Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T13:57:32.794Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

But: Do age and working memory influence conventional implicature processing?*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2014

LEEN JANSSENS*
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, KU Leuven (University of Leuven)
STEPHANIE DROOGHMANS
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, KU Leuven (University of Leuven)
WALTER SCHAEKEN
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychology, KU Leuven (University of Leuven)
*
Address for correspondence: Leen Janssens, Tiensestraat 102 ( Postbox 3711, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. tel: 0032 16 325873; fax: 0032 16 326099; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Conventional implicatures are omnipresent in daily life communication but experimental research on this topic is sparse, especially research with children. The aim of this study was to investigate if eight- to twelve-year-old children spontaneously make the conventional implicature induced by but, so, and nevertheless in ‘p but q’ sentences. Additionally, the study aimed to shed light on the cognitive effort required for these inferences by measuring working memory (WM) capacity. Our results show that children do make these inferences to a certain extent, but are sensitive to the content of the arguments. We found a significant effect of sentence type, but did not observe any developmental effect, nor any effect of WM: a higher age or WM capacity does not result in more pragmatic inferences.

Type
Brief Research Reports
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We would like to thank the two schools, ‘De Klimboom’ and ‘GVBS Klavertje 4’, for allowing us to perform our experiment. We would also like to thank Pieter Moors for his help with the analyses. This research was carried out with the financial support of the National Council for Scientific Research – Flanders, Belgium (FWO grant G.0634.09).

References

REFERENCES

Anscombre, J. & Ducrot, O. (1977). Deux ‘mais’ en français? Lingua 43, 2340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J. & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M. & Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes, R package version 0.999375-42, online: <http://www.R-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Blakemore, D. (2002). Relevance and linguistic meaning: the semantics and pragmatics of discourse markers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bott, L. & Noveck, I. A. (2004). Some utterances are underinformative: the onset and time course of scalar inferences. Journal of Memory and Language 51, 437457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. & Rumain, B. (1981). Development of comprehension of ‘or’: evidence for a sequence of competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 31, 4670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Neys, W. & Schaeken, W. (2007). When people are more logical under cognitive load: dual task impact on scalar implicature. Experimental Psychology 54, 128133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dieussaert, K., Verkerk, S., Gillard, E. & Schaeken, W. (2011). Some effort for some: further evidence that scalar implicatures are effortful. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 64, 23522367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3, 410419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Haeseryn, W., Romijn, K., Geerts, G., de Rooij, J. & van den Toorn, M. C. (1997). Algemene Nederlandse spraakkunst. Groningen & Leuven: Wolters Noordhoff.Google Scholar
Hall, A. (2004). The meaning of but: a procedural reanalysis. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics 16, 199236.Google Scholar
Horn, L. R. (2004). Implicature. In Horn, L. R. & Ward, G. (eds), The handbook of pragmatics, 328. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical Data Analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janssens, L. & Schaeken, W. (2012). The role of task characteristics in children's scalar implicature production. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Cogsci, 509514, Sapporo, Japan.Google Scholar
Janssens, L. & Schaeken, W. (2013). ‘But’ how do we reason with it: an experimental investigation of the implicature stemming from ‘but’. Journal of Pragmatics 57, 194209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lepère, J. (2008). Het gebruik van ‘maar’ en ‘toch’ bij negenjarige kinderen. Eenexperimenteelonderzoek. Unpublished master's thesis, Catholic University of Leuven, Faculty of Arts, Department of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Noveck, I. A. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition 78, 165188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noveck, I. A. & Posada, A. (2003). Characterizing the time course of an implicature: an evoked potentials study. Brain and Language 85, 203210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papafragou, A. & Musolino, J. (2003). Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognition 86, 253282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaeken, W., Sevenants, A. & Madruga, J. (2011). The dramatic effect of content on children's ‘unless’ reasoning: pragmatic modulation or reconstruction?Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 11701175, Cogsci, Boston, US.Google Scholar
Van Belle, W. (2003). Zwijgen is niet altijd toestemmen: De rol van inferenties bij het interpreteren en argumenteren. Leuven: Uitgeverij Acco.Google Scholar
Van Belle, W. & Devroy, G. (1992). Tegenstellende en toegevende connectoren. Eenargumentatievebeschrijving (Preprint 143, voorlopigepublicatie). Catholic University of Leuven, Faculty of Arts, Department of Linguistics.Google Scholar