We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (eds)., 1992. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York (NY): Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, G. & Cardew, G. (eds.), 1997. Characterizing Human Psychological Adaptations. (Ciba Foundation Symposia.) New York (NY): John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Mithen, S., 1994. Technology and society during the Middle Pleistocene: hominid group size, social learning and industrial variability. Cambridge Archaeological Journal4(1), 3–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S., 1996. The Prehistory of Mind. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Noble, W. & Davidson, I., 1996. Human Evolution, Language, and Mind: a Psychological and Archaeological Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S., 1997. Language as a psychological adaptation, in Bock, & Cardew, (eds.), 162–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, T., Tierson, F. & Palmer, C., 1996. Evolution of sex differences in spatial cognition. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology39, 11–42.3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle Scholar