Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T02:55:42.137Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's epistemic inferences through modal verbs and prosody

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 March 2020

Meghan ARMSTRONG*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts Amherst, USA
*
Address for correspondence: 420 Herter Hall, 161 President's Drive, UMass Amherst, Amherst, MA01001, USA. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study explores how young children infer nuances in epistemic modality through prosody. A forced-choice task was used, testing children's (ages three to seven) comprehension of the might/will distinction (modal condition) as well their ability to modulate the strength of might through two prosodic tunes (prosody condition). Positive and negative valence conditions were included. Younger children were shown to start off performing above chance for the modal condition, and at around chance for the prosody condition, but after age four performance on the prosody condition quickly improved. For both modal verbs and prosody, children performed significantly better when valence was positive. By age seven, children performed at ceiling for all conditions. Qualitative analysis of children's justifications for prosody responses showed metalinguistic awareness of prosodic meaning as early as age four, with the ability to relate prosody to epistemic modal meaning becoming quite common by age seven.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Armstrong, M. E. (2014). Child comprehension of intonationally-encoded disbelief. In Orman, W. & Valleau, M. J. (Eds.), BUCLD 38: Proceedings of the 38th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (vol. 2, pp. 2538). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Armstrong, M. E. (2017). Accounting for intonational form and function in Puerto Rican Spanish polar questions. Probus, 29(1), 140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, M. E. (2018). Production of mental state intonation in the speech of toddlers and their caretakers. Language Acquisition, 25(2), 119–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, M. E., Andreu, L., Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto, P. (2016). Children's processing of morphosyntactic and prosodic cues in overriding context-based hypotheses: an eyetracking study. Probus, 28(1), 5790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, M., Esteve-Gibert, N., Hübscher, I., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P. (2018). Developmental and cognitive aspects of children's disbelief comprehension through intonation and facial gesture. First Language, 38(6), 596616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, M. E., & Hübscher, I. (2018). Children's development of internal state prosody. In Prieto, P. & Esteve-Gibert, N. (Eds.), The development of prosody in first language acquisition (pp. 271–94). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, M. E., & Lesho, M. (2017). Intonationally-encoded implicatures and regional variation in American English imperatives. Poster presentation at the Linguistic Society of America 2017 Meeting, Austin, TX.Google Scholar
Armstrong, M. E., & Prieto, P. (2015). The contribution of context and contour to perceived belief in polar questions. Journal of Pragmatics, 81, 7792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, M. E., & Schwenter, S. (2016). Epistemic modulation through intonation in American English. Paper presented at the Linguistic Society of America 2016 Meeting, Washington, DC. Online <https://www.academia.edu/20092091/Epistemic_modulation_through_intonation_in_American_English_-_Paper_presented_at_the_LSA_Annual_Meeting_2016_-_Washington_DC>..>Google Scholar
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(3), 614–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckman, M. E., Hirschberg, J., & Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (2004). The original ToBI system and the evolution of the ToBI framework. In Jun, S.-A. (Ed.), Prosodic typology (pp. 954). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benuš, Š., Gravano, A., & Hirschberg, J. (2007). Prosody, emotions and … ‘whatever’. Proceedings of the 8th Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association (INTERPSPEECH 2007) (pp. 2629–32). Online <http://www1.cs.columbia.edu/nlp/papers/2007/benus_al_07a.pdf>.Google Scholar
Bever, T. G., Melher, J., & Epstein, J. (1968). What children do in spite of what they know. Science, 162, 921–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J., & Lucariello, J. (1989). Monologue as narrative of the world. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 7397). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., & Duff, M. A. (1989). Young children's comprehension of modal expressions. Cognitive Development, 4(4), 369–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutler, A., & Swinney, D. A. (1987). Prosody and the development of comprehension. Journal of Child Language, 14, 145–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frick, R. W. (1985). Communicating emotion: the role of prosodic features. Psychological Bulletin, 97(3), 412–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujisaki, H. (1997). Prosody, models and spontaneous speech. In Sagisaka, Y., Campbell, N., & Higuchi, N. (Eds.), Computing prosody: computational models for processing spontaneous speech (pp. 2742). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, R., & Greeno, J. (1989). On the nature of competence: principles for understanding a domain. In Resnick, L. (Ed.), Knowing and learning: essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 125–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gravano, A., Benuš, Š., Hirschberg, J., German, E. S., & Ward, G. (2008). The effect of contour type and epistemic modality on the assessment of speaker certainty. In Barbosa, P. A., Madureira, S., & Reis, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 401–4). Campinas, Brazil, 6–9 May. Online <http://sprosig.org/sp2008/papers/id033.pdf>.Google Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (2004). The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (2004 [1975]). Learning how to mean. In Walker, J. (Ed.), The language of early childhood: collected works of M.A.K. Halliday (vol. 4, pp. 2859). London/New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
Hara, Y., Kawahara, S., & Feng, Y. (2014). The prosody of enhanced bias in Mandarin and Japanese negative questions. Lingua, 150, 92116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L., de Rosnay, M., & Pons, F. (2005). Language and children's understanding of mental states. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(2), 6973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirschberg, J., & Ward, G. (1985). Fall–rise intonation and the place of intonational meaning. In Proceedings of the Eleventh Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 447–58). Online <https://journals.linguisticsociety.org/proceedings/index.php/BLS/article/view/1893>.Google Scholar
Hirschberg, J., & Ward, G. (1992). The influence of pitch range, duration, amplitude and spectral features on the interpretation of the rise-fall-rise intonation contour in English. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 241–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirschberg, J., & Ward, G. (1995). The interpretation of the high-rise question contour in English. Journal of Pragmatics, 24, 402–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hualde, J. I., & Prieto, P. (2015). Intonational variation in Spanish: European and American varieties. In Frota, S. & Prieto, P. (Eds.), Intonational variation in Romance (pp. 350–91). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hübscher, I. (2018). Preschoolers’ pragmatic development: how prosody and gesture lend a helping hand. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from <http://prosodia.upf.edu/home/arxiu/tesis/doctorat/tesi_Iris_H%C3%BCbscher.pdf>..>Google Scholar
Hübscher, I., Esteve-Gibert, N., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P. (2017). Intonation and gesture as bootstrapping devices in speaker uncertainty. First Language, 37(1), 2441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hübscher, I., & Prieto, P. (2019). Gestural and prosodic development act as sister systems and jointly pave the way for children's sociopragmatic development. Frontiers in Psychology, 10:1259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttar, G. L. (1968). Relations between prosodic variables and emotions in normal American English utterances. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 11, 481–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, M., & Kaufman, S. (2012). Epistemic particles and performativity. In Chereches, A. (Ed.), Proceedings of the 22nd Semantics and Linguistic Theory Conference (pp. 208–25). Chicago, IL. Retrieved from <http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.714.241&rep=rep1&type=pdf>.Google Scholar
Krahmer, E., & Swerts, M. (2005). How children and adults produce and perceive uncertainty in audiovisual speech. Language and Speech, 48(1), 2953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurumada, C., & Clark, E. (2017). Pragmatic inferences in context: learning to interpret contrastive prosody. Journal of Child Language, 44(4), 850–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Michelas, A., Portes, C., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2015). When pitch accents encode speaker commitment: evidence from French intonation. Language and Speech, 59(2), 266–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., Bryant, D., & Furrow, D. (1989). Mental terms and the development of certainty. Child Development, 60, 167–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, C., Pure, K., & Furrow, D. (1990). Children's understanding of the modal expression of speaker certainty and uncertainty and its relation to the development of a representational theory of mind. Child Development, 61(3), 722–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, C., Harris, L., & Patriquin, M. (1993). Lexical and prosodic cues in the comprehension of relative certainty. Journal of Child Language, 20(1), 153–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, J. B., & Trehub, S. E. (2001). Children's understanding of emotion in speech. Child Development, 72(3), 834–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, N., & Russell, J. A. (2011). Preschoolers’ use of dynamic facial, bodily and vocal cues to emotion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 5261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Neill, D. K., & Atance, C. M. (2000). ‘Maybe my daddy give me a big piano’: the development of children's use of modals to express uncertainty. First Language, 20(58), 2952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozturk, O., & Papafragou, A. (2015). The acqisition of epistemic modality: from semantic meaning to pragmatic interpretation. Language Learning and Development, 11, 191214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, F. R. (2013). Modality and the English modals (2nd ed.). New York/London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A. (1998). The acquisition of modality: implications for theories of semantic represetation. Mind and Language, 13, 370–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. (1980). The phonetics and phonology of English intonation. New York: Garland Press.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J., & Hirschberg, J. (1990). The meaning of inonational contours in the intpretation of discourse. In Cohen, P., Morgan, J., & Pollack, M. (Eds.), Intentions in communication (pp. 271311). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pon-Barry, H. (2008). Prosodic manifestations of confidence and uncertainty in spoken language. Proceedings of the 9th Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association (Interspeech 2008) (pp. 74–7). Online <https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/7e0d/2e15ef41c294e732189233329f1045f81e54.pdf>.Google Scholar
Portes, C., & Lancia, L. (2017). Earlier or higher? Comparing French rising-falling contour with rising contour in a corpus of conversation. Journal of Phonetics, 63, 3552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prieto, P. (2014). The intonational phonology of Catalan. In Jun, S.-A. (Ed.), Prosodic typology II: the phonology of intonation and phrasing (pp. 4380). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prieto, P., & Borràs-Comes, J. (2018). Question intonation contours as dynamic epistemic operators. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 36(2), 563–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prieto, P., Estrella, A., Thorson, J., & Vanrell, M. M. (2012). Is prosodic development correlated with grammatical development? Evidence from emerging intonation in Catalan and Spanish. Journal of Child Language, 39(2), 221–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quam, C., & Swingley, D. (2012). Development in children's interpretation of pitch cues to emotions. Child Development, 83(1), 236–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2017). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Online <https://www.R-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Roseano, P., González, M., Borràs-Comes, J., & Prieto, P. (2016). Communicating epistemic stance: how speech and gesture patterns reflect epistemicity and evidentiality. Discourse Processes, 53, 135–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., & Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: a systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state. Cognition, 14(3), 301–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tardif, T., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin- and Cantonese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 3543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ünal, E., & Papafragou, A. (2016). Production-comprehension asymmetries and the acquisition of evidential morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 179–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanrell, M. M., Ballone, F., Schirru, C., & Prieto, P. (2015). Intonation and its interfaces in Sardinian polar questions. Loquens, 1(2), e014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanrell, M. M., Mascaró, I., Torres-Tamarit, F., & Prieto, P. (2013). Intonation as an encoder of speaker certainty: information and confirmation yes-no questions in Catalan. Language and Speech, 56(2), 163–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Visser, M., Krahmer, E., & Swerts, M. (2014). Children's expression of uncertainty in collaborative and competitive contexts. Language and Speech, 57(1), 86107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, G., & Hirschberg, J. (1985). Implicating uncertainty: the pragmatics of fall-rise intonation. Language, 61, 747–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, G., & Hirschberg, J. (1988). Intonation and propositional attitude: the pragmatics of L*+H LH %. In Powers, J. (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fifth Eastern States Conference on Linguistics (pp. 512–22). Online <https://www.researchgate.net/publication/230876643_Intonation_and_Propositional_Attitude_The_Pragmatics_of_LH_L_H>.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar