Toward a Posthuman Model of Character Development
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 December 2014
Some of the debates around the concept of moral enhancement have focused on whether the improvement of a single trait, such as empathy or intelligence, would be a good in general, or in all circumstances. All virtue theories, however, both secular and religious, have articulated multiple virtues that temper and inform one another in the development of a mature moral character. The project of moral enhancement requires a reengagement with virtue ethics and contemporary moral psychology to develop an empirically grounded model of the virtues and a fuller model of character development. Each of these virtues may be manipulable with electronic, psychopharmaceutical, and genetic interventions. A set of interdependent virtues is proposed, along with some of the research pointing to ways such virtues could be enhanced.
1. Persson, I, Savulescu, J. Moral transhumanism. The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 2010;35(6):656–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2. Persson, I, Savulescu, J. The perils of cognitive enhancement and the urgent imperative to enhance the moral character of humanity. Journal of Applied Philosophy 2008;25(3):162–77.Google Scholar
3. Harris, J. “Ethics is for bad guys!” Putting the “moral” into moral enhancement. Bioethics 2013;27(3):169–73.Google Scholar
4. Harris, J. Moral enhancement and freedom. Bioethics 2011;25(2):102–11.Google Scholar
5. Jotterand, F. “Virtue engineering” and moral agency: Will post-humans still need the virtues? AJOB Neuroscience 2011;2(4):3–9.Google Scholar
6. Grant, AM, Schwartz, B. Too much of a good thing: The challenge and opportunity of the inverted U. Perspectives on Psychological Science 2011;6(1):61–76.Google Scholar
7. See note 5, Jotterand 2011.
8. Fröding, BEE. Cognitive enhancement, virtue ethics and the good life. Neuroethics 2011;4(3):223–34.Google Scholar
9. Shook, J. Neuroethics and the possible types of moral enhancement. AJOB Neuroscience 2012;3(4):3–14.Google Scholar
10. Casebear, W. Moral cognition and its neural constituents. Perspectives Nature Neuroscience 2003;4:841–6.Google Scholar
11. Peterson, CP, Seligman, M. Character Strengths and Virtues: A Handbook and Classification. Washington, DC: APA Press; 2004.Google Scholar
12. McGrath, R. Scale- and item-level factor analyses of the VIA inventory of strengths. Assessment 2012 Feb;21(1):4–14.Google Scholar
13. Shryack, J, Steger, MF, Krueger, RF, Kallie, CS. The structure of virtue: An empirical investigation of the dimensionality of the virtues in action inventory of strengths. Personality and Individual Differences 2010;48:714–19.Google Scholar
14. Digman, JM. Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology 1990;41:417–40.Google Scholar
15. Ozer, DJ, Benet-Martinez, V. Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology 2006;57:401–21.Google Scholar
16. Luo, X, Kranzler, HR, Zuo, L, Zhang, H, Wang, S, Gelernter, J. CHRM2 variation predisposes to personality traits of agreeableness and conscientiousness. Human Molecular Genetics 2007;16(13):1557–68.Google Scholar
17. Macdonald, C, Bore, M, Munro, D. Values in action scale and the big 5: An empirical indication of structure. Journal of Research in Personality 2008;42(4):787–99.Google Scholar
18. Bartels, M, van Weegen, F, van Beijsterveldt, CM, Carlier, M, Polderman, TJC, Hoekstra, RA, Boomsma, DI. The five factor model of personality and intelligence: A twin study on the relationship between the two constructs. Personality and Individual Differences 2012;53(4):368–73.Google Scholar
19. Blasi, G, Lo Bianco, L, Taurisano, P, Gelao, B, Romano, R, Fazio, L, et al. Functional variation of the dopamine D2 receptor gene is associated with emotional control as well as brain activity and connectivity during emotion processing in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 2008;29(47):14812–19.Google Scholar
20. See note 15, Ozer, Benet-Martinez 2006.
21. Faraone, SV, Mick, E. Molecular genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatric Genetics 2010;33(1):159–80.Google Scholar
22. Baumeister, RF, Vohs, KD, Tice, DM. The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science 2007;16(6):351–5.Google Scholar
23. Sandberg, A. Cognition enhancement: Upgrading the brain. In Savulescu, J, Kahane, G, ter Meulen, R, eds. Enhancing Human Capacities. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell; 2011.Google Scholar
24. Killgore, WD, Kahn-Greene, ET, Grugle, NL, Killgore, DB, Balkin, TJ. Sustaining executive functions during sleep deprivation: A comparison of caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil. Sleep 2009 Feb;32(2):205–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Sofuoglua, M, DeVito, EE, Waters, AJ, Carroll, KM. Cognitive enhancement as a treatment for drug addictions. Neuropharmacology 2013;64:452–63.Google Scholar
26. Savulescu, J, Sandberg, A. Neuroenhancement of love and marriage: The chemicals between us. Neuroethics 2008;1:31–44.Google Scholar
27. See note 6, Grant, Schwartz 2011.
28. Boyce, CJ, Wood, A, Brown, GDA. The dark side of conscientiousness: Conscientious people experience greater drops in life satisfaction following unemployment. Journal of Research in Personality 2010;44:535–9.Google Scholar
29. Del Barrio, V, Aluja, A, Garcia, LF. Relationship between empathy and the big five personality traits in a sample of Spanish adolescents. Social Behavior and Personality 2004;32(7):677–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30. Carlo, G, Okun, MA, Knight, GP, de Guzman, MRT. The interplay of traits and motives on volunteering: Agreeableness, extraversion, and prosocial value motivation. Personality and Individual Differences 2005;38:1293–305.Google Scholar
31. Rodrigues, SM, Saslow, LR, Garcia, N, John, OP, Keltner, D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2010;107(31):13936–41.Google Scholar
32. Domes, G, Heinrichs, M, Michel, A, Berger, C, Herpertz, SC. Oxytocin improves “mind-reading” in humans. Biological Psychiatry 2007;61:731–3.Google Scholar
33. Guastellaemail, AJ, Einfeld, SL, Gray, KM, Rinehart, NJ, Tonge, BJ, Lambert, TJ, et al. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry 2010;67(1):692–4.Google Scholar
34. Kosfeld, M, Heinrichs, M, Zak, PJ, Fischbacher, U, Fehr, E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 2005 June 2;435:673–6.Google Scholar
35. Zak, PJ, Stanton, AA, Ahmadi, S. Oxytocin increases generosity in humans. PLoS One 2007;2(11):e1128.Google Scholar
36. Thompson, MR, Callaghan, PD, Hunt, GE, Cornish, JL, McGregor, IS. A role for oxytocin and 5-HT(1A) receptors in the prosocial effects of 3,4 methylenedioxymethamphetamine. Neuroscience 2007;146(2):509–14.Google Scholar
37. Crockett, MJ, Clark, L, Tabibnia, G, Lieberman, MD, Robbins, TW. Serotonin modulates behavioral reactions to unfairness. Science 2008;320(5884):1739.Google Scholar
38. Tabibnia, G, Lieberman, MD. Fairness and cooperation are rewarding evidence from social cognitive neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 2007;1118:90–101.Google Scholar
39. Windsor, TD, Anstey, KJ, Rodgers, B. Volunteering and psychological well-being among young-old adults: How much is too much? The Gerontologist 2008;48:59–70.Google Scholar
40. De Dreu, CKW, Greer, LL, Van Kleef, GA, Shalvi, S, Handgraaf, MJJ. Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2011;108(4):1262–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41. Marsh, AA, Crowe, SL, Yu, HH, Gorodetsky, EK, Goldman, D, Blair, RJR. Serotonin transporter genotype (5-HTTLPR) predicts utilitarian moral judgments. PLoS ONE 2011;6(10):1–5.Google Scholar
42. Crockett, MJ, Clark, L, Hauser, MD, Robbins, TW. Serotonin selectively influences moral judgment and behavior through effects on harm aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2010;107(40):17433–8.Google Scholar
43. Shook, J. Neuroethics and the possible types of moral enhancement. AJOB Neuroscience 2012;3(4):3–14.Google Scholar
44. Haworth, CMA, Wright, MJ, Martin, NW, Martin, NG, Boomsma, DI, Bartels, M, et al. A twin study of the genetics of high cognitive ability selected from 11,000 twin pairs in six studies from four countries. Behavior Genetics 2009;39:359–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
45. Jang, KL, Livesly, WJ, Vemon, PA. Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: A twin study. Journal of Personality 1996;64(3):577–92.Google Scholar
46. McCrae, RR. Creativity, divergent thinking, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1987;52(6):1258–65.Google Scholar
47. Furnham, A, Chamorro-Premuzic, T. Personality, intelligence, and general knowledge. Learning and Individual Differences 2006;16:79–90.Google Scholar
48. Mussell, P. Epistemic curiosity and related constructs: Lacking evidence of discriminant validity. Personality and Individual Differences 2010;49(5):506–10.Google Scholar
49. Furnham, A, Swami, V, Arteche, A, Chamorro-Premuzic, T. Cognitive ability, learning approaches and personality correlates of general knowledge. Educational Psychology 2008;28(4):427–37.Google Scholar
50. DeYoung, CG, Peterson, JB, Higgins, DM. Sources of openness/intellect: Cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. Journal of Personality 2005;73(4):825–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51. Akbik, FV, Bhagat, SM, Patel, PR, Cafferty, WB, Strittmatter, SM. Anatomical plasticity of adult brain is titrated by Nogo Receptor 1. Neuron 2013;77(5):859–66.Google Scholar
52. Heishman, SJ, Kleykamp, BA, Singleton, EG. Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance. Psychopharmacology 2010;210(4):453–69.Google Scholar
53. Bagot, KS, Kaminer, Y. Efficacy of stimulants for cognitive enhancement in non-attention deficit hyperactivity disorder youth: A systematic review. Addiction 2014;109(4):547–57.Google Scholar
54. Damasio, A. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Penguin Books; 2005.Google Scholar
55. Michael, K, Young, L, Adolphs, R, Tranel, D, Cushman, F, Hauser, M, et al. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature 2007;446(7138):908–11.Google Scholar
56. Urban, KR, Gao, W-J. Performance enhancement at the cost of potential brain plasticity: Neural ramifications of nootropic drugs in the healthy developing brain. Frontiers in Systems of Neuroscience 2014; available at http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00038 (last accessed 15 May 2014).Google Scholar
57. Pearce, D. The Hedonistic Imperative; 1995; available at http://www.hedweb.com/hedethic (last accessed 15 May 2014).Google Scholar
58. Oishi, S, Diener, E, Lucas, RE. The optimal level of well-being: Can we be too happy? Perspectives on Psychological Science 2007;2:346–60.Google Scholar
59. Inglehart, R, Foa, R, Peterson, C, Welzel, C. Development, freedom, and rising happiness: A global perspective (1981−2007). Perspectives on Psychological Science 2008;3(4):264–85.Google Scholar
60. Goudie R, Mukherjee S, De Neve JE, Oswald AJ, Wu S. Happiness as a driver of risk-avoiding behavior: A conceptual framework with an application to seatbelt wearing and automobile accidents. Paper no. CEP 1126. Centre for Economic Performance, London School of Economics; 2012.
61. James, HS. Is the just man a happy man? An empirical study of the relationship between ethics and subjective well-being. Kyklos 2011;64(2):193–212.Google Scholar
62. See note 58, Oishi et al. 2007.
63. Walker, M. Happy-People-Pills for All. West Sussex: John Willey & Sons; 2013.Google Scholar
64. De Neve, JE, Christakis, NA, Fowler, JH, Frey, BS. Genes, economics and happiness. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics 2012:5(4):193–211.Google Scholar
65. Rietveld, CA, Cesarini, D, Benjamin, DJ, Koellinger, PD, De Neve, J-E, Tiemeier, H, et al. Molecular genetics and subjective well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2013;110(24):9692–7.Google Scholar
66. Krebs, TS, Johansen, P-Ø. Psychedelics and mental health: A population study. PLOS One 2013;8(8):1–9.Google Scholar
67. Kraehenmann, R, Preller, KH, Scheidegger, M, Pokorny, T, Bosch, OG, Seifritz, E, et al. Psilocybin-induced decrease in amygdala reactivity correlates with enhanced positive mood in healthy volunteers. Biological Psychiatry; forthcoming; available at http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006322314002753 (last accessed 15 May 2014).Google Scholar
68. See note 8, Fröding 2011, at 232.