the needs of their communication partners, monolinguals do not. Child Development, 90(1), 98107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(13), 8030–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. A., & Argyle, M. (1975). A cross-cultural study of the communication of extra-verbal meaning by gestures. International Journal of Psychology, 10(1), 5767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, E. T. (1989). Beyond culture. New York: Anchor.Google Scholar
Hallers-Haalboom, E. T., Mesman, J., Groeneveld, M. G., Endendijk, J. J., van Berkel, S. R., van der Pol, L. D., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2014). Mothers, fathers, sons and daughters: parental sensitivity in families with two children. Journal of Family Psychology, 28(2), 138–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampden-Turner, C., & Trompenaars, F. (2000). Building cross-cultural competence: how to create wealth from conflicting values. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Kalashnikova, M., Escudero, P., & Kidd, E. (2018). The development of fast-mapping and novel word retention strategies in monolingual and bilingual infants. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalashnikova, M., Mattock, K., & Monaghan, P. (2015). The effects of linguistic experience on the flexible use of mutual exclusivity in word learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 626–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kärtner, J., Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R. D., Chaudhary, N., & Su, Y. (2008). Similarities and differences in contingency experiences of 3-month-olds across sociocultural contexts. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(3), 488500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kärtner, J., Keller, H., & Yovsi, R. D. (2010). Mother–infant interaction during the first 3 months: the emergence of culture-specific contingency patterns. Child Development, 81(2), 540–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidd, C., Palmeri, H., & Aslin, R. N. (2013). Rational snacking: young children's decision-making on the marshmallow task is moderated by beliefs about environmental reliability. Cognition, 126(1), 109–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Flexible learning of multiple speech structures in bilingual infants. Science, 325(5940), 611–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lancy, D. F. (2008). The anthropology of childhood: cherubs, chattel, changelings. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leonardi, G., Nomikou, I., Rohlfing, K. J., & Rączaszek-Leonardi, J. (2016). Vocal interactions at the dawn of communication: the emergence of mutuality and complementarity in mother–infant interaction. 2016 Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-EpiRob) (pp. 288–93). https://doi.org/10.1109/DEVLRN.2016.7846835CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & Vos, C. de (2012). A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucassen, N., Tharner, A., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Volling, B. L., Verhulst, F. C., … Tiemeier, H. (2011). The association between paternal sensitivity and infant–father attachment security: a meta-analysis of three decades of research. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(6), 986–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meissner, F., & Vertovec, S. (2015). Comparing super-diversity. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 38(4), 541–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercure, E., Kushnerenko, E., Goldberg, L., Bowden-Howl, H., Coulson, K., Johnson, M. H., & MacSweeney, M. (2019). Language experience influences audiovisual speech integration in unimodal and bimodal bilingual infants. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesman, J., Minter, T., Angnged, A., Cissé, I. A. H., Salali, G. D., & Migliano, A. B. (2018). Universality without uniformity: a culturally inclusive approach to sensitive responsiveness in infant caregiving. Child Development, 89(3), 837–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicoladis, E., Pika, S., & Marentette, P. (2009). Do French–English bilingual children gesture more than monolingual children? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38(6), 573–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43(1), 93120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pika, S., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. F. (2006). A cross-cultural study on the use of gestures: Evidence for cross-linguistic transfer? Bilingualism, 9(3), 319–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Retrieved from <http://www.R-project.org>..>Google Scholar
Richman, A. L., Miller, P. M., & LeVine, R. A. (1992). Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 614–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., Mosier, C., Chavajay, P., & Heath, S. B. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(8), i179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohlfing, K. J., & Nomikou, I. (2014). Intermodal synchrony as a form of maternal responsiveness: association with language development. Language, Interaction and Acquisition, 5(1), 117–36.Google Scholar
Sherman, J., & Nicoladis, E. (2004). Gestures by advanced Spanish–English second-language learners. Gesture, 4(2), 143–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Maternal responsiveness and early language acquisition. In Kail, R. V. & Reese, H. W. (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 29, pp. 89127). https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2407(02)80052-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ting-Toomey, S. (1999). Communicating across cultures. New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Verhagen, J., Grassmann, S., & Küntay, A. C. (2017). Monolingual and bilingual children's resolution of referential conflicts: effects of bilingualism and relative language proficiency. Cognitive Development, 41, 1018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webber, C. L., & Zbilut, J. P. (2007). Recurrence quantifications: feature extractions from recurrence plots. International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, 17(10), 3467–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wermelinger, S., Gampe, A., & Daum, M. M. (2017). Bilingual toddlers have advanced abilities to repair communication failure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 155, 8494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whaley, S. E., Sigman, M., Beckwith, L., Cohen, S. E., & Espinosa, M. P. (2002). Infant-caregiver interaction in kenya and the United States: the importance of multiple caregivers and adequate comparison samples. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33(3), 236–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yow, W. Q. (2015). Monolingual and bilingual preschoolers’ use of gestures to interpret ambiguous pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1394–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yow, W. Q., & Markman, E. M. (2011). Young bilingual children's heightened sensitivity to referential cues. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(1), 1231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yow, W. Q., & Markman, E. M. (2016). Children increase their sensitivity to a speaker's nonlinguistic cues following a communicative breakdown. Child Development, 87(2), 385–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeegers, M. A. J., Vente, W. de, Nikolić, M., Majdandžić, M., Bögels, S. M., & Colonnesi, C. (2018). Mothers’ and fathers’ mind-mindedness influences physiological emotion regulation of infants across the first year of life. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed