Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T17:12:00.189Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On the pragmatics of contrast*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Eve V. Clark*
Affiliation:
Stanford University
*
Department of Linguistics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA.

Abstract

In this paper, I review properties and consequences of the PRINCIPLE OF CONTRAST. This principle, which I have argued from the beginning has a pragmatic basis, captures facts about the inferences speakers and addressees make for both conventional and novel words. Along with a PRINCIPLE OF CONVENTIONALITY, it accounts for the pre-emption of novel words by well-established ones. And it holds just as much for morphology as it does for words and larger expressions. In short, Contrast has the major properties Gathercole (1989) proposed as characteristic of her alternative to Contrast.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Preparation of this paper was supported in part by the Sloan Foundation. I would like to thank Dwight Bolinger, Willem J. Levelt, Brian MacWhinney and Dan I. Slobin for raising some of the issues I address here, and in particular Herbert H. Clark for discussion of and comments on an earlier draft.

This article concludes a discussion initiated in JCL by Clark (1988) and continued by Gathercole (1989). Ed.

References

REFERENCES

Aimard, P. (1975). Les jeux de mots des enfants. Villeurbanne: Simép Editions.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koc, A. & Slobin, D. I. (1985). The acquisition of Turkish. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1. The data. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
American Heritage (1970). The American heritage dictionary of the English language. New York: American Heritage Publishing Co. & Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Anisfeld, M. & Gordon, M. (1968). On the psychophonological structure of English inflectional rules. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 7. 973–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Au, Terry K.-F. & Markman, E. M. (1987). Acquiring new words via linguistic contrast. Cognitive Development 2. 217–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Clark, E. V. (1989). Learning to use compounds for contrast: data from Hebrew. First Language 9. 247270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berman, R. A. & Sagi, Y. (1981). al darxey tetsurat hamilim ve xidushan bagil hats'ir [Word-formation processes and lexical innovations of young children]. Hebrew Computational Linguistics Bulletin 18. 3662.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. L. (1956). Subjunctive -ra and -se: free variation? Hispania 39. 345–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. L. (1967). Adjectives in English: attribution and predication. Lingua 18. 134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. L. (1968). Entailment and the meaning of structures. Glossa 2. 119–27.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. L. (1971). The phrasal verb in English. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. L. (1972). That's that. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolinger, D. L. (1977). Meaning and form. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Borkin, A. (1984). Problems in form and function. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1982). Evaluating competing linguistic models with language acquisition data: implications of developmental errors with causative verbs. Quaderni di Semantica 3. 566.Google Scholar
Bréal, M. (1897). Essai de sémantique. Paris: Hachette.Google Scholar
Budwig, N. (1986). Agentivity and control in early child language. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
Budwig, N. (1989). The linguistic marking of agentivity and control in child language. Journal of Child Language 16. 263–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bybee, J. L. & Slobin, D. I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58. 265–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word-learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J. & Miller, G. A. (eds), Linguistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 15, 1729.Google Scholar
Cassell, . (1971). Cassell's modern guide to synonyms and related words. London: Cassell.Google Scholar
Cazden, C. B. (1968). The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development 39. 433–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. (1980). Convention and contrast in acquiring the lexicon. Keynote Address, 12th Annual Child Language Research Forum, Stanford University (April). Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 19. 120.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1981). Lexical innovations: how children learn to create new words. In Werner, Deutsch (ed.), The child's construction of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1982). The young word-maker: a case study of innovation in the child's lexicon. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1983). Meanings and concepts. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (eds), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Cognitive development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). The Principle of Contrast: a constraint on acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1988). On the logic of Contrast. Journal of Child Language 15. 317–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. & Berman, R. A. (1984). Structure and use in the acquisition of word formation. Language 60. 542–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). Types of linguistic knowledge: interpreting and producing compound nouns. Journal of Child Language 14. 547–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V. & Clark, H. H. (1979). When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55. 767811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Cohen, S. A. (1984). Productivity and memory for newly formed words. Journal of Child Language 11. 611–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V., Gelman, S. A. & Lane, N. M. (1985). Noun compounds and category structure in young children. Child Development 56. 8494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Hecht, B. F. (1982). Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition 12. 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E. V., Hecht, B. F. & Mulford, R. C. (1986). Coining complex compounds in English: affixes and word order in acquisition. Linguistics 24. 729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1983). Making sense of nonce sense. In d'Arcais, G. Flores & Jarvella, R. (eds), The process of understanding language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Carlson, T. B. (1982). Speech acts and hearers' beliefs. In Smith, N. V. (ed.), Mutual knowledge. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Marshall, C. R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In Joshi, A., Webber, B. & Sag, I. (eds), Elements of discourse understanding. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Dockrell, J. E. (1981). The child's acquisition of unfamiliar words: an experimental study. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Stirling, Scotland.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. (1989). Contrast: a semantic constraint? Journal of Child Language 16. 685702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerhardt, J. (1988). From discourse to semantics: the development of verb morphology and forms of self-reference in the speech of a two-year-old. Journal of Child Language 15. 337–93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Baduini, C. & Lavallee, A. (1985). What's in a word? The young child's predisposition to use lexical contrast. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Child Language, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. L. (eds), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3. Speech acts. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gruber, J. S. (1976). Lexical structures in syntax and semantics. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Haiman, J. (1980). The iconicity of grammar: isomorphism and motivation. Language 56. 515–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, L. R. (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. II (1977). The acqusition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16. 589600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, David K. (1969). Convention: a philosophical study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lo Duca, M. G. (1988). Acquisizione del linguaggio e formazione dei ‘nomina agentis’ in italiano. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Padua, Italy.Google Scholar
Lyamina, G. M. (1960). Razvitie ponimaniya rechi u deteǐ vtorogo goda zhizni. Voprosy Psikhologii 3. 106–21.Google Scholar
McCawley, J. D. (1978). Conversational implicature and the lexicon. In Cole, P. (ed.), Syntax and semantics, vol. 9. Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1973). How Hungarian children learn to speak. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology 20. 121–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulford, R. C. (1983). On the acquisition of derivational morphology in Icelandic: learning about -ari. Islensk mál og almenn málfraedi 5. 104–25.Google Scholar
Paul, H. (1898). Principien der Sprachgeschichte (3rd ed.). Halle: Max Niemeyer Verlag.Google Scholar
Room, A. (1981). Room's dictionary of distinguishables. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Saussure, Ferdinand de (1919/1968). Cours de linguistique générale (publié par Charles Bally & Albert Sechehaye). Paris: Payot.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (1985 a). Developmental paths between form and meaning: crosslinguistic and diachronic perspectives. Keynote Address, 10th Annual Boston University Conference on Child Language, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (ed.) (1985 b). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stern, C. & Stern, W. (1928). Die Kindersprache: eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung (2nd ed.). Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Toivainen, J. (1980). Inflectional affixes used by Finnish-speaking children aged 1–3 years. Helsinki: Suomilaisen Kirjallisuuden Seura.Google Scholar
Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar