Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-19T00:51:00.707Z Has data issue: true hasContentIssue false

Preventing the risk of iatrogenic harm when assessing and diagnosing functional neurological disorders and other functional somatic symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 September 2024

Giovanni Stanghellini*
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, University of Florence, Florence, Italy Centro de Estudios de Fenomenologìa y Psiquiatrìa, ‘Diego Portales’ University, Santiago, Chile
George Ikkos
Affiliation:
Royal National Orthopaedic Hospital, Stanmore, UK
Cecilia Maria Esposito
Affiliation:
Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, IRCCS Fondazione Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Milan, Italy
*
Corresponding author: Giovanni Stanghellini; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

The present commentary raises some concerns about the risk of iatrogenic harm arising out of the diagnosis of functional neurologic and somatic disorders. These concerns are supported by evidence from the history of hysteria and findings from contemporary brain imaging. We discuss their implications for practice.

Type
Viewpoint
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatric Association

Functional neurological symptoms (FNSs) and functional somatic symptoms (FSSs) including, for example, fibromyalgia and unexplained medical symptoms are clinical presentations that tread the border between the psychic and the somatic. In general, they come much more often to the attention of the general practitioner or neurologist than the psychiatrist. Terminology is varied and can be confusing but Creed et al. [Reference Creed, Barsky, Leiknes, Creed, Henningsen and Fink1] report that medically unexplained symptoms are the presenting problem in up to 35–53% of new patients at specialist medical clinics and FSS have been found to be present in up to a third of healthcare consultations both in primary care and in specialist practice [Reference Burton, Fink, Henningsen, Löwe and Rief2]. With respect to functional neurological disorders (FNDs), FSS, and FND/psychiatric comorbidity, Carle-Toulemonde et al. [Reference Carle-Toulemonde, Goutte, Do-Quang-Cantagrel, Mouchabac, Joly and Garcin3] found that fatigue is the most common symptom reported in FND (from 47 to 93%). Psychopathological disorders are reported in 40 to 100% FND patients, anxiety disorders being the most frequent, followed by mood disorders and neurodevelopmental disorders. Stress factors such as childhood trauma (mainly emotional neglect and physical abuse) have also been identified in up to 75% of FND patients, along with maladaptive coping strategies.

Recently, neurologists have paid greater attention to these clinical conditions, leading to important reflections. For example, Stone et al. [Reference Stone, Hoeritzauer, McWhirter and Carson4] propose to overcome the conceptual mind–body gap by understanding FND as a “predictive brain” disorder. Changes observed with neuroimaging techniques in the network involved in sense of agency “–the parts of the brain that let you know that it is ‘you’ who made a movement–”, as they argue, may explain why it would be difficult for FND patients to acquire awareness of their own bodily state and movements and thus adjust future action in the light of accurate perceptual feedback.

We applaud the endeavor to promote the constructive engagement of neurologists in the care of patients with FND. It is a major advance in clinical practice. However, we also have some concern that a perverse effect of this genuine progress may be to lead some psychiatrists to conclude that, for practical purposes, FND is a brain or neurological disorder. This may lead to losing out on useful insights generated by the rich history of different approaches. Therefore, we stress that each patient presenting with FNS and FSS requires full assessment of all relevant clinical phenomena, not only neurological ones, but also psychopathological and psychological factors. Also, we must consider not only the neurological and psycho(patho)logical correlates of functional disorders, but also the sociocultural roots that often underly them.

Psychiatric disorders, including neuropsychiatric syndromes, are not a-temporal entities but are influenced by changes in society and the medical paradigm. Changes in medical knowledge can produce changes in the presentation of clinical conditions as well as their medical formulation and treatment. Arguably, FND, FSS, etc., are (at least in part) modern labels for “hysteria” – a set of clinical phenomena that have historically been greatly influenced by the social context. The term “hysteria” has justly been criticized for its misogynistic provenance and associated discriminatory practice, but this does not mean we cannot learn from its history in a way that the total abolition of reference to the term would make impossible [Reference Esposito and Stanghellini5]. Here, we focus on some medical and social dynamics that may contribute to the onset, maintenance, and adverse prognosis of these varied and overlapping clinical presentations, if not appropriately addressed. For the avoidance of doubt: in advocating our thesis below, we do not suggest that every patient who presents with functional or medically unexplained symptoms conforms to our analysis. Only a significant number does, and this is across the various contemporary diagnoses of FND, FSS, etc.

Hysterical symptoms are known to be highly influenced by the doctor–patient relationship. Didi-Huberman [Reference Didi-Huberman6] has described how the “modern” delineation of hysteria, dating back to Charcot during the 19th century, was strongly affected by a climate of seductiveness between doctor and patient. To get doctors’ attention, this apparently led the latter to “mimic” hysteria in line with then current medical formulations of the condition. The need for recognition by others, which represents an evolutionarily acquired profound human need [Reference Baumeister and Leary7], becomes the only way for people in the grip of hysterical dynamics to feel validated in their existence. Medical malpractice, when it has often ridiculed or overseen these presentations, has then served to exacerbate this need.

For those with a magmatic and inconstant perception of their own body from within (i.e., coenesthetically), the optical imprint of the other on themselves takes on particular importance: “Videor ergo sum - I am seen therefore I am” [Reference Esposito and Stanghellini5]. The core of hysteria has in fact been described as a “manque d’être” (‘lack of being’). The attempt to compensate for this lack gives rise to the need to adapt to models and identifications proposed from outside [Reference Esposito and Stanghellini8]. While we agree that patients presenting with these symptoms are not malingering, fabricating, or pretending, or lying, we would argue that their utterances and behavior have a performative function, that of capturing the gaze of the clinician. We refer here to the interested, not the dismissive clinician!

It is not surprising that the medical gaze, with its promise not only of recognition but also of diagnostic definition, has over time been one of the privileged targets of hysterical capture. The medical gaze serves the search for identity and diagnosis and represents a “device of subjectivation” [Reference Foucault9] for these patients. Some, medical doctors, in turn, respond with great attentiveness, as such presentations literally embody a fundamental question that has fascinated medicine since its beginnings – the mysterious relationship between soma and psyche. By embodying this mystery, people with hysterical symptoms catch the medical eye and gain much needed attention and recognition. Furthermore, the anthropological disproportion that sees the optical dimension prevail over the coenesthetic dimension is in accordance with the “Zeitgeist” of today [Reference Stanghellini10]. Biomedicine, with its array of imaging techniques, is one of those “machines to be seen and to be talked about” that philosophers [Reference Deleuze11] refer to. These help to produce a specific kind of subjectivity through a particular kind of process of subjectivation and this mutation has its epicenter in the “society of the image” [Reference Stanghellini10]. To this general social phenomenon, biomedicine adds its contribution by making the body itself the object of ocular knowledge [Reference Foucault9].

From these considerations, a question arises: since hysterical dynamics, including their contemporary declinations as FNS and FSS, are nourished by medical attention and by the visibility so obtained, are we always truly serving the patient’s interest by making these diagnoses or are we reinforcing an intrinsic tendency of their psychopathological functioning? Far from helping clinical discernment, may the diagnosis of FNS and FSS be contributing to iatrogenic harm sometimes?

People who, after a long periplus, finally receive a diagnosis of FNS or FSS are often happy to be recognized as persons suffering from a disease included in medical nosography. Perhaps, sometimes, their aim is to have a diagnosis and medical support, that is, to be investigated as sick without being subjected to the stigma related to a diagnosis of mental disorder – rather than to recover from their symptoms. In addition to this, they may not just be looking for a medical diagnosis and recognition, but also for an identity. To be more precise: they may be looking for a diagnosis to identify with. That’s why these diagnoses can contribute to the long-term maintenance of symptoms, rather than their amelioration. Importantly, it will be noted that recent findings on brain imaging [Reference Stone, Hoeritzauer, McWhirter and Carson4] are highly consistent with our observations about coenesthetic hypofunction in patients surviving with hysterical dynamics, strengthening our sociocultural approach. Of course, brain imaging cannot aspire to capture adequately the dynamics relating to social and medical history and their impact on clinical consultations in primary, neurological, psychiatric, or other care.

In conclusion, it is important that those who care for patients with functional neurological and somatic symptoms do not stop at diagnosis – which it should be noted, is now a positive diagnosis, not one by exclusion [Reference Stone, Hoeritzauer, McWhirter and Carson4]. If we are not to miss any facet of the patients’ experience, it is essential to develop an appropriately rich shared perspective between psychiatrists, neurologists, and GPs. Unless we do so, we will fail to do full justice to the nature of FNS and FSS and the associated distress.

Author contribution

Conceptualization: G.S., G.I., C.M.E.; Supervision: G.S., G.I.; Writing – original draft: C.M.E.; Writing – review and editing: G.S., G.I.

Financial support

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

References

Creed, F, Barsky, A, Leiknes, KA. Epidemiology: prevalence, causes and consequences. In: Creed, F, Henningsen, P, Fink, P, editors. Medically unexplained symptoms, somatisation and bodily distress: developing better clinical services. Cambridge: CUP; 2011;142.Google Scholar
Burton, C, Fink, P, Henningsen, P, Löwe, B, Rief, W on behalf of the EURONET-SOMA Group. Functional somatic disorders: discussion paper for a new common classification for research and clinical use. BMC Med. 2020;18:34. doi:10.1186/s12916-020-1505-4.Google Scholar
Carle-Toulemonde, G, Goutte, J, Do-Quang-Cantagrel, N, Mouchabac, S, Joly, C, Garcin, B. Overall comorbidities in functional neurological disorder: a narrative review. L’Encéphale. 2023;49(4):S24–32.Google Scholar
Stone, J, Hoeritzauer, I, McWhirter, L, Carson, A. Functional neurological disorder: defying dualism. World Psychiatry. 2024;23:53–4. doi:10.1002/wps.21151.Google Scholar
Esposito, CM, Stanghellini, G. What does “being a woman” really mean? A reappraisal of the concept of “hysteria” through the analysis of a phenomenological-dynamic psychotherapy of a case study. Psychiatry Research Case Reports. 2023;1(2):100147. doi:10.1016/j.psycr.2023.100147.Google Scholar
Didi-Huberman, G. Invention of hysteria: Charcot and the photographic iconography of the Salpêtrière. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2004.Google Scholar
Baumeister, RF, Leary, MR. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol Bull. 1995;117(3):497529.Google Scholar
Esposito, CM, Stanghellini, G. The body in question in the existence of hysteric persons: a phenomenological perspective. Psychopathology. 2023;28:17. doi:10.1159/000530355.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. The birth of the clinic: an archeology of medical perception. Paris: PUF; 1969.Google Scholar
Stanghellini, G. The optical-coenaesthetic disproportion in feeding and eating disorders. Eur Psychiatry. 2019;58:70–1. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.02.005.Google Scholar
Deleuze, G. Foucault. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press; 1988.Google Scholar
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.