Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:52:50.450Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Politics of Intersecting Crises: The Effect of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Climate Policy Preferences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

Parrish Bergquist*
Affiliation:
McCourt School of Public Policy, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA
Gabriel De Roche
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of California San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA
Erick Lachapelle
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
Matto Mildenberger
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of California Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
Kathryn Harrison
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Few contemporary crises have reshaped public policy as dramatically as the COVID-19 pandemic. In its shadow, policymakers have debated whether other pressing crises—including climate change—should be integrated into COVID-19 policy responses. Public support for such an approach is unclear: the COVID-19 crisis might eclipse public concern for other policy problems, or complementarities between COVID-19 and other issues could boost support for broad government interventions. In this research note, we use a conjoint experiment, panel study, and framing experiment to assess the substitutability or complementarity of COVID-19 and climate change among US and Canadian publics. We find no evidence that the COVID-19 crisis crowds out public concern about the climate crisis. Instead, we find that the publics in both countries prefer that their governments integrate climate action into COVID-19 responses. We also find evidence that analogizing climate change with COVID-19 may increase concern about climate change.

Type
Letter
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

In the shadow of the COVID-19 pandemic, policymakers have debated whether other pressing crises—including climate change—should be integrated into their COVID-19 responses. Is it politically feasible to integrate climate policies into pandemic recovery plans? Conventional wisdom suggests that the public may struggle to prioritize multiple crises simultaneously, particularly when they unfold across different timescales. However, public experience with one crisis could also increase public comfort with the type of aggressive policy interventions necessary to manage other crises, especially where such interventions are complementary. The contemporary moment thus raises pressing questions about how policy preferences and issue prioritization shift in the face of competing policy challenges. Have the health and economic emergencies triggered by the pandemic displaced public concern about climate change? Or, has public experience with one crisis—the COVID-19 pandemic—increased support for climate action?

In this article, we draw from four new surveys of the Canadian and US publics to explore linkages (or lack thereof) in the public mind between COVID-19 and climate change. First, we use a conjoint experiment to examine whether the public supports integrating climate action into COVID-19 recovery packages. Next, we use a panel study to examine whether the pandemic has changed the extent to which the public believes the government should prioritize climate change. We then use a framing experiment to explore whether communication about COVID-19 can shape the way the public thinks about climate change. Overall, we find that the COVID-19 crisis has not crowded out public support for climate action, but, instead, created political opportunities for integrated policy responses that include climate change mitigation in a COVID-19-related economic stimulus package.

While we assess potential linkages between climate change and COVID-19, the study's implications extend beyond this specific case. COVID-19 has dominated the news media and the zeitgeist for over two years. This intense media focus might lead the public to prefer a single-minded policy focus for COVID-19 (as distinct from other issues). Conversely, climate change might be easily displaced in public consciousness, as many view it as a temporally and geographically distant phenomenon. In this way, the COVID-19–climate change intersection can be viewed as a tough test of whether intersecting crises crowd each other out in the public mind.

The Politics of Intersecting Crises

To date, public opinion scholars have largely studied COVID-19 and climate change as independent issues. Political analyses of COVID-19 have focused on support for pandemic-related policies (Amat et al. Reference Amat2020; Lachapelle et al. Reference Lachapelle2021) and/or the impact of the pandemic on support for incumbent governments (Devine et al. Reference Devine2020; Esaiasson et al. Reference Esaiasson2020; Johansson, Hopmann, and Shehata Reference Johansson, Hopmann and Shehata2021; Leininger and Schaub Reference Leininger and Schaub2020; Schraff Reference Schraff2020; Yam et al. Reference Yam2020). Several studies have also examined the extent of partisan polarization about COVID-19. Political polarization was initially low in the United States (Gadarian, Goodman, and Pepinsky Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021; Myers Reference Myers2021) and in Canada (Merkley et al. Reference Merkley2020) but increased in the United States as the pandemic wore on (Allcott et al. Reference Allcott2020; Grossman et al. Reference Grossman2020). By contrast, in an extensive literature assessing climate change opinion, scholars have focused on how such drivers as elite cues (Guntermann and Lachapelle Reference Guntermann and Lachapelle2020; Lee et al. Reference Lee2021; Merkley and Stecula Reference Merkley and Stecula2020), framing (Aklin and Urpelainen Reference Aklin and Urpelainen2013; Bernauer and McGrath Reference Bernauer and McGrath2016; Feldman and Hart Reference Feldman and Hart2018; Gifford and Comeau Reference Gifford and Comeau2011; Nisbet Reference Nisbet2009; Spence and Pidgeon Reference Spence and Pidgeon2010), information about economic costs and benefits (Ansolabehere and Konisky Reference Ansolabehere and Konisky2014; Stokes and Warshaw Reference Stokes and Warshaw2017), personal experience with a changing climate (Bechtel and Mannino Reference Bechtel and Mannino2021; Bergquist and Warshaw Reference Bergquist and Warshaw2019; Egan and Mullin Reference Egan and Mullin2012; Howe et al. Reference Howe2019; Konisky, Hughes, and Kaylor Reference Konisky, Hughes and Kaylor2016; Marlon et al. Reference Marlon2021), economic downturns (Bakaki and Bernauer Reference Bakaki and Bernauer2018; Elliott, Seldon, and Regens Reference Elliott, Seldon and Regens1997; Inglehart Reference Inglehart1977; Mildenberger and Leiserowitz Reference Mildenberger and Leiserowitz2017), and social norms (Bechtel, Genovese, and Scheve Reference Bechtel, Genovese and Scheve2019; Mildenberger and Tingley Reference Mildenberger and Tingley2019) influence beliefs about climate change and support for policies to address it. Scholars have also mapped the spatial distribution of climate beliefs in both the United States (Howe et al. Reference Howe2015; Bergquist and Warshaw Reference Bergquist and Warshaw2019) and Canada (Mildenberger et al. Reference Mildenberger2016).

These literatures leave open the question of how concern about one crisis—climate change—is impacted by other types of crisis experiences, but the climate opinion literature does provide some fruitful directions for theorizing on this question. Economic downturns represent one class of experiences that has been theorized to cause a downturn in public environmentalism (Elliott, Seldon, and Regens Reference Elliott, Seldon and Regens1997; Inglehart Reference Inglehart1977). Recent studies have revisited this link with mixed success in identifying a linkage between economic distress and climate action (Bakaki and Bernauer Reference Bakaki and Bernauer2018; Mildenberger and Leiserowitz Reference Mildenberger and Leiserowitz2017). We extend this work by assessing the relationship between climate concern and a different type of massive public upheaval—COVID-19. Relatedly, scholars have explored how the framing of climate change and climate policy influences public attitudes (Aklin and Urpelainen Reference Aklin and Urpelainen2013; Bernauer and McGrath Reference Bernauer and McGrath2016; Feldman and Hart Reference Feldman and Hart2018; Gifford and Comeau Reference Gifford and Comeau2011; Nisbet Reference Nisbet2009; Spence and Pidgeon Reference Spence and Pidgeon2010). For the most part, this work has focused on emphasizing different features of climate change itself. However, climate change is not occurring in a vacuum, and framing it as similar to or different from other pressing political issues might provide leverage for building climate policy support.

More generally, theoretical perspectives lead to three distinct expectations about how the public might respond to simultaneous crises. First, new crises may compete within the public's “finite pool of worry” (Weber Reference Weber2006) and crowd out concerns about other policy challenges. In this view, simultaneous crises act as substitutes in the public mind. Secondly, a new crisis may open a window of opportunity for addressing pre-existing issues (Kingdon and Thurber Reference Kingdon and Thurber1984). This might occur if the government response to a new crisis normalizes a particular type or scale of policy response. For example, the scale of COVID-19 stimulus spending could recalibrate expectations about appropriate government interventions in the economy. In this view, simultaneous crises could complement each other in the public mind. Thirdly, in the model implied by standard issue-specific studies of public opinion like those cited earlier, public attitudes about simultaneous policy challenges could remain independent. In this view, the emergence of new crises does not affect public opinion about longstanding issues.

To our knowledge, scholars have not tested these competing theories of public policy linkage. One study shows that climate policy is more popular when economic and social policies are integrated into policy packages—but it does not examine COVID-19 specifically (Bergquist, Mildenberger, and Stokes Reference Bergquist, Mildenberger and Stokes2020). Separately, two recent studies have found that the public is more willing to accept some policies when they are proposed as measures to address COVID-19, as opposed to responses to climate change (Amat et al. Reference Amat2020; Kallbekken and Sælen Reference Kallbekken and Sælen2021). These findings suggest that the public views different policy instruments as more appropriate in some contexts than others, but they do not tell us whether exposure to intersecting crises influences how the public thinks about the crises or policies to address them. In practice, some governments have linked COVID-19 and climate change by incorporating a green stimulus into their COVID-19 relief plans, but we lack an understanding of the political benefits or drawbacks to this strategy.

Methods

Our analysis leverages four datasets: two collected during the early months of the COVID-19 pandemic; and two from mid-2021, over a year after pandemic-related restrictions began in the United States and Canada. First, we leverage data from the Canadian Climate Opinion Panel (CCOP). The CCOP was a custom five-wave public opinion panel survey administered online in five Canadian provinces between February 2019 and May 2020 to a sample drawn from the Leger 360 platform (Mildenberger et al. Reference Mildenberger2022). Complete information on the CCOP is provided as Section 1.1 in the Online Supplementary Material. Secondly, we fielded a national survey of the US public simultaneously with the fifth wave of the CCOP in May 2020. US respondents were recruited by Qualtrics between May 15 and May 20 (n = 1,049), and quota-sampled by race, age, and gender. Thirdly, in April 2021, we fielded a second national survey of Americans, this time using the Lucid Theorem service (n = 1,695). Again, respondents were quota-sampled by race, age, and gender. Fourthly, in June 2021, we fielded a national survey of Canadians, also using Lucid. Canadian respondents were quota-sampled on language, age, and gender (n = 1,058). We then merged local COVID-19 prevalence data into all four datasets, using data from provincial health authorities in Canada and a non-partisan repository of health data in the United States. We provide full details about the surveys and COVID-19 prevalence data in Section 1.2 in the Online Supplementary Material.

We use these surveys to explore the relationship between climate change and COVID-19 in three ways. Our primary focus explores whether public support for climate policy increases or decreases support for government responses to the COVID-19 pandemic. We explore this topic using a conjoint survey experiment embedded in the 2021 Canadian and US surveys. Conjoint experiments capture the dynamics of multidimensional decision-making and show how different choice dimensions vary in relative importance (Bansak, Hainmueller, and Hangartner Reference Bansak, Hainmueller and Hangartner2016; Hainmueller, Hopkins, and Yamamoto Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014a). Respondents are asked to choose between two choice bundles that contain randomly varying combinations of policy elements. The researcher can then estimate average marginal component-specific effects (AMCEs) for each policy element (Hainmueller, Hopkins, and Yamamoto Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014a). The AMCE shows how much a given policy element increases or decreases public support for the policy package, holding all other elements constant. We estimate the AMCE for each policy element by regressing a binary indicator for whether a policy bundle was preferred on treatment variables indicating the presence or absence of each element. Our results can be interpreted as the marginal change in support associated with the inclusion of each policy level, holding all other elements constant (including, crucially, the cost of the package).Footnote 1

As explained more thoroughly in Section 2.1 in the Online Supplementary Material, we designed the conjoint experiment to reflect contemporaneous policy discourses in the US Congress and the Canadian Parliament, and to speak to scholarly debates about climate policy, energy justice, and social policy. Respondents were asked to evaluate three randomly generated pairs of policy packages, which varied with respect to five dimensions (shown in Figure 1): climate action, infrastructure, individual support, business support, and costs. The elements included in each policy dimension are included in Appendix Table 2 in the Online Supplementary Material. It should be recalled that our primary focus is on support for including climate policies in COVID-19 economic recovery packages. We thus focus particular attention on the elements contained within the climate action and infrastructure dimensions.Footnote 2

Fig. 1. Policy attributes included in our conjoint experiment.

Note: The figure shows the policy dimensions included in our conjoint experiment and was presented to respondents in the explanation preceding the comparison task.

This conjoint experiment will reveal whether the US and Canadian publics prefer that COVID-19 response packages integrate policy to address climate change, but it does not tell us whether or how the pandemic has changed views of climate change and climate policy. We assess this question in two ways. First, we evaluate the effect of COVID-19 incidence on climate concern by evaluating within-subject changes in Canadian climate policy preferences between the December 2019 and May 2020 waves of the CCOP. Details on question wording are provided in Section 2.2 in the Online Supplementary Material. We analyze these panel data using a (two-way, fixed-effects) ordinary least squares (OLS) model of the form:

(1)$${\rm Concern}_{it} = \gamma _i + \omega _t + \alpha \,{\rm COVID}\hbox{-}{\rm 19\ incidenc}{\rm e}_{it} + \eta _{it}$$

where: Concernit is one of our two climate attitude measures (climate concern and support for carbon pricing) i in survey wave t; γi are individual respondent fixed effects; ωt are survey-wave fixed effects; and ηit is the error term. The key parameter of interest is α, the coefficient on COVID-19incidenceit, which gives the best linear approximation of the average treatment effect (ATE), subject to the conditional ignorability assumption holding.

Secondly, in all four surveys, we tested whether framing the climate crisis as similar to the COVID-19 pandemic might increase public climate concern or prioritization of climate change. We experimentally examined the effect of a vignette that described COVID-19 and climate change as problems that grow exponentially and are best mitigated through early action.Footnote 3 We randomly assigned survey respondents to read one of three vignettes that either discussed the COVID-19 pandemic alone or highlighted similarities between COVID-19 and the climate crisis. We provide further details about these experiments in Section 2.3 in the Online Supplementary Material. We identify the ATE of the vignettes using OLS. We also conduct a mini meta-analysis using a random-effects model to identify an overall effect size across the four surveys (Baldwin and Lammers Reference Baldwin and Lammers2016; Goh, Hall, and Rosenthal Reference Goh, Hall and Rosenthal2016).

Results

Our conjoint-experimental results show that Americans and Canadians support an approach to COVID-19 relief that incorporates climate policy (see Figure 2). All of the climate action measures boost support for a COVID-19 relief package. Conditioning business support on pollution reductions boosts support by the greatest amount in both countries. Including green infrastructure spending also increases support for a COVID-19 relief package. Further, in both countries, support for clean energy and clean transportation boosts support more than fossil fuel infrastructure investments. In Canada, clean energy investments boost support more than roads, bridges, and tunnels, whereas we cannot distinguish between the increased support associated with clean energy and traditional infrastructure in the United States.

Fig. 2. How social, economic, and climate programs shape support for bundled COVID-19 response packages in Canada and the United States.

Notes: The figure shows average effects of each policy element on support for the COVID-19 policy bundle in the United States (left panel) and Canada (right panel). Point estimates are AMCEs: the change in probability of selecting a COVID-19 package, ceteris paribus, if the package includes each element. AMCEs are estimated using the R package cjoint (Hainmueller, Hopkins, and Yamamoto Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014a; Hainmueller, Hopkins, and Yamamoto Reference Hainmueller, Hopkins and Yamamoto2014b). Standard errors are clustered at the respondent level. Bars reflect 95 per cent confidence intervals. Hash marks show 83 per cent confidence intervals to aid visual inspection of significant differences in effect sizes (Bolsen and Thornton Reference Bolsen and Thornton2014; Payton, Greenstone, and Schenker Reference Payton, Greenstone and Schenker2003). Each element is compared against a base category for each policy dimension, denoted by a dot on the 0 intercept.

Climate change is conditioned by a high degree of partisan polarization in both the United States and Canada (Lachapelle, Borick, and Rabe Reference Lachapelle, Borick and Rabe2012; Mildenberger et al. Reference Mildenberger2017). However, as detailed in Section 2.1.1 in the Online Supplementary Material, these results are stable when disaggregated by partisan subgroups in the United States and ideological subgroups in Canada.

We also examine whether these effects correspond to meaningful shifts in public support for a COVID-19 relief package. As explained in detail in Section 2.1.2 in the Online Supplementary Material and following prior work (for example, Bechtel and Scheve Reference Bechtel and Scheve2013), we address this question by comparing support for packages that include the most popular climate policy elements with baseline packages that do not. Even with large differences in costs, we find that including climate policy elements boosts support for a COVID-19 relief package by 12 and 14 points on a 0–100-point support scale in the United States and Canada, respectively. These conjoint-experimental results suggest that the public prefers that governments take simultaneous action to manage intersecting crises, though these results do not shed light on potential shifts in issue prioritization. For instance, the public might prefer addressing climate change in COVID-19 recovery packages even as the intensity with which they want climate change prioritized declines. Our panel study estimating the effect of local COVID-19 prevalence on Canadians' climate concern suggests otherwise. The results reported in Table 1 show no significant effect of local COVID-19 prevalence on the importance Canadians assign to the issue of climate change (Column 1) or support for carbon pricing (Column 2). Results are similar for a range of other issues reported in Section 2.2 in the Online Supplementary Material. We thus find no evidence that COVID-19 crowded climate change out of a “finite pool of worry” for Canadians.Footnote 4

Table 1. Results of two-way fixed-effects models, showing no statistically significant effect of COVID-19 prevalence at the local level on the importance Canadians assign to climate change (Column 1) and support for carbon pricing (column2)

Notes: Local COVID-19 prevalence is measured as the natural log of the percentage of the local population that had contracted COVID-19 (cumulative through May 18, 2020). Issue importance is measured on a five-point scale (1–5) but has been rescaled to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. Support for carbon pricing is measured on a five-point scale (1–5) but has also been rescaled to have mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. The table reports standardized coefficients, such that the effects should be interpreted in standard-deviation units. Robust standard errors clustered at the respondent level.

These results still leave open the question of how the public might respond to media or political communications that link the two crises. Thus, we also explore whether concern about climate change might be enhanced when respondents receive messages about parallels between the COVID-19 pandemic and the climate crisis. We find suggestive evidence that this is the case. While results across the four vignette experiments were inconsistently significant (see Figure 7 in Section 2.3 in the Online Supplementary Material), a mini meta-analysis of the four studies finds a small, positive, and significant overall effect of analogizing climate change to the “exponential growth” of COVID-19 on respondents' level of worry about climate change. Using a random-effects model, we estimate an overall standardized mean difference between the control groups (COVID-19 information only) and the treatment groups (COVID-19 analogized to climate change) of 0.085 (standard error: 0.035, p < 0.017). Results of the mini meta-analysis are presented in Figure 3. These results mirror the results from the panel analysis. COVID-19 does not crowd climate change out of the public consciousness, but neither does its structural similarity to climate change dramatically increase public climate concern.

Fig. 3. Mini meta-analysis of four survey experiments framing climate change as analogous to COVID-19.

Notes: The figure shows the results of a mini meta-analysis, presenting standardized mean differences between groups that read a vignette analogizing the “exponential growth” of COVID-19 to that of climate change. The experiment was conducted four times in two countries: the United States (May 2020 and February 2021) and Canada (May 2020 and June 2021). The dependent variable is worry about climate change, measured on a four-point scale. The meta-analysis was conducted using a random-effects model.

Conclusion

In this research note, we have assessed public support for integrated policy approaches that address COVID-19 and climate change simultaneously. We have also explored the attitudinal bases for the public's policy preferences by asking whether exposure to or communication about the new COVID-19 crisis changes how the public thinks about climate change. We find no evidence that the COVID-19 pandemic reduced support for climate action by crowding climate change out of a finite pool of worry. COVID-19 recovery packages that included climate action were more popular than those that did not. Moreover, we do not find evidence that exposure to COVID-19 reduced the importance of climate change in the public mind or eroded support for climate policy. In addition, we find evidence that rhetorically linking the two crises might increase public concern about climate change. The COVID-19 crisis has not dampened public support for climate action; instead, it may create opportunities for integrated policy solutions. While our study focuses on the direct relationship between public experience with COVID-19 and policy-relevant views, political elites can also mediate this relationship. Future research could investigate how the public responds to political elites' efforts to link issues or keep them distinct.

Supplementary Material

Online appendices are available at: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123422000266

Data Availability Statement

Replication data for this article can be found at the British Journal of Political Science Dataverse, available at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/LULYEB

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful for feedback from Leah Stokes, Chad Hazlett, participants at the 2020 American Political Science Association annual conference, participants at the 2020 Global Conference on Environmental Taxation, and three anonymous reviewers.

Financial Support

This project received funding support from the Smart Prosperity Institute at the University of Ottawa, the Institute for Social, Behavioral, and Economic Research at UCSB, and the CIGI-INET Program on New Economic Theory, Practice, and Governance.

Competing Interests

The authors have no competing interests to disclose.

Footnotes

1 We note here that since respondents were forced to select a package for each choice task, estimated AMCEs range from −0.5 (a 0.5-point decrease in the probability of supporting a package) to 0.5 (a 0.5-point increase in the probability of supporting a package).

2 In Section 2.1.3 in the Online Supplementary Material, we report the results from a follow-up study we conducted to confirm our results don't change if we also include COVID-19 mitigation policies in the conjoint experiment. We did not include these policies in our primary study because responsibility for managing the severity of COVID-19 was left primarily to the states and provinces (not the federal government), and because these debates happened independently from economic recovery debates.

3 We acknowledge that while the structure of the underlying problems causing the pandemic and climate change are similar, the timing of policy impacts is quite different. The impact of policy responses to COVID-19 are observable in days, weeks, or months, whereas policies to mitigate climate change will take years or decades to realize their full impact. If respondents make this distinction, then their understanding of the problems as similar might not lead them to support aggressive climate policy. This could be one explanation for the null result from this experiment.

4 In Appendix Figures 4 and 5 in the Online Supplementary Material, we show the results from a simulation examining the minimum detectable effect of our panel study. Our samples are large enough to enable us to detect an effect of about 0.2 standard deviations on the issue-priority variable and 0.25 standard deviations on support for carbon pricing. Thus, while we acknowledge that our study is underpowered to detect very small effects, our design would enable us to detect a substantively meaningful effect.

References

Aklin, M and Urpelainen, J (2013) Debating clean energy: frames, counter frames, and audiences. Global Environmental Change 23(5), 12251232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allcott, H et al. (2020) Polarization and public health: partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. Journal of Public Economics 191, 104254.10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amat, F et al. (2020) Pandemics Meet Democracy. Experimental Evidence from the COVID-19 Crisis in Spain. Working paper available at https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/dkusw/.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ansolabehere, S and Konisky, DM (2014) Cheap and Clean: How Americans Think about Energy in the Age of Global Warming. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakaki, Z and Bernauer, T (2018) Do economic conditions affect public support for environmental policy? Journal of Cleaner Production 195, 6678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, M and Lammers, J (2016) Past-focused environmental comparisons promote proenvironmental outcomes for conservatives. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113(52), 1495314957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bansak, K, Hainmueller, J, and Hangartner, D (2016) How economic, humanitarian, and religious concerns shape European attitudes toward asylum seekers. Science 354(6309), 217222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechtel, MM and Mannino, M (2021) Ready when the big one comes? Natural disasters and mass support for preparedness investment. Political Behavior (2021), 126.Google ScholarPubMed
Bechtel, MM and Scheve, KF (2013) Mass support for global climate agreements depends on institutional design. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110(34), 1376313768.10.1073/pnas.1306374110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechtel, MM, Genovese, F, and Scheve, KF (2019) Interests, norms and support for the provision of global public goods: the case of climate co-operation. British Journal of Political Science 49(4), 13331355.10.1017/S0007123417000205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergquist, P et al. (2022) Replication data for: The Politics of Intersecting Crises: The Effect of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Climate Policy Preferences. Available from https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/LULYEB, Harvard Dataverse, V1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergquist, P and Warshaw, C (2019) Does global warming increase public concern about climate change? The Journal of Politics 81(2), 686691.10.1086/701766CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergquist, P, Mildenberger, M, and Stokes, L (2020) Combining climate, economic, and social policy builds public support for climate action in the US. Environmental Research Letters 15(5), 054019.10.1088/1748-9326/ab81c1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernauer, T and McGrath, LF (2016) Simple reframing unlikely to boost public support for climate policy. Nature Climate Change 6(7), 680683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolsen, T and Thornton, J (2014) Overlapping confidence intervals and null hypothesis testing. The Experimental Political Scientist 4(1), 1216.Google Scholar
Devine, D et al. (2020) Trust and the coronavirus pandemic: what are the consequences of and for trust? An early review of the literature. Political Studies Review 19(2), 274–285.Google Scholar
Egan, PJ and Mullin, M (2012) Turning personal experience into political attitudes: the effect of local weather on Americans’ perceptions about global warming. The Journal of Politics 74(3), 796809.10.1017/S0022381612000448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliott, E, Seldon, B, and Regens, J (1997) Political and economic determinants of individuals’ support for environmental spending. Journal of Environmental Management 51(1), 1527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esaiasson, P et al. (2020) How the coronavirus crisis affects citizen trust in institutions and in unknown others: evidence from “the Swedish experiment.” European Journal of Political Research 60(3), 748–760.Google Scholar
Feldman, L and Hart, PS (2018) Climate change as a polarizing cue: framing effects on public support for low-carbon energy policies. Global Environmental Change 51, 5466.10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2018.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadarian, SK, Goodman, SW, and Pepinsky, T (2021) Partisan endorsement experiments do not affect mass opinion on COVID-19. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 31(sup1), 122131.10.1080/17457289.2021.1924727CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gifford, R and Comeau, LA (2011) Message framing influences perceived climate change competence, engagement, and behavioral intentions. Global Environmental Change 21(4), 13011307.10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2011.06.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goh, JX, Hall, JA, and Rosenthal, R (2016) Mini meta-analysis of your own studies: some arguments on why and a primer on how. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 10(10), 535549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, G et al. (2020) Political partisanship influences behavioral responses to governors’ recommendations for COVID-19 prevention in the United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117(39), 2414424153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guntermann, E and Lachapelle, E (2020) Canadian parties matter more than you think: party and leader ratings moderate party cue effects. Canadian Journal of Political Science/Revue canadienne de science politique 53(4), 839852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J, Hopkins, D, and Yamamoto, T (2014a) Causal inference in conjoint analysis: understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference experiments. Political Analysis 22(1), 130.10.1093/pan/mpt024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J, Hopkins, D, and Yamamoto, T (2014b) Cjoint: Causal Inference in Conjoint Analysis: Understanding Multi-Dimensional Choices via Stated Preference Experiments. 1. R Package Version 2.1.0. Available from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=cjointCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, PD et al. (2015) Geographic variation in opinions on climate change at state and local scales in the USA. Nature Climate Change 5(6), 596603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howe, PD et al. (2019) How will climate change shape climate opinion? Environmental Research Letters 14(11), 113001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R (1977) The Silent Revolution: Changing Values and Political Styles among Western Publics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Johansson, B, Hopmann, DN, and Shehata, A (2021) When the rally-around-the-flag effect disappears, or: when the COVID-19 pandemic becomes “normalized.” Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 31(sup1), 321334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kallbekken, S and Sælen, H (2021) Public support for air travel restrictions to address COVID-19 or climate change. Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment 93, 102767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingdon, J and Thurber, J (1984) Agendas, Alternatives, and Public Policies, vol. 45. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Konisky, DM, Hughes, L, and Kaylor, CH (2016) Extreme weather events and climate change concern. Climatic Change 134(4), 533547.10.1007/s10584-015-1555-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachapelle, E, Borick, CP, and Rabe, B (2012) Public attitudes toward climate science and climate policy in federal systems: Canada and the United States compared. Review of Policy Research 29(3), 334.10.1111/j.1541-1338.2012.00563.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lachapelle, E et al. (2021) Citizens’ willingness to support new taxes for COVID-19 measures and the role of trust. Politics Policy 49(3), 534565.10.1111/polp.12404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, N et al. (2021) More accurate, but no less polarized: comparing the factual beliefs of government officials and the public. British Journal of Political Science 51(3), 13151322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leininger, A and Schaub, M (2020) Voting at the Dawn of a Global Pandemic. Working paper available at https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/a32r7/.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlon, JR et al. (2021) Hot dry days increase perceived experience with global warming. Global Environmental Change 68, 102247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCright, AM and Dunlap, RE (2011) The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public's views of global warming, 2001–2010. The Sociological Quarterly 52(2), 155194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merkley, E and Stecula, DA (2020) Party cues in the news: democratic elites, republican backlash, and the dynamics of climate skepticism. British Journal of Political Science 51(4), 118.Google Scholar
Merkley, E et al. (2020) A rare moment of cross-partisan consensus: elite and public response to the COVID-19 pandemic in Canada. Canadian Journal of Political Science/Revue canadienne de science politique 53(2), 311318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mildenberger, M and Leiserowitz, A (2017) Public opinion on climate change: is there an economy–environment tradeoff? Environmental Politics 26(5), 801824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mildenberger, M and Tingley, D (2019) Beliefs about climate beliefs: the importance of second-order opinions for climate politics. British Journal of Political Science 49(4), 12791307.10.1017/S0007123417000321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mildenberger, M et al. (2016) The distribution of climate change public opinion in Canada. PLoS ONE 11(8), e0159774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mildenberger, M et al. (2017) The spatial distribution of Republican and Democratic climate opinions at state and local scales. Climatic Change 145(3), 539548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mildenberger, M et al. (2022) Limited impacts of carbon tax rebate programmes on public support for carbon pricing. Nature Climate Change 12, 17.Google Scholar
Myers, D (2021) No effect of partisan framing on opinions about the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 31(sup1), 132144.10.1080/17457289.2021.1924747CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbet, MC (2009) Communicating climate change: why frames matter for public engagement. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development 51(2), 1223.Google Scholar
Payton, M, Greenstone, M, and Schenker, N (2003) Overlapping confidence intervals or standard error intervals: what do they mean in terms of statistical significance? Journal of Insect Science 3(34), 1–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schraff, D (2020) Political trust during the COVID-19 pandemic: rally around the flag or lockdown effects? European Journal of Political Research 60(4), 1007–1017.Google ScholarPubMed
Spence, A and Pidgeon, N (2010) Framing and communicating climate change: the effects of distance and outcome frame manipulations. Global Environmental Change 20(4), 656667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokes, LC and Warshaw, C (2017) Renewable energy policy design and framing influence public support in the United States. Nature Energy 2(8), 16.10.1038/nenergy.2017.107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber, E (2006) Experience-based and description-based perceptions of long-term risk: why global warming does not scare us (yet). Climatic Change 77(1–2), 103120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yam, KC et al. (2020) The rise of COVID-19 cases is associated with support for world leaders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 117(41), 2542925433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Policy attributes included in our conjoint experiment.Note: The figure shows the policy dimensions included in our conjoint experiment and was presented to respondents in the explanation preceding the comparison task.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. How social, economic, and climate programs shape support for bundled COVID-19 response packages in Canada and the United States.Notes: The figure shows average effects of each policy element on support for the COVID-19 policy bundle in the United States (left panel) and Canada (right panel). Point estimates are AMCEs: the change in probability of selecting a COVID-19 package, ceteris paribus, if the package includes each element. AMCEs are estimated using the R package cjoint (Hainmueller, Hopkins, and Yamamoto 2014a; Hainmueller, Hopkins, and Yamamoto 2014b). Standard errors are clustered at the respondent level. Bars reflect 95 per cent confidence intervals. Hash marks show 83 per cent confidence intervals to aid visual inspection of significant differences in effect sizes (Bolsen and Thornton 2014; Payton, Greenstone, and Schenker 2003). Each element is compared against a base category for each policy dimension, denoted by a dot on the 0 intercept.

Figure 2

Table 1. Results of two-way fixed-effects models, showing no statistically significant effect of COVID-19 prevalence at the local level on the importance Canadians assign to climate change (Column 1) and support for carbon pricing (column2)

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Mini meta-analysis of four survey experiments framing climate change as analogous to COVID-19.Notes: The figure shows the results of a mini meta-analysis, presenting standardized mean differences between groups that read a vignette analogizing the “exponential growth” of COVID-19 to that of climate change. The experiment was conducted four times in two countries: the United States (May 2020 and February 2021) and Canada (May 2020 and June 2021). The dependent variable is worry about climate change, measured on a four-point scale. The meta-analysis was conducted using a random-effects model.

Supplementary material: Link

Bergquist et al. Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: PDF

Bergquist et al. supplementary material

Bergquist et al. supplementary material

Download Bergquist et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 5.7 MB