Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T08:53:17.748Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Misperceptions about Immigration: Reviewing Their Nature, Motivations and Determinants

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 May 2022

Philipp Lutz*
Affiliation:
University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
Marco Bitschnau
Affiliation:
University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland University of California, Berkeley, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Across Western democracies, immigration has become one of the most polarizing and salient issues, with public discourses and individual attitudes often characterized by misperceptions. This condition undermines people's ability to develop informed opinions on the matter and runs counter to the ideal of deliberative democracy. Yet, our understanding of what makes immigration so prone to misperceptions is still limited – a conundrum that this review seeks to answer in three steps. First, we take stock of the existing evidence on the nature of misperceptions about immigration. Secondly, we borrow from diverse bodies of literature to identify their motivational underpinnings and elaborate on how the protection of group identity, the defence of self-interest and security concerns can lead to distorted perceptions of immigration. Thirdly, we highlight relevant determinants of misperceptions at the level of both contextual influences and individual predispositions. We conclude that misperceptions about immigration are ubiquitous and likely to remain a key element of immigration politics.

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

The only useful approach is to discover why they can swallow absurdities on one particular subject while remaining sane on others’. (George Orwell Reference Orwell1945)

Introduction

Issues of immigration have become crucial to political contestation in Western democracies, with many people holding polarized views that are embedded in perceptions rather than reality (see, e.g., Blinder Reference Blinder2015; Cornelius and Rosenblum Reference Cornelius and Rosenblum2005; Gorodzeisky and Semyonov Reference Gorodzeisky and Semyonov2020). These perceptions are often erroneous or fallacious, ranging from overestimations of the number of immigrants living in one's country (see, e.g., Herda Reference Herda2010; Sides and Citrin Reference Sides and Citrin2007) to complex conspiracy theories about the purported replacement of native Europeans with Africans or Middle Easterners (see, e.g., Gaston and Uscinski Reference Gaston and Uscinski2018). Such misperceptions have gained an important foothold in the public sphere, pervading online culture (Ganesh Reference Ganesh2018; Graham Reference Graham2016) and right-wing populism (Bergmann Reference Bergmann2018; Castanho Silva, Vegetti and Littvay, Reference Castanho Silva, Vegetti and Littvay2017), contributing to polarization, and undermining people's ability to form reasonable and balanced opinions on the matter. It is thus hardly surprising that these misperceptions have repeatedly been identified as a key determinant of policy preferences and political behaviour (see, e.g., Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva Reference Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva2022; Rodriguez-Justicia and Theilen Reference Rodriguez-Justicia and Theilen2022; Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky Reference Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky2008; Sides and Citrin Reference Sides and Citrin2007), as well as a major obstacle to democratic deliberation and evidence-based policy making (Ruhs, Tamas and Palme Reference Ruhs, Tamas and Palme2019). More specifically, the distorted trope of an ‘immigrant invasion’ has contributed to disruptive events, such as the Brexit vote and the election of Donald Trump to the US presidency (Gavin Reference Gavin2018). Yet, we still lack a systematic account of these misperceptions, their prevalence and their underlying drivers.

This review article offers a comprehensive overview of the scholarly research on misperceptions about immigration, which is fragmented and dispersed across several disciplines and research areas: political scientists, sociologists and communication scholars focus on their determinants and consequences for society; psychologists are mostly interested in their underlying cognitive and emotional mechanisms, or in the phenomenon of prejudice; and economists examine how they deviate from the ideal of informed and enlightened agency. Taking this range of perspectives into account, our review covers three central aspects of misperceptions about immigration: their nature; the motivations to hold them; and their determinants. We first take stock of the misperceptions to be found in the literature and distinguish between three categories related to the properties, effects and governance of immigration. Then, we examine the motivational undercurrents of selective information seeking and biased information processing. Drawing on research about immigration attitudes and the theory of motivated reasoning, we identify the protection of identity, the defence of self-interest and concerns about security as issue-specific motivations for the bias against immigrants. In a third step, we discuss contextual and individual factors that have been found to determine misperceptions. We conclude by summarizing the key insights of our review and making suggestions for future research.

Varieties of Misperceptions about Immigration

People rely on mental images to make sense of the social and political realities that surround them in their everyday lives. Whenever they think of a phenomenon like immigration, they think of a mental representation of what immigration means or is supposed to mean (Blinder Reference Blinder2015). If this representation does not correspond to the empirically observable reality, we speak of it as a misperception, that is, a belief that is ‘false or contradicts the best available evidence in the public domain’ (Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler Reference Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler2017: 128). Misperceptions manifest themselves in an abundance of ways, for example, as unfounded rumours and suspicions, prejudices, conspiracy theories, and misestimations. Still, they share the epistemic trait of being neither supported by the best available evidence, nor rooted in a mere lack of information. After all, they stem from being misinformed, that is, holding an objectively false belief about the answer to a question, rather than uninformed, that is, having no answer at all (Kuklinski et al. Reference Kuklinski2000). Consequently, we may define misperceptions about immigration as evidently false beliefs about immigration.Footnote 1 While biases against immigration have a long history, survey evidence on public misperceptions has only been collected for a few years, and primarily in Europe and the United States (for a list of empirical studies, see the Online Supplementary Material). In the following, we distinguish between three distinct categories of misperceptions found in the general publicFootnote 2 to derive conclusions about their nature and prevalence.

A first category of misperceptions relates to the properties of immigration. Perhaps the most prominent is immigration innumeracy, that is, the systematic overestimation of the population share of immigrants living in a particular country, region or neighbourhood (see, e.g., Citrin and Sides Reference Citrin and Sides2008; Herda Reference Herda2010; Herda Reference Herda2019; Lundmark and Kokkonen Reference Lundmark and Kokkonen2017; Steele and Perkins Reference Steele and Perkins2019).Footnote 3 This innumeracy is common across Western democracies but has also been evidenced elsewhere, for example, in Latin American and East Asian countries (Ipsos 2015), South Africa (Gordon et al. Reference Gordon2020) and Turkey (Herda Reference Herda2015a).Footnote 4 In a similar vein, people overestimate the size of immigrant flows (Blinder and Schaffner Reference Blinder and Schaffner2020) and the number of immigrants entering their country relative to those heading for other countries (Sides and Citrin Reference Sides and Citrin2007: 486).

Misperceptions have also been found for qualitative properties of immigration, such as the demographic composition and socio-economic characteristics of immigrant populations. These include equating immigration with permanent settlement and immigrants with refugees (Blinder Reference Blinder2015), overestimating the religious, cultural or geographical distance of immigrants vis-à-vis the native population (Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva Reference Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva2022; Herda Reference Herda2015b), and erroneously thinking of them as poorer and less educated than they actually are (Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva Reference Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva2022). Furthermore, people misperceive the reasons why immigrants leave their countries of origin in the first place. They consider access to welfare benefits as the primary motivator (Dixon et al. Reference Dixon2019; Zimmermann Reference Zimmermann, Ruhs, Tamas and Palme2019) and either disregard or downplay the need for humanitarian protection (Mancini et al. Reference Mancini2020; Pedersen and Hartley Reference Pedersen and Hartley2017; Skinner and Gottfried Reference Skinner and Gottfried2017). It is thus not surprising that people overestimate the relative share of those moving for humanitarian and family reasons but underestimate the relative share of those moving for education and work (Blinder Reference Blinder2015).

A second category of misperceptions concerns the effects of immigration – its economic, cultural and security-related impact – on host societies. Regarding the economy, many view immigration as a burden and immigrants as soldiers of fortune with whom they must now share their hard-earned wealth (Caplan Reference Caplan2007; Johnston and Ballard Reference Johnston and Ballard2016; McLaren and Johnson Reference McLaren and Johnson2007). Economists have repeatedly refuted this sentiment, underscoring the welfare-enhancing effects of immigration instead (see, e.g., Dustmann and Preston Reference Dustmann and Preston2019; Johnston and Ballard Reference Johnston and Ballard2016; Kemeny and Cooke Reference Kemeny and Cooke2018). A similar gap between perceptions and evidence exists with respect to whether immigration is detrimental to wages (Bansak, Simpson and Zavodny Reference Bansak, Simpson, Zavodny, Bansak, Simpson and Zavodny2021; Scheve and Slaughter Reference Scheve and Slaughter2001) and employment (McLaren and Johnson Reference McLaren and Johnson2007): empirical economic research has found negligible, if any, effects in either case (see, e.g., Battisti et al. Reference Battisti2018; Beerli et al. Reference Beerli2021; Friedberg and Hunt Reference Friedberg and Hunt1995; Manacorda, Manning and Wadsworth Reference Manacorda, Manning and Wadsworth2012; Ottaviano and Peri Reference Ottaviano and Peri2012). These misperceptions are complemented by misattributions: immigrants are held responsible for job losses, even if the latter originate in structural disruptions, such as trade liberalization and technological change (Wu Reference Wu2021). Moreover, perceptions of the fiscal consequences of immigration (Markaki and Blinder Reference Markaki and Blinder2018; Martinsen and Rotger Reference Martinsen and Rotger2017) and the number of immigrant welfare recipients are subject to a strong negativity bias (see, e.g., Ekins and Kemp Reference Ekins and Kemp2021). For instance, despite the positive fiscal impact of immigration to the UK (Dustmann and Frattini Reference Dustmann and Frattini2014), perceptions of immigrants lowering the quality of public services are prevalent (Duffy and Frere-Smith Reference Duffy and Frere-Smith2014).

In the domain of culture, immigrants and ‘migranticized’ minorities (most notably, Muslims) are often perceived as culturally distant, accused of defying integration efforts (see, e.g., Kalkan, Layman and Uslaner Reference Kalkan, Layman and Uslaner2009; Panagopoulos Reference Panagopoulos2006) and distrusted for their alleged lack of loyalty to their host societies (see, e.g., Helbling et al. Reference Helbling2017; McLaren and Johnson Reference McLaren and Johnson2007). Lastly, the link between immigration and security deserves to be highlighted. Throughout Europe, people believe that immigration increases crime – a perception that exists independently of their country's real crime levels (Ceobanu Reference Ceobanu2011). Accordingly, people tend to overestimate the share of crimes committed by foreigners (see, e.g., Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky Reference Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky2008; for Britain, see also Stansfield and Stone Reference Stansfield and Stone2018), the share of the prison population that foreigners comprise (IPSOS 2018; Rumbaut and Ewing Reference Rumbaut and Ewing2007) and the share of immigrants involved in criminal gangs (Moore-Berg, Hameiri and Bruneau Reference Moore-Berg, Hameiri and Bruneau2022). Terrorism is also commonly associated with immigration (Skinner and Gottfried Reference Skinner and Gottfried2017), but, again, there is little evidence of a direct link between them (Helbling and Meierrieks Reference Helbling and Meierrieks2022).

A third category of misperceptions deals with issues of immigration governance, such as the belief that Western governments hide their real immigration agendas and suppress unpleasant facts. For example, a 2018 cross-national survey had a significant share of the European population (ranging from about 13 per cent in Portugal to 48 per cent in Hungary) suspecting that the truth about immigration levels is hidden by their governments (YouGov 2018). Similarly, more than half of all respondents in the UK and the United States are convinced that unvarnished information about the real costs of immigration is deliberately withheld from the public eye (Gaston and Uscinski Reference Gaston and Uscinski2018: 5). Other misperceptions in this category relate to immigration control: people overestimate the share of immigrants that enter and stay in their countries without holding a legal permit (Blinder and Jeannet Reference Blinder and Jeannet2017; Ekins and Kemp Reference Ekins and Kemp2021; Eurobarometer 2018), as well as the share of immigrant children who have been transnationally trafficked (Moore-Berg, Hameiri and Bruneau Reference Moore-Berg, Hameiri and Bruneau2022). Furthermore, many assume that a large number of unauthorized immigrants vote in elections (Ekins and Kemp Reference Ekins and Kemp2021) – a misperception that became particularly widespread in the context of the 2020 US presidential race.

Some of these governance misperceptions even morph into sophisticated conspiracy theories, according to which governmental and non-governmental actors are controlled by ruthless globalist elites that use immigration to alter the ethnocultural composition of their societies (see, e.g., Bergmann Reference Bergmann2018; Davey and Ebner Reference Davey and Ebner2014, Gaston and Uscinski Reference Gaston and Uscinski2018). On this explicitly conspiratorial reading, governments are suspected not only of obscuring the unpleasant realities described earlier, but also of deliberately rendering their formerly monocultural countries multicultural. Such theories can take on a life of their own and serve as the foundation of far-right narratives, such as the ‘Great Replacement’ – a term that denotes the idea that autochthonous Europeans are being ‘replaced’ with immigrants from Africa and the Middle East (see, e.g., Bergmann Reference Bergmann2018; Önnerfors Reference Önnerfors, Önnerfors and Krouwel2021). Once only found at the political fringes, many of these conspiracy elements have been increasingly mainstreamed into current right-wing politics. Similarly, instruments of international immigration governance, such as the Global Compact for Safe and Orderly Migration (GCM), are presented as cases of sovereignty being dismantled and mass immigration being imposed through the backdoor. These efforts have reaped remarkable success: false claims about the GCM, originally promoted by a smattering of anti-immigration activists in 2018, have caused some European governments to withdraw their initial support for the GCM and others to waver in theirs.

This overview illustrates that misperceptions about immigration are diverse in character and prevalence. The literature also suggests that, once adopted, they are often persistent and stable, driving people to go to considerable lengths to protect their beliefs (Druckman, Fein and Leeper Reference Druckman, Fein and Leeper2012). Duffy and Frere-Smith's (Reference Duffy and Frere-Smith2014) study shines a light on this tendency, showing that many survey participants not only overestimate the number of immigrants in their country, but, upon being informed of the correct numbers, also insist on their estimates being accurate and the official data being false. In other words, people are prone to believe in second-order misperceptions as well, that is, misperceptions that exist in order to defend or legitimize others. Related to this phenomenon, yet distinct from it, is a lack of awareness regarding the proliferation of misperceptions. Research from the United States indicates that most people are ignorant of how common anti-immigration biases are in society (see, e.g., Earle and Hodson Reference Earle and Hodson2020; McConahay, Hardee and Batts Reference McConahay, Hardee and Batts1981) and how much they themselves are affected by the prejudices these biases evoke (West and Eaton Reference West and Eaton2019).

It must be noted, however, that misperceptions are difficult to measure because we often cannot be sure whether we are measuring real beliefs or, rather, on-the-spot judgements elicited by the survey-response process (Zaller Reference Zaller1992).Footnote 5 For this reason, survey questions about misperceptions may measure non-beliefs or expressive responding rather than the sincere opinions of respondents (Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler Reference Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler2017). Bearing these limitations in mind, we can nevertheless derive some conclusions from the evidence presented so far, most notably, that misperceptions are common across a great range of issues and countries, and thus appear to be an innate characteristic of how people reason about immigration. However, this does not imply that they are equally widespread everywhere: we know that the spatial variance of innumeracy levels is significant, with Scandinavia at the bottom and Latin American countries at the top of the scale (see, e.g., Aalberg and Strabac Reference Aalberg and Strabac2010; Eurobarometer 2018; Gorodzeisky and Semyonov Reference Gorodzeisky and Semyonov2020; Herda Reference Herda2018; IPSOS 2015).Footnote 6 Furthermore, misperceptions are also subject to changes over time, with immigration innumeracy being on the rise in the United States (Herda Reference Herda2019) but anti-immigration biases decreasing in Canada (Wilkes and Corrigall Reference Wilkes and Corrigall2011).Footnote 7 Despite this great variability, misperceptions about immigration are directional and not random deviations resulting from blind guessing or a lack of information. They skew towards highlighting aspects of immigration deemed threatening, dangerous or harmful, and thus carry an inherent negativity bias. Put differently, they paint the picture of immigration in far darker colours than reality warrants.

Motivations for Misperceptions about Immigration

Following this inquiry into the nature and prevalence of misperceptions about immigration, the second part of this review systematizes their underlying motivations. Based on Zaller's (Reference Zaller1992, 6) understanding of beliefs as a ‘marriage of information and predisposition’, a growing literature views human reasoning as guided by the inclination to favour interpretations of the world that align with one's pre-existing beliefs and sentiments (Jerit and Zhao Reference Jerit and Zhao2020; Kunda Reference Kunda1990; Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2013). This is even true for situations where such interpretations come at the expense of logical consistency or factual accuracy (see, e.g., Nyhan and Reifler Reference Nyhan and Reifler2019) because the desire to arrive at their preferred conclusion takes precedence. Consequently, some researchers have argued that directional motivated reasoning is the default way in which humans seek and process information and form beliefs (Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler Reference Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler2017; Redlawsk Reference Redlawsk2002; Taber and Lodge Reference Taber and Lodge2006).

Whether they do so in practice should depend on the purpose that is most salient in their minds during information processing (Groenendyk and Krupnikov Reference Groenendyk and Krupnikov2021), which implies that the form and frequency of misperceptions should vary across different issues and sub-issues (Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler Reference Flynn, Nyhan and Reifler2017; Riek, Mania and Gaertner Reference Riek, Mania and Gaertner2006). This has sparked calls for a closer examination of the conditions and motivations at the issue level (see, e.g., Bayes et al. Reference Bayes2020; Druckman Reference Druckman2012: 206). In the case of immigration, the salience and emotional charge of the issue have been highlighted (Cornelius and Rosenblum Reference Cornelius and Rosenblum2005, 103; Johnston and Ballard Reference Johnston and Ballard2016), yet the question of specific motivations that arise from the nature of the issue remains unanswered.

Our review combines insights from the literature on immigration attitudes with theoretical accounts of motivated reasoning and is premised on the distinction between natives and immigrants as two distinct social groups (Berry Reference Berry2001; Brader, Valentino and Suhay Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008; Green, Sarrasin and Fasel Reference Green, Sarrasin, Fasel and Wright2015).Footnote 8 It further builds on a comprehensive body of literature which suggests that the modal response of natives to immigrants is to feel threatened (for overviews, see Hainmueller and Hopkins Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2014; Stephan and Stephan Reference Stephan, Stephan and Oskamp2000), up to the point where merely thinking of immigration is already sufficient to stimulate a sense of threat (Homola Reference Homola2020).Footnote 9 Accordingly, group-centric threat sentiment, that is, the fear of suffering negative consequences from the presence of immigrants, is assumed to be the common reaction of natives to immigration. This view of immigrants as a threatening out-group has been linked to xenophobic prejudices and stereotypes (see, e.g., Esses et al. Reference Esses, Crandall and Schaller2005). It generates and nourishes a negative affective state that leads to motivated reasoning, inasmuch as people are guided by threat perception and in-group favouritism, rather than by the striving for factual correctness (see, for example, Boyer Reference Boyer2021; Erisen, Lodge and Taber Reference Erisen, Lodge and Taber2014; Gadarian and Albertson Reference Gadarian and Albertson2014).

The literature about immigration attitudes finds that the experience of cultural, economic and security threats can cause different reactions to immigration (see, e.g., Hainmueller and Hopkins Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2014; Homola and Tavits Reference Homola and Tavits2018; Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior Reference Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior2004). The nature of the threat can be individual (egotropic) or collective (sociotropic), with different types of threat fulfilling different psychological functions and triggering different cognitive and behavioural responses (see, e.g., Ben-Nun Bloom, Arikan and Lahav Reference Ben-Nun Bloom, Arikan and Lahav2015; Gorodzeisky Reference Gorodzeisky2013; Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior Reference Sniderman, Hagendoorn and Prior2004). This implies that the multidimensional nature of threats and their effects must be kept in mind. Over the following paragraphs, we review the literature to determine how cultural, economic and security threats motivate misperceptions about immigration.

The Cultural Threat: Social Identity

Immigration issues are often linked to debates about sociocultural identity, in which immigrants, their descendants and their communities assume the position of a salient out-group (see, e.g., Burns and Gimpel Reference Burns and Gimpel2000; Hopkins Reference Hopkins2010). In most of these cases, perceptions of cultural threat stem from concerns that immigrants reject the values of the host society and violate its social norms (Stephan and Stephan Reference Stephan, Stephan and Oskamp2000). Tajfel and Turner's (Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979) theory of social identity, according to which people identify with social groups and share the belief that their in-group is positively distinct from out-groups, can explain such concerns: in order to forge a strong and durable group identity, it is helpful to emphasize intra-group differences that denigrate the out-group and cast one's in-group in an exceedingly positive light.

Perceptions of cultural threat are therefore likely to evoke motivated reasoning, with people rejecting accurate information if it poses a psychological threat to their self-concept and group identity (Kunda Reference Kunda1990; Nyhan and Reifler Reference Nyhan and Reifler2019). The more one attaches their personal identity to a social group – such as an ethnicity or a nationality – the more attention they will pay to threats to said group and the more inclined they will be to believe information that foments feelings of out-group threat (Herrmann Reference Herrmann2017). In this regard, misperceptions about immigration can provide individuals with something useful to protect their identity and boost their self-esteem as the gap between themselves and the out-group becomes manifest (Brewer Reference Brewer1991; Hewstone, Rubin and Willis Reference Hewstone, Rubin and Willis2002; Sherman and Cohen Reference Sherman, Cohen and Zanna2006). Hence, the cultural threat motivates misperceptions about the properties of immigration, for instance, that immigrants adhere to strange and incompatible norms and lifestyles (Helbling et al. Reference Helbling2017; McLaren and Johnson Reference McLaren and Johnson2007).

The Economic Threat: Group Competition

The second threat can be traced to the suspicion that immigration may adversely affect one's access to scarce goods. This idea is embedded in what is commonly referred to as ‘realistic conflict theory’, that is, the in-group's belief that out-groups are direct competitors in the struggle for limited resources (LeVine and Campbell Reference LeVine and Campbell1972; Quillian Reference Quillian1995; Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky Reference Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky2008; Stephan, Ybarra and Morrison Reference Stephan, Ybarra, Morrison and Nelson2009). Realistic conflict theory posits that rational individuals strive to secure their own and their in-group's material welfare and, as a result, tend to perceive the presence of competing out-groups as threatening (see, e.g., Bobo Reference Bobo1988). Applied to immigration, the native in-group aspires to preserve its privileged economic position and to protect it against immigrants who lack the same institutional access and entitlements. According to evidence from Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva (Reference Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva2022), merely thinking about immigration already reduces people's willingness to share resources with others. In practice, this defence of material interest is most pronounced in such fields as education, housing or public services, where resources and opportunities are scarce, and immigrants are perceived as putting additional strain on the system (Esses et al. Reference Esses2001). Yet, it is worth noting that group competition is not limited to material goods, but can extend to non-material power resources like social status and participatory rights as well (see, e.g., Dancygier Reference Dancygier2010). More so than purely material benefits, the latter are traditionally regarded as privileges of the native population, or those who, by virtue of their birth, constitute the sovereign people that politics ought to represent.

Perceptions of economic threat are likely to evoke motivated reasoning based on group-specific self-interestFootnote 10 and binary categories of deservingness: the deservingness of immigrants is discounted while that of natives is validated (see, e.g., Quist and Resendez Reference Quist and Resendez2002). We may consequently deduce that the more a person believes that group competition endangers their distributive interests, the more likely this person is to form misperceptions linked to the economic effects of immigrant presence. A particularly notable kind of misperception for which this effect can be observed are zero-sum beliefs, such as the lump of labour fallacy, that is, the assumption that the labour market is a zero-sum game with a steady number of jobs. Victims of this fallacy believe that immigrants can only thrive at the expenses of natives (or other immigrants) since they are only imagined as job takers and not as job creators (Esses et al. Reference Esses2001).Footnote 11

The Security Threat: Public Safety

The third threat relates to issues of public safety as immigrants, especially those from Muslim-majority countries, are often thought to be associated with crime and terrorism (see, e.g., Fasani et al. Reference Fasani2019; Helbling and Meierrieks Reference Helbling and Meierrieks2022; Huysmans Reference Huysmans2006; Lahav and Courtemanche Reference Lahav and Courtemanche2012; Merolla and Zechmeister Reference Merolla and Zechmeister2009). For example, Semyonov, Gorodzeisky and Glikman (Reference Semyonov, Gorodzeisky and Glikman2012) analyse survey data from 21 European countries and find a strong correlation between people's fear of crime and the share of non-Europeans living in their neighbourhood. Using a quasi-experimental design, Ajzenman, Dominguez and Undurraga (Reference Ajzenman, Dominguez and Undurraga2021) similarly show that a surge in immigration increased Chileans' fears of crime but did not raise the actual crime rate. In many countries, immigrant communities are also believed to undermine the rule of law and defy integration efforts (see, e.g., Fitzgerald, Curtis and Corliss Reference Fitzgerald, Curtis and Corliss2012; Givens, Freeman and Leal Reference Givens, Freeman and Leal2008) – a narrative that culminates in the imagery of parallel societies and no-go areas from which state authorities have long retreated (see, e.g., Gruner Reference Gruner2010). The security threat is thus twofold, as it concerns matters of both personal and collective security.

Like in the other cases, perceptions of security threat may evoke motivated reasoning that leads people to misperceive the actual association between immigration and security risks.Footnote 12 Empirical research has provided evidence for the assumption that concerns about immigrant crime are crucial for the formation of anti-immigration attitudes (see, e.g., Ceobanu Reference Ceobanu2011; Fitzgerald, Curtis and Corliss Reference Fitzgerald, Curtis and Corliss2012; McLaren and Johnson Reference McLaren and Johnson2007), and more so if they are accompanied by broader feelings of insecurity and powerlessness. The latter likely reinforce in-group identification since the in-group is perceived as providing needed protection against the hostile outside world; a case in point are terrorist attacks, which have been found to foster resentment against immigrants (Helbling and Meierrieks Reference Helbling and Meierrieks2022). Although the security threat has received less scholarly attention than its economic and cultural counterparts, it has become increasingly investigated since the 11 September 2001 attacks and the subsequent securitization of migration (Huysmans Reference Huysmans2006).

Determinants of Misperceptions

In the third and final part of this review, we assess the contextual and individual factors identified as conducive to misperceptions about immigration. What do we know about the contexts in which they flourish, the predispositions that determine individual susceptibility and the prospect of corrections?

At the contextual level, one can find several analytical foci, with researchers highlighting the influence of the mass media, political elites and macroeconomic conditions. Concerning the mass media, which has the potential to, at the same time, distort perceptions and enlighten about social reality, the literature shows a clear tendency: most media reports on immigration are negative and depict immigrants as a menace to society (for an overview, see Eberl et al. Reference Eberl2018). Such content is likely to corroborate threat perception and bolster inaccurate beliefs. In a framing experiment, Blinder and Jeannet (Reference Blinder and Jeannet2017) found that media depictions of immigration affect the accuracy of the British public's perceptions of immigrants. In the same way, an over-representation of ‘foreign crime’ articles in an Austrian newspaper led to greater misperceptions among its readership (Arendt Reference Arendt2010). These effects may depend, in part, on the type of media people get their information from; consuming newspapers and online news has been linked with a decrease in misperceptions, whereas the opposite effect has been found for social media and television (Aalberg and Strabac Reference Aalberg and Strabac2010; Herda Reference Herda2010; Meltzer and Schemer Reference Meltzer, Schemer and Strömbäck2021). In sum, there is strong evidence that the mass media's framing of immigration shapes our beliefs about the issue.

Political elites, in their capacity as influential actors who shape discourses and perceptions, are a second contextual factor that has drawn considerable scholarly attention. In view of widespread concerns about immigration, they may have electoral incentives to stoke fears, posture as adopting tough stances on the issue (Lutz Reference Lutz2021) and exaggerate its negative effects (Golder Reference Golder2003). In doing so, they seek to capitalize on the status of immigrants as outsiders onto whom the public can project their fears and anxieties (Cochrane and Nevitte Reference Cochrane and Nevitte2014; Dinas and van Spanje Reference Dinas and van Spanje2011). Heizmann and Huth (Reference Heizmann and Huth2021) provide evidence for such a dynamic on the macro level; they find that perceptions of economic threat are more likely to emerge in countries where political parties adopt a more hostile rhetoric towards immigrants. Meanwhile, at the micro level, experimental studies from the United States show that elite cues of immigrant threat trigger anxiety (Brader, Valentino and Suhay Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008) and elite polarization on immigration reform reinforces the role of partisan reasoning (Druckman, Peterson and Slothuus Reference Druckman, Peterson and Slothuus2013).

The assumption that macroeconomic conditions have an impact is derived from group threat theory: the worse the state of the economy, the greater the native population's perceptions of immigrants as competitors (see, e.g., Quillian Reference Quillian1995). However, the evidence seems a little more ambiguous. While cross-sectional studies indicate that perceptions of immigrants' societal impact are more negative in countries with poor economic performance (see, e.g., Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky Reference Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky2006; Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky Reference Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky2008), recent longitudinal research finds that the attitudinal effect of major economic shocks is restricted to those in the formative years of young adulthood (Cotofan, Dur and Meier Reference Cotofan, Dur and Meier2021; Kustov, Laaker and Reller Reference Kustov, Laaker and Reller2021). Still, the perceived state of the economy remains a better predictor of immigration views than objective economic conditions (Hainmueller and Hopkins Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2014; Heizmann and Huth Reference Heizmann and Huth2021; Sides and Citrin Reference Sides and Citrin2007).

In addition to these contextual factors, individual traits, such as personal predisposition and contact with immigrants, may also affect people's susceptibility to misperceptions. One key element here is ideology: those who identify as right-wing or hold right-wing convictions (for example, tradition over progress, authority over equality and control over openness) are more prone to misperceptions about immigration, especially to such that highlight the negative social impact of immigrants (see, e.g., Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva Reference Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva2022; Heizmann and Huth Reference Heizmann and Huth2021; Herda Reference Herda2019; Johnston and Ballard Reference Johnston and Ballard2016; Meltzer and Schemer Reference Meltzer, Schemer and Strömbäck2021; Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky Reference Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky2006). Nonetheless, some experimental studies have found that individuals across the ideological spectrum succumb to motivated reasoning on immigration to a similar extent (Lind et al. Reference Lind2022; Washburn and Skitka Reference Washburn and Skitka2017). This paradox can be resolved by accounting for the different character of people's ideological tenets. Right-wing ideologies rest on more exclusionary conceptions of identity, emphasize group antagonism and thereby facilitate the perception of immigrants as members of a hostile out-group (Albertson and Gadarian Reference Albertson and Gadarian2015; Blinder and Lundgren Reference Blinder and Lundgren2019; Jost et al. Reference Jost2003). Accordingly, individuals with right-wing views are more likely to feel threatened by the social reality of ethnocultural diversity and to develop misperceptions in response. Aside from ideology, education also seems to be an important determinant, with higher education levels being associated with more accurate perceptions about immigration (see, e.g., Aalberg and Strabac Reference Aalberg and Strabac2010; Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva Reference Alesina, Miano and Stantcheva2022; Gorodzeisky and Semyonov Reference Gorodzeisky and Semyonov2020; Heizmann and Huth Reference Heizmann and Huth2021; McLaren and Johnson Reference McLaren and Johnson2007).

A third explanatory factor relates to one's own immigration experiences and contact with immigrants. Individuals who have themselves migrated demonstrate greater immigration innumeracy (Aalberg and Strabac Reference Aalberg and Strabac2010), as do ethnic minority members (Herda Reference Herda2010), residents of more diverse neighbourhoods (Herda Reference Herda2010; Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky Reference Semyonov, Raijman and Gorodzeisky2008) and, albeit to a lesser degree, natives who count immigrants among their friends and co-workers (see, e.g., Herda Reference Herda2010; Lundmark and Kokkonen Reference Lundmark and Kokkonen2017). Conversely, those who live in countries with more immigrants display lower innumeracy (Gorodzeisky and Semyonov Reference Gorodzeisky and Semyonov2020; Herda Reference Herda2013). Being exposed to immigrants in everyday life may thus contribute to overestimating their share, while living in a society with high levels of immigration predisposes individuals to more accurate perceptions.

Finally, we ask whether misperceptions can be corrected through targeted interventions. In general, the empirical evidence indicates that people are reluctant to update their beliefs when they are presented with accurate information (see, e.g., Duffy and Frere-Smith Reference Duffy and Frere-Smith2014). Studying misperceptions in the aftermath of the Brexit referendum, Rolfe et al. (Reference Rolfe2018) provided participants of UK focus groups with evidence about the actual economic effect of immigration only to find that their perceptions remained unchanged. Jørgensen and Osmundsen (Reference Jørgensen and Osmundsen2022) used a survey experiment to show that while Danish citizens update their beliefs about immigrants' population share, welfare dependency and crime rates, they reinterpret this new information to make it consistent with their pre-existing immigration preferences. Hopkins, Sides and Citrin (Reference Hopkins, Sides and Citrin2019) came to a similar conclusion for the United States, but Grigorieff, Roth and Ubfal (Reference Grigorieff, Roth and Ubfal2020) found that information treatments have a significant correctional effect that may even last for several weeks (see also Carnahan, Bergan and Lee Reference Carnahan, Bergan and Lee2021; Haaland and Roth Reference Haaland and Roth2020). Given these mixed results, simple information treatments may not be the most effective method to correct misperceptions, but they can still reduce support for anti-immigration policies if combined with an empathy treatment (Moore-Berg, Hameiri and Bruneau Reference Moore-Berg, Hameiri and Bruneau2022). Other studies point out that more realistic portrayals of immigrants could have a positive effect on perceptual accuracy (Blinder and Jeannet Reference Blinder and Jeannet2017) or expound how narratives can be a powerful source of perceptions (Dennison Reference Dennison2021). Overall, it does not appear as if people can be easily swayed by exposing them to accurate information; for this reason, the most promising interventions focus on reducing affective immigrant threat.

Conclusion

Undoubtedly, the ‘way in which the world is imagined determines … what men will do’ (Lippmann Reference Lippmann1997, 25). Yet, the way in which people imagine immigration is often characterized by inaccurate beliefs and plain falsehoods. This article has offered the first systematic account of such misperceptions by reviewing their nature, motivations and determinants. We found that they are not only widespread and diverse, but also directional, that is, they are marked by a negativity bias and revolve around the threatening properties of immigration. We then identified three distinct immigrant threats that motivate this bias: a cultural threat to one's identity and values; an economic threat based on inter-group competition; and a security threat to public and personal safety. Finally, our review cast light on the relevant determinants of misperceptions about immigration. We found that individual predispositions, such as right-wing views or low levels of education, as well as the influence of the mass media and political elites, explain their prevalence. We also found that misperceptions are mostly resistant to correction and that providing accurate information is largely ineffective in combating them. Reflecting the deep-seated concerns of many natives, they are likely to persist as a key element of immigration politics.

To conclude this review, there are both lessons to be learned and pathways for future research. Although the literature on attitudes towards immigration is vast and expanding, research dealing with perceptions and misperceptions still constitutes a minor and fragmented field. We believe that this fact leaves a wide intellectual territory uncharted, the exploration of which may allow us to gain relevant insights into the distinctive role immigration plays in political conflicts.

First, our review underscores that misperceptions about immigration are neither exceptional nor restricted to the fringes of society; rather, they seem to be the norm. Given their pervasive proliferation, it is even more surprising that there is no coherent body of research. In addition, many studies that measure perceptions bundle them with other attitudinal items (such as policy preferences) or use them only as an additional factor to explain immigration attitudes. We hence consider misperceptions (and perceptions in general) to be an essential but under-studied aspect of how people develop their attitudes, preferences and behaviours with respect to immigration.

Second, empirical research on misperceptions about immigration has become more common over time. Yet, some notable shortcomings in its scope and validity hamper the progress of the field. Most survey measurements only capture a very narrow range of misperceptions since question items are confined to innumeracy and neglect the many other forms of inaccuracies that exist. Moreover, there is only little longitudinal survey data that track misperceptions over time and almost no coverage beyond Western democracies. Additional research in these directions would allow us to better understand the different scope conditions of misperceptions. Lastly, in terms of methodology, most current measurement instruments fail to adequately capture the central distinction between being uninformed and being misinformed. Assuming the latter without empirical evidence entails the risk of overestimating the extent of misperceptions and misclassifying survey responses. Here, we recommend measuring either confidence in or the certainty of one's beliefs to separate non-beliefs from actual misperceptions (see, e.g., Carlson and Hill Reference Carlson and Hill2021).

Third, questions of causality (whether misperceptions are antecedents or consequences of immigration attitudes) remain largely unresolved. Our review has identified immigrant threat as the underlying motivation for misperceptions that may, in turn, reinforce and consolidate this sense of threat. This vicious circle calls for additional research in the form of innovative experimental studies that delve deeper into the causal dynamics and feedback effects at work. On a related note, a general lesson from the attitudinal literature is that different threats evoke different cognitive and emotional responses, yet we still lack this differentiation in research on misperceptions. Understanding these causalities would enable us to anchor the study of misperceptions in the broader literature and help us to design effective interventions to correct them.

Finally, our understanding of the effects of misperceptions on immigration politics are still limited. This bears potential for scholarship that explores how they shape the behaviour of both citizens and political elites: when do those in power attempt to correct misperceptions, and when do they harness them to pander to the electorate instead? Research has shown that many politicians employ anti-immigration rhetoric to address the concerns of their constituents but refrain from restrictive policies (see, e.g., Lutz Reference Lutz2021; Slaven and Boswell Reference Slaven and Boswell2019). Future research should pay more attention to the political context and examine the ramifications of misperceptions for political communication, policy making and liberal democracy more generally.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material is available at: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123422000084

Acknowledgements

We thank Lea Portmann, Elisa Volpi and our two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments. We also thank the panellists and panel attendees of the 2020 Oxford Migration Conference and the 2021 Annual Congress of the Swiss Political Science Association, where earlier versions of this article were presented.

Financial Support

This article was financially supported by the ‘NCCR – on the Move’, a research instrument of the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant Number 51NF40-182897).

Competing Interests

None.

Footnotes

1 It should be noted that we confine this review article to misperceptions about international migration and thus exclude misperceptions about internal migration or, more generally, ethnic or religious minorities, who are often conflated with immigrants. Evidence suggests that misperceptions about such minority groups are also widespread (see, e.g., Nadeau, Niemi and Levine Reference Nadeau, Niemi and Levine1993).

2 Specific groups may deviate from these general public perceptions. Perceptions of government officials, for example, have been found to be more accurate than those of the average citizen (Lee et al. Reference Lee2021).

3 The degree of immigration innumeracy ranges from single-percentage differences to a multiple of the actual immigrant share. Comparative research on Western democracies suggests that average estimations are about twice as large as the reality (see, e.g., Citrin and Sides Reference Citrin and Sides2008).

4 Israel and Saudi Arabia, which have rather high immigrant shares, are rare exceptions: Ipsos (2015) found that people in both countries tend to slightly underestimate the share of immigrants.

5 For a list of survey items that measure misperceptions about immigration in general social science surveys, see the Online Supplementary Material.

6 The degree of innumeracy can be conceived of as the relative or absolute difference between the perceived and the real share of the immigrant population. When taking relative difference as criteria, misperceptions are more pronounced in countries with low immigration levels, such as those of Eastern Europe (see Eurobarometer 2018).

7 The lack of panel data limits our ability to explain how misperceptions develop over time. However, a panel survey by Meltzer and Schemer (Reference Meltzer, Schemer and Strömbäck2021) on immigration innumeracy suggests that misperceptions only change at a slow pace.

8 Some researchers argue that sorting people into different social groups already elicits cognitive biases (see, e.g., Ellemers, Spears and Doosje Reference Ellemers, Spears and Doosje2002). Bursztyn and Yang (Reference Bursztyn and Yang2021) find that misperceptions about out-groups are systematically larger than about in-groups.

9 The sense of immigrant threat does not need any objective foundation, but can stem from subjective perceptions alone (Stephan and Stephan Reference Stephan, Stephan and Oskamp2000).

10 It should be noted that there is no a priori reason to assume that misperceptions rooted in the defence of one's self-interest do really serve this purpose (Elster Reference Elster2016, 157). Instead, they may even prevent people from fostering this goal.

11 Equating immigration with unemployment is a common strategy of the far right to exploit grievances and mobilize voters (see, e.g., Givens Reference Givens2005).

12 Coping with insecurity has been identified as a key function of motivated reasoning and a key element of modern-day conspiracism (see, e.g., Abalakina-Paap et al. Reference Abalakina-Paap1999; Imhoff and Bruder Reference Imhoff and Bruder2014).

References

Aalberg, T and Strabac, Z (2010) Media use and misperceptions. Nordicom Review 31, 3552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abalakina-Paap, M et al. (1999) Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychology 20, 637647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajzenman, N, Dominguez, P and Undurraga, R (2021) Immigration, Crime, and Crime (Mis)Perceptions. IZA Discussion Paper No. 14087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Albertson, B and Gadarian, SK (2015) Anxious Politics: Democratic Citizenship in A Threatening World. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alesina, AF, Miano, A and Stantcheva, S (2022) Immigration and Redistribution. The Review of Economic Studies, https://doi.org/10.1093/restud/rdac011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arendt, F (2010) Cultivation effects of a newspaper on reality estimates and explicit and implicit attitudes. Journal of Media Psychology 22(4), 147159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bansak, C, Simpson, N and Zavodny, M (2021) Labor market effects of immigration: evidence. In Bansak, C, Simpson, N and Zavodny, M (eds), The Economics of Immigration. London: Routledge, 208246.Google Scholar
Battisti, M et al. (2018) Immigration, search and redistribution: a quantitative assessment of native welfare. Journal of the European Economic Association 16, 11371188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayes, R et al. (2020) When and how different motives can drive motivated political reasoning. Political Psychology 41, 10311052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beerli, A et al. (2021) The abolition of immigration restrictions and the performance of firms and workers: evidence from Switzerland. American Economic Review 111, 976–1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Nun Bloom, P, Arikan, G and Lahav, G (2015) The effect of perceived cultural and material threats on ethnic preferences in immigration attitudes. Ethnic and Racial Studies 38, 17601778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergmann, E (2018) Conspiracy and Populism: The Politics of Misinformation. Cambridge: Polity.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, JW (2001) A psychology of immigration. Journal of Social Issues 57, 615631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blinder, S (2015) Imagined immigration: the impact of different meanings of ‘immigrants’ in public opinion and policy debates in Britain. Political Studies 63, 80100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blinder, S and Jeannet, A (2017) The ‘illegal’ and the skilled: effects of media portrayals on perceptions of immigrants in Britain. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 44, 14441462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blinder, S and Lundgren, L (2019) Roots of group threat: anti-prejudice motivations and implicit bias in perceptions of immigrants as threats. Journal of Ethnic and Racial Studies 42, 19711989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blinder, S and Schaffner, BF (2020) Going with the flows – information that changes Americans’ immigration preferences. International Journal of Public Opinion Research 32, 153164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bobo, L (1988) Attitudes toward the black political movement: trends, meaning, and effects on racial policy preferences. Social Psychology Quarterly 51, 287302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyer, MM (2021) Aroused argumentation: how the news exacerbates motivated reasoning. The International Journal of Press/Politics, https://doi.org/10.1177/19401612211010577.Google Scholar
Brader, T, Valentino, NA and Suhay, E (2008) What triggers public opposition to immigration? Anxiety, group cues, and immigration threat. American Journal of Political Science 52, 959978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, MB (1991) The social self: on being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 17, 475482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, P and Gimpel, JG (2000) Economic insecurity, prejudicial stereotypes, and public opinion on immigration policy. Political Science Quarterly 115, 201225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bursztyn, L and Yang, DY (2021) Misperceptions about Others. NBER Working Paper No. 29168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, B (2007) The Myth of the Rational Voter: Why Democracies Choose Bad Policies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Carlson, TN and Hill, SJ (2021) Experimental measurement of misperception in political beliefs. Journal of Experimental Political Science, https://doi.org/10.1017/XPS.2021.2.Google Scholar
Carnahan, D, Bergan, DE and Lee, S (2021) Do corrective effects last? Results from a longitudinal experiment on beliefs toward immigration in the U.S. Political Behavior 43, 12271246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castanho Silva, B, Vegetti, F and Littvay, L (2017) The elite is up to something. Exploring the relationship between populism and beliefs in conspiracy theories. Swiss Political Science Review 23, 423443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ceobanu, AM (2011) Usual suspects? Public views about immigrants’ impact on crime in European countries. International Journal of Comparative Sociology 52, 114131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Citrin, J and Sides, J (2008) Immigration and the imagined community in Europe and the United States. Political Studies 56, 3356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cochrane, C and Nevitte, N (2014) Scapegoating: unemployment, far-right parties and anti-immigrant sentiment. Comparative European Politics 12, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornelius, W and Rosenblum, MR (2005) Immigration and politics. Annual Review of Political Science 8, 99119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cotofan, M, Dur, R and Meier, S (2021) Does growing up in a recession increase compassion? The case of attitudes towards immigration. CEP Working Paper No. 1757.Google Scholar
Dancygier, R (2010) Immigration and Conflict in Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davey, J and Ebner, J (2014) ‘The Great Replacement’: The Violent Consequences of Mainstreamed Extremism. London: Institute for Strategic Dialogue.Google Scholar
Dennison, J (2021) Narratives: a review of concepts, determinants, effects, and uses in migration research. Comparative Migration Studies 9, 50. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40878-021-00259-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinas, E and van Spanje, J (2011) Crime story: the role of crime and immigration in the anti-immigration vote. Electoral Studies 30, 658671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, T et al. (2019) Attitudes towards National Identity, Immigration, and Refugees in Greece. More in Common. https://www.moreincommon.com/media/ltinlcnc/0535-more-in-common-greece-report_final-4_web_lr.pdf.Google Scholar
Druckman, JN (2012) The politics of motivation. Critical Review: A Journal of Politics and Society 24, 199216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, JN, Fein, J and Leeper, TJ (2012) A source of bias in public opinion stability. American Political Science Review 106, 430454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, JN, Peterson, E and Slothuus, R (2013) How elite partisan polarization affects public opinion formation. American Political Science Review 107, 5779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, B and Frere-Smith, T (2014) Perceptions and reality: public attitudes to immigration. IPSOS Mori Social Research Institute. https://www.ipsos.com/sites/default/files/publication/1970-01/sri-perceptions-and-reality-immigration-report-2013.pdfGoogle Scholar
Dustmann, C and Frattini, T (2014) The fiscal effects of immigration to the UK. The Economic Journal 124, 593643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dustmann, C and Preston, IP (2019) Free movement, open borders, and the global gains from labor mobility. Annual Review of Economics 11, 783808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Earle, M and Hodson, G (2020) Questioning white losses and anti-white discrimination in the United States. Nature Human Behaviour 4, 160168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eberl, JM et al. (2018) The European media discourse on immigration and its effects: a literature review. Annals of the International Communication Association 42, 207223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekins, E and Kemp, D (2021) E Pluribus Unum: Findings from the Cato Institute 2021 Immigration and Identity National Survey. Available from https://www.cato.org/survey-reports/e-pluribus-unum-findings-cato-institute-2021-immigration-identity-national-surveyGoogle Scholar
Ellemers, N, Spears, R and Doosje, B (2002) Self and social identity. Annual Review of Psychology 53, 161186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elster, J (2016) Sour Grapes: Studies in the Subversion of Rationality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erisen, C, Lodge, M and Taber, CS (2014) Affective contagion in effortful political thinking. Political Psychology 35, 187206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esses, VM et al. (2001) The immigration dilemma: the role of perceived group competition, ethnic prejudice, and national identity. Journal of Social Issues 57, 389412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esses, VM et al. (2005) Historical and modern perspectives on group competition. In Crandall, CS and Schaller, M (eds), Social Psychology of Prejudice: Historical and Contemporary Issues. Lawrence, KS: Lewinian Press, 97116.Google Scholar
Eurobarometer (2018) Integration of Immigrants in the European Union. Special Eurobarometer 469. Brussels: European Commission.Google Scholar
Fasani, F et al. (2019) Does Immigration Increase Crime? Migration Policy and the Creation of the Criminal Immigrant. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzgerald, J, Curtis, KA and Corliss, CL (2012) Anxious publics: worries about crime and immigration. Comparative Political Studies 45, 477506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, DJ, Nyhan, B and Reifler, J (2017) The nature and origins of misperceptions: understanding false and unsupported beliefs about politics. Advances in Political Psychology 38, 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedberg, RM and Hunt, J (1995) The impact of immigration on host country wages, employment and growth. Journal of Economic Perspectives 9, 2344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadarian, SK and Albertson, B (2014) Anxiety, immigration, and the search for information. Political Psychology 35, 133164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganesh, B (2018) The ungovernability of digital hate culture. Journal of International Affairs 71, 3049.Google Scholar
Gaston, S and Uscinski, JE (2018) Out of the Shadows: Conspiracy Thinking on Immigration. London: The Henry Jackson Society.Google Scholar
Gavin, NT (2018) Media definitely do matter: Brexit, immigration, climate change and beyond. British Journal of Politics and International Relations 20, 827845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givens, TE (2005) Voting Radical Right in Western Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givens, TE, Freeman, GP and Leal, DL (2008) Immigration Policy and Security: U.S., European, and Commonwealth Perspectives. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golder, M (2003) Explaining variation in the success of extreme right parties in Western Europe. Comparative Political Studies 36, 432466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, S et al. (2020) Size does matter: the relationship between perceived immigrant group size and attitudes towards foreign nationals. Southern African Journal of Demography 20, 2856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorodzeisky, A (2013) Mechanisms of exclusion: attitudes toward allocation of social rights to out-group population. Ethnic and Racial Studies 36, 795817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorodzeisky, A and Semyonov, M (2020) Perceptions and misperceptions: actual size, perceived size and opposition to immigration in European societies. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 46, 612630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, R (2016) Inter-ideological mingling: white extremist ideology entering the mainstream on Twitter. Sociological Spectrum 36, 2436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, EGT, Sarrasin, O and Fasel, N (2015) Immigration: social psychological aspects. In Wright, JD (ed.), International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, vol. 11, 2nd edn. Oxford: Elsevier, 675681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grigorieff, A, Roth, C and Ubfal, D (2020) Does information change attitudes toward immigrants? Demography 57, 11171143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groenendyk, E and Krupnikov, A (2021) What motivates reasoning? A theory of goal-dependent political evaluation. American Journal of Political Science 65, 180196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruner, S (2010) ‘The others don't want…’. Small-scale segregation: hegemonic public discourses and racial boundaries in German neighbourhoods. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 36, 275292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haaland, I and Roth, C (2020) Labor market concerns and support for immigration. Journal of Public Economics 191, 104256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J and Hopkins, D (2014) Public attitudes toward immigration. Annual Review of Political Science 17, 225249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heizmann, B and Huth, N (2021) Economic conditions and perceptions of immigrants as an economic threat in Europe: temporal dynamics and mediating processes. International Journal of Comparative Sociology 62, 5682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helbling, M and Meierrieks, D (2022) Terrorism and migration: an overview. British Journal of Political Science, 52, 977–996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helbling, M et al. (2017) Attitudes Towards National Identity, Immigration and Refugees in Germany. More in Common. https://www.moreincommon.com/media/r4dd05ba/more-in-common-germany-report-english.pdfGoogle Scholar
Herda, D (2010) How many immigrants? Foreign-born population innumeracy in Europe. Public Opinion Quarterly 74, 674695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herda, D (2013) Too many immigrants? Examining alternative forms of immigrant population innumeracy. Sociological Perspectives 56, 213240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herda, D (2015a) Innumeracy in Turkey: misperceptions of an emerging immigrant population. Insight Turkey 17(2), 187205.Google Scholar
Herda, D (2015b) Beyond innumeracy: heuristic decision-making and qualitative misperceptions about immigrants in Finland. Ethnic and Racial Studies 38, 16271645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herda, D (2018) Comparing ignorance: imagined immigration and the exclusion of migrants in the US and Western Europe. Societies Without Borders 12, 135.Google Scholar
Herda, D (2019) Tracking ignorance: examining changes in immigrant population innumeracy in the United States from 2005 to 2013. Migration Letters 16, 329339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, RK (2017) How attachments to the nation shape beliefs about the world: a theory of motivated reasoning. International Organization 71, 6184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewstone, M, Rubin, M and Willis, H (2002) Intergroup bias. Annual Review of Psychology 53, 575604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Homola, J (2020) Partisanship and perceived threats about immigration. Party Politics 27, 977982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Homola, J and Tavits, M (2018) Contact reduces immigration-related fears for leftist but not for rightist voters. Comparative Political Studies 51, 17891820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, DJ, Sides, J and Citrin, J (2019) The muted consequences of correct information about immigration. Journal of Politics 81, 315320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, HJ (2010) Politicized places: explaining where and when immigrants provoke local opposition. The American Political Science Review 104, 4060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huysmans, J (2006) The Politics of Insecurity: Fear, Migration and Asylum in the EU. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imhoff, R and Bruder, M (2014) Speaking (un-)truth to power: conspiracy mentality as a generalized political attitude. European Journal of Personality 28, 2543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ipsos (2015) Perils of Perceptions 2015: A 33 Country Study. Available from https://www.ipsos.com/sites/default/files/2017-07/ipsos-perils-of-perception-charts-2015.pdfGoogle Scholar
Ipsos (2018) Ipsos Update: A Selection of the Latest Research and Thinking from Ipsos Teams around the World. Available from https://www.ipsos.com/sites/default/files/ct/publication/documents/2018-01/ipsos_update_january_2018.pdfGoogle Scholar
Jerit, J and Zhao, Y (2020) Political misinformation. Annual Review of Political Science 23, 7794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, CD and Ballard, AO (2016) Economists and public opinion: expert consensus and economic policy judgments. The Journal of Politics 78, 443456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jørgensen, FJ and Osmundsen, M (2022) Correcting citizens’ misperceptions about non-Western immigrants: corrective information, interpretations, and policy opinions. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 9, 64–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, JT et al. (2003) Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin 129, 339375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalkan, KO, Layman, GC and Uslaner, EM (2009) ‘Bands of others’? Attitudes toward Muslims in contemporary American society. Journal of Politics 71, 847862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemeny, T and Cooke, A (2018) Spillovers from immigrant diversity in cities. Journal of Economic Geography 18, 213245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuklinski, JH et al. (2000) Misinformation and the currency of democratic citizenship. Journal of Politics 62, 790816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z (1990) The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 108, 480498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kustov, A, Laaker, D and Reller, C (2021) The stability of immigration attitudes: evidence and implications. Journal of Politics 83, 14781494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahav, G and Courtemanche, M (2012) The ideological effects of framing threat on immigration and civil liberties. Political Behavior 34, 477505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, N et al. (2021) More accurate, but no less polarized: comparing the factual beliefs of government officials and the public. British Journal of Political Science 51, 13151322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeVine, RA and Campbell, DT (1972) Ethnocentrism: Theories of Conflict, Ethnic Attitudes, and Group Behavior. New York, NY: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Lind, T et al. (2022) Motivated reasoning when assessing the effects of refugee intake. Behavioural Public Policy, 6, 213–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lippmann, W (1997) Public Opinion (With a New Introduction by Michael Curtis). New Brunswick, NJ, and London: Transaction Press.Google Scholar
Lundmark, S and Kokkonen, A (2017) Measuring immigrant populations: subjective versus objective assessments. Survey Methods: Insights from the Field, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.13094/SMIF-2017-00004Google Scholar
Lutz, P (2021) Reassessing the gap-hypothesis: tough talk and weak action in migration policy? Party Politics 27, 174186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manacorda, M, Manning, A and Wadsworth, J (2012) The impact of immigration on the structure of wages. Evidence from Britain. Journal of the European Economic Association 10, 120151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mancini, T et al. (2020) Support for rejection and reception policies toward asylum seekers in Italy: the role of conservative ideologies and legitimizing myths. Journal of Social Psychology 160, 751767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markaki, Y and Blinder, S (2018) The Effects of Immigration on Welfare across the EU: Do Subjective Evaluations Align with Estimations? REMINDER Working Paper.Google Scholar
Martinsen, DS and Rotger, GB (2017) The fiscal impact of EU immigration on the tax-financed welfare state: testing the ‘welfare burden’ thesis. European Union Politics 18, 620639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConahay, JB, Hardee, BB and Batts, V (1981) Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. The Journal of Conflict Resolution 25, 563579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaren, L and Johnson, M (2007) Resources, group conflict and symbols: explaining anti-immigration hostility in Britain. Political Studies 55, 709732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzer, CE and Schemer, C (2021) Miscounting the others: media effects on perceptions of the immigrant population size. In Strömbäck, J et al. (eds), Media and Public Attitudes toward Migration in Europe: A Comparative Approach. London: Routledge, 174189.Google Scholar
Merolla, JL and Zechmeister, EJ (2009) Democracy at Risk: How Terrorist Threats Affect the Public. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore-Berg, SL, Hameiri, B and Bruneau, EG (2022) Empathy, dehumanization, and misperceptions: a media intervention humanizes migrants and increases empathy for their plight but only if misinformation about migrants is also corrected. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13, 645–655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadeau, R, Niemi, RG and Levine, J (1993) Innumeracy about minority populations. Public Opinion Quarterly 57, 332347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyhan, B and Reifler, J (2019) The roles of information deficit and identity threat in the prevalence of misperceptions. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion, and Parties 29, 222244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Önnerfors, A (2021) Der Grosse Austausch: conspiratorial frames of terrorist violence in Germany. In Önnerfors, A and Krouwel, A (eds), Europe: Continent of Conspiracies. London: Routledge, 7696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orwell, G (1945) Anti-Semitism in Britain. London: Contemporary Jewish Record.Google Scholar
Ottaviano, GIP and Peri, G (2012) Rethinking the effect of immigration on wages. Journal of the European Economic Association 10, 152197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panagopoulos, C (2006) The polls-trends: Arab and Muslim Americans and Islam in the aftermath of 9/11. Public Opinion Quarterly 70, 608624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pedersen, A and Hartley, LK (2017) False beliefs about asylum seekers to Australia: the role of confidence in such beliefs, prejudice, and the third person effect. Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology 11, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quillian, L (1995) Prejudice as a response to perceived group threat. American Sociological Review 60, 586611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quist, RM and Resendez, MG (2002) Social dominance threat: examining social dominance theory's explanation of prejudice as legitimizing myths. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 24, 287293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redlawsk, DP (2002) Hot cognition or cool consideration? Testing the effects of motivated reasoning on political decision making. Journal of Politics 64, 10211044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riek, BM, Mania, EW and Gaertner, SL (2006) Intergroup threat and outgroup attitudes: a meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10, 336353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez-Justicia, D and Theilen, B (2022) Immigration and tax morale: the role of perceptions and prejudices. Empirical Economics, 62, 1801–1832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolfe, H et al. (2018) Post-Brexit Immigration Policy: Reconciling Public Perceptions with Economic Evidence. NIES. https://www.niesr.ac.uk/wp-content/uploads/2021/10/FINAL-Leverhulme-report-FINAL-3.pdfGoogle Scholar
Ruhs, M, Tamas, K and Palme, J (2019) Bridging the Gaps: Linking Research to Public Debates and Policy-Making on Migration and Integration. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumbaut, RG and Ewing, E (2007) The Myth of Immigrant Criminality and the Paradox of Assimilation. American Immigration Council. https://www.americanimmigrationcouncil.org/sites/default/files/research/Imm%20Criminality%20%28IPC%29.pdfGoogle Scholar
Scheve, KF and Slaughter, MJ (2001) Globalization and the Perceptions of American Workers. Washington, DC: Peterson Institute for International Economics.Google Scholar
Semyonov, M, Gorodzeisky, A and Glikman, A (2012) Neighborhood ethnic composition and resident perceptions of safety in European countries. Social Problems 59, 117135.Google Scholar
Semyonov, M, Raijman, R and Gorodzeisky, A (2006) The rise of anti-foreigner sentiment in European societies, 1988–2000. American Sociological Review 71, 426449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Semyonov, M, Raijman, R and Gorodzeisky, A (2008) Foreigners’ impact on European societies: public views and perceptions in a cross-national comparative perspective. International Journal of Comparative Sociology 49, 529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherman, DK and Cohen, GL (2006) The psychology of self-defense: self-affirmation theory. In Zanna, MP (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 38. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 183242.Google Scholar
Sides, J and Citrin, J (2007) European opinion about immigration: the role of identities, interest and information. British Journal of Political Science 37, 477504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, G and Gottfried, G (2017) Global Views on Immigration and the Refugee Crisis. Available from https://www.ipsos.com/en/global-views-immigration-and-refugee-crisisGoogle Scholar
Slaven, M and Boswell, C (2019) Why symbolise control? Irregular migration to the UK and symbolic policy-making in the 1960s. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 45, 14771495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sniderman, PM, Hagendoorn, L and Prior, M (2004) Predisposing factors and situational triggers: exclusionary reactions to immigrant minorities. American Political Science Review 98, 3549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stansfield, R and Stone, B (2018) Threat perceptions of migrants in Britain and support for policy. Sociological Perspectives 61, 592609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steele, LG and Perkins, KM (2019) The effects of perceived neighborhood immigrant population size on preferences for redistribution in New York City: a pilot study. Frontiers in Sociology 4, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephan, WG and Stephan, CW (2000) An integrated threat theory of prejudice. In Oskamp, S (ed.), Reducing Prejudice and Discrimination. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2345.Google Scholar
Stephan, WG, Ybarra, O and Morrison, KR (2009) Intergroup threat theory. In Nelson, TD (ed.), Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 4360.Google Scholar
Taber, CS and Lodge, M (2006) Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science 50, 755769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taber, CS and Lodge, M (2013) The Rationalizing Voter. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H and Turner, JC (1979) An integrative theory of inter-group conflict. In Austin, WG and Worchel, S (eds), The Social Psychology of Inter-group Relations. Monterrey, CA: Brooks/Cole, 3347.Google Scholar
Washburn, AN and Skitka, LJ (2017) Science denial across the political divide: liberals and conservatives are similarly motivated to deny attitude-inconsistent science. Social Psychological and Personality Science 20, 19.Google Scholar
West, K and Eaton, AA (2019) Prejudiced and unaware of it: evidence for the Dunning-Kruger model in the domains of racism and sexism. Personality and Individual Differences 146, 111119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkes, R and Corrigall, C (2011) Explaining time trends in public opinion: attitudes towards immigration and immigrants. International Journal of Comparative Sociology 52, 7999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, N (2021) Misattributed blame? Attitudes toward globalization in the age of automation. Political Science Research and Methods, https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2021.43.Google Scholar
YouGov (2018) YouGov Cambridge Results – Conspiracy Theories. Available from https://bit.ly/2vNKHo9.Google Scholar
Zaller, J (1992) The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmermann, KF (2019) Gaps and challenges of migration policy advice: the German experience. In Ruhs, M, Tamas, K and Palme, J (eds), Bridging the Gaps: Linking Research to Public Debates and Policy-Making on Migration and Integration. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 111126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Lutz and Bitschnau supplementary material

Lutz and Bitschnau supplementary material

Download Lutz and Bitschnau supplementary material(File)
File 352.1 KB