Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-2wr7h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-04T07:26:39.696Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Music on the move: understanding music as otherwise knowledge in early childhood

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 April 2025

Alejandra Pacheco-Costa*
Affiliation:
Arts Education Department, Universidad de Sevilla, Seville, Spain
José J. Roa-Trejo
Affiliation:
Communication and Education Department, Universidad Loyola Andalucía, Seville, Spain
Fernando Guzmán-Simón
Affiliation:
Department of Language and Literature Teaching, Universidad de Sevilla, Seville, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Alejandra Pacheco-Costa; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Posthuman understanding of music and bodies as matter highlights otherwise forms of musical embodied learning. In this paper, we focus on an early childhood classroom music event and think diffractively with cognitive and posthuman theories in order to extend our insight into it. Accordingly, we explore cognitive approaches to music and movement, as well as posthuman concepts such as agency, embodiment, affect and desire, (de)territorialisations and assemblages. As music educators, we acknowledge the relationship between music and movement in early childhood, but our posthuman reading of the event enables a more equitable understanding of children’s music learning.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The recent posthuman turn in educational research has enabled scholars to consider topics that until recently have been left out of research, such as affect (Dernikos et al., Reference DERNIKOS, LESKO, MCCALL, NICCOLINI, Dernikos, Lesko, McCall and Niccolini2020), embodiment (Perry & Medina, Reference PERRY and MEDINA2015; Ellingson, Reference ELLINGSON2017), matter (Hickey-Moody, Reference HICKEY-MOODY2020) or a new concept of childhood (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016). The posthuman troubling of Western humanist ways of knowing, based on the mind/body binarism, opens doors to overcome (humanist) barriers and limits by constructing the world otherwise. Kuby and Rucker (Reference KUBY and RUCKER2020) use the term otherwise to highlight the non-linear, non-skill based, unexpected, equitable and relational dimensions of literacy. In educational contexts, thinking in otherwise terms involves considering ways of knowing beyond preconceived and stereotypical configurations of childhood. The questioning of binaries such as adult/child or man/nature allows a different understanding of the way in which children construct worlds with their bodies (Malone, Reference MALONE2019) and the levelling of the child with the adult, thereby acknowledging the child as a fully capable and autonomous being who should not be dominated, misjudged or alienated just because of being a child (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016). The awareness of how the non-human is intertwined with the human leads to a decentring of the human and a widening of the lens through which we look at the world. In doing so, new and suggestive perspectives emerge that allow us to view the world with a fresh gaze.

Posthumanism (along with other related poststructuralist theories, such as critical feminism or new-materialism) has provided the impetus for new perspectives on research, hence post-qualitative methodologies (MacLure, Reference MACLURE2013). The consequences can be profound, such as the immersion of the researcher in the data or the questioning of linguistic discourse as the privileged way to construct knowledge. Consequently, both posthumanism and post-qualitative methodologies open up other ways to understand research in education, in which music and sound acquire special relevance due to the affective power of sound (Gallagher, Reference GALLAGHER2016), its agential character (Dernikos, Reference DERNIKOS2020), its intertwining with movement (Hackett & Somerville, Reference HACKETT and SOMERVILLE2017) or the agentialities of silence (Wargo, Reference WARGO2018).

The idea of knowledge acquisition in childhood, understood as a linear process in which children progress from one stage to another guided by a teacher, has been denied by scholars who appeal to the nature of children’s being in the world (Hargreaves & Lamont, Reference HARGREAVES and LAMONT2017). Although enactive or phenomenological approaches to music cognition highlight the role of movement and bodies, the idea of music as cognition is seldom questioned and possesses a dominant position among educators. Music cognition as a tool for developing other skills has helped to justify its presence in the general educational system, even when these skills – memory, logic thinking, attention – perpetuate the mind/body divide and overlook the complexity of music learning (Bowman, Reference BOWMAN and Bresler2004). The adult’s attempt to order learning in a linear chronology ‘not only overlooks children’s deep entanglement with places, things, objects and atmospheres, it also assumes a particular kind of sanitized adult, capable themselves of being extracted from place, community, pain, stress, trauma and affective flow’ (Hackett, Reference HACKETT2021, 154). For music educators, attending to the multiplicity of relations, agentialities and embodiments entangled in children’s musicianship may enrich the paradigm of what counts as music education, far beyond adults’ expectations about the development of musical knowledge. Therefore, in this article, we ask ourselves about the paths opened when we read a musical event in a classroom of 5-year-olds. More specifically, we think about the following questions:

  • What kind of music relations are being overlooked if we only apply a cognitive theoretical frame?

  • How do cognitive and more-than-human readings of an event interrupt the inquiry on the (children) bodies’ entanglements?

  • Can we challenge the idea of flaw, mistake and knowledge in the school if we open our lens to the more-than-human?

This paper offers a dialogue between a posthumanist and more-than-human reading of a musical event in a classroom and a reading based on the cognitive approach that underlies current Western educational curriculums and many educators’ pedagogical practices. We understand this cognitive approach as deductive, logical and disembodied, where music owes its presence in the classroom to its equation with other objective, mind- and language-based forms of knowledge (Bowman, Reference BOWMAN and Bresler2004). To this end, we draw on theories about movement, embodiment and music understanding coming from the two theoretical perspectives of posthumanism and developmental cognition. Following Barad (Reference BARAD2007), we diffract our analysis of this event into them, seeing them not as oppositions but as ‘overlapping optical intra-actions in practice’ (Murris & Bozalek, Reference MURRIS and BOZALEK2019, 874). Murris (Reference MURRIS2016) uses the example of ‘waves of water closely rolling towards the beach, “interfering” with each other, adding to each other’s force and without clear boundaries’ (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016, 130) to illustrate how divergent ideas overlap when thinking diffractively. In our research, the two different theoretical propositions – cognitive and posthuman – diffract as we read through different concepts instead of against them (Schrader, Reference SCHRADER, Bayley and Chan2023). Our diffractions aim to show what each approach can offer to contemporary music education.

Movement and music in early childhood education: theoretical groundings for an ongoing dialogue

Music educators who draw on a cognitive paradigm have highlighted the relevance of movement and dance in the musical development of children under 8 years old. Some of them draw on Piaget’s developmental theories and his proposition that children learn through their bodies or on the kinetics intelligence proposed by Gardner, to argue the benefits of associating movement and dance in educational contexts (Minton & Faber, Reference MINTON and FABER2016). As noted by Hargreaves and Lamont (Reference HARGREAVES and LAMONT2017), movement has been proposed as a tool to analyse children’s responses to music stimuli, as an indicator of children’s engagement with music (Brown, Reference BROWN and McPherson2016) and as a more complete way to relate to music (Davis, Reference DAVIS and McPherson2016). The multiple benefits of movement and dance in education include cognitive, emotional and social advances, based on the principle that motor development is essential to other aspects of development. Koff (Reference KOFF2000) suggests that movement and dance may help young children to use up their energy in the classroom and prepare them for moving on to more static tasks. Although movement seems to be a powerful resource in practice- and phenomenology-based music education, Bowman and Powell (Reference BOWMAN, POWELL and Bresler2007) have pointed out the overall failure to integrate an embodied perspective fully in the classroom.

Researchers tend to differentiate dance and movement, depending on the level of organisation and complexity. Dance has been defined as a highly organised, pre-designed sequence of movements, whereas movement tends to be seen in a more general way, associated with improvisation and freedom (Faber, Reference FABER2017). Other conceptualisations have also been proposed in educational contexts (Wiens, Reference WIENS2015). Martín Escobar (Reference MARTÍN ESCOBAR2005) also describes two kinds of movement in the early childhood classroom, namely rhythmic dances and songs with gestures. The latter have been frequently used to represent the meaning of lyrics (Paquette & Rieg, Reference PAQUETTE and RIEG2008) and to improve linguistic skills, such as vocabulary acquisition and retention (Coyle & Gómez Gracia, Reference COYLE and GÓMEZ GRACIA2014). Faber (Reference FABER2017) transcends the music–gesture association in linguistic development when she proposes free movement as a facilitator of children’s cognitive and spatial skills and as the provider of a system of symbols prior to language development. In this process, dance becomes an embodiment of complex and resourceful thought, which extends its impact to a wide range of cognitive and social skills (Minton & Faber, Reference MINTON and FABER2016).

The link between movement and language development resonates with research about the benefits of music education for cognitive and linguistic skills. Although the degree of association between music training and language and literacy skills development varies, some research points to the benefits of music training for improving the level of reading in children (Slater et al., Reference SLATER, TIERNEY and KRAUS2014). For children aged 3–5, Mullen (Reference MULLEN2015) reported the benefits of nursery rhymes in developmental areas such as physical health, language and cognition or social competence.

Bowman and Powell (Reference BOWMAN, POWELL and Bresler2007) have discussed music educators’ oblivion to human bodies as knowledge. Although phenomenologists may regard bodies as ways of knowledge, posthuman and new materialist approaches level the mind and body (Braidotti, Reference BRAIDOTTI2019). This levelling (Snaza et al., Reference SNAZA, SONU, TRUMAN, ZALIWSKA, Snaza, Sonu, Truman and Zaliwska2016) offers the possibility of no longer seeing bodies and movement as the product of a superior mind; rather, they have been regarded as central in children’s meaning-making and learning (Hackett & Somerville, Reference HACKETT and SOMERVILLE2017). Matter, as stated by Snaza and Sonu (Reference SNAZA, SONU, Snaza, Sonu, Truman and Zaliwska2016, 32), is active and productive, lending value to the ‘multitude of systems and forces that engender a specific scenario or condition’. Music and sound, as matter, participate in such meaning-making (Powell & Somerville, Reference POWELL and SOMERVILLE2020), where movement arises not as a quality but as a means of signification that defines the contouring, forces and intensities of bodies (Manning, Reference MANNING2014).

More-than-human bodies involve the human and the non-human (Hackett, Reference HACKETT2021) and come-to-be in their relations and assemblages. Desires, intensities and affect constitute sets of relations in which bodies come to be (Buchanan, Reference BUCHANAN2021). Assemblages, from this perspective, are not the sum of bodies but their operational network defined by their dynamic relations, understood as interactions coming from the inside (Guyotte et al., Reference GUYOTTE, FLINT, KIDD, POTTS, IRWIN and BENNETT2020). Assemblages, being productive and mobile, have been described as ongoing processes of merging and branching of matter, discourses and social dynamics (Daniels, Reference DANIELS2021a). Moments in which movement flourishes are full of flows of affect, multiplicities and emergence, especially when sound is part of these relations (Borovica, Reference BOROVICA2019). The affective power of sound has been described by Gallagher (Reference GALLAGHER2016) and Dernikos et al. (Reference DERNIKOS, LESKO, MCCALL, NICCOLINI, Dernikos, Lesko, McCall and Niccolini2020), highlighting its role in more-than-human assemblages.

The iteration of assemblages creates territories, understood as fixed, predictable and normative structures (Deleuze & Guattari, Reference DELEUZE and GUATTARI1987). Far beyond a limited spatial meaning, territories are made up of heterogeneous elements and contain an internal organisation that tends to preserve their own principles and structures (Parr, Reference PARR2010). Territories may be challenged by lines of flight, moves that trouble the well-known routines of territories, and create new and unexpected deterritorialised assemblages (Deleuze & Guattari, Reference DELEUZE and GUATTARI1987). Music, as both agential matter and art, is ‘capable of that destruction and deformation that destroys territories and enables them to revert to the chaos from which they were temporarily wrenched’ (Grosz, Reference GROSZ2008, 13).

From paying attention to interrupting: a methodological journey

The data we analyse in this paper were gathered in the context of a wider research project in the south of Spain. The children’s parents were aware of the research’s aims and scope and signed a written informed consent agreement, according to the policies of the University of Sevilla. All the names in this article are pseudonyms. This project applied an ethnographic collaborative approach (Pahl et al., Reference PAHL, STEADMAN-JONES and VASUDEVAN2022), where data gathering was based on observation, field notes and audio-visual recordings. Our observation cannot be considered either objective or neutral, for we accept our presence in the classroom as part of its assemblage. In this being-in-the-world, we apply post-qualitative research techniques (St. Pierre, Reference ST. PIERRE2019), where the qualitative ‘paying attention’ is challenged by the post-qualitative ‘interrupting’ (Malone & Murris, Reference MALONE, MURRIS and Murris2022, 145).

The current paper draws on specific data belonging to a session in which a group of 22 five-year-olds, their teacher, Edith, and Alejandra, one of the researchers, were all present. The research team had been working in the field with this group weekly for 2 years, so the presence of the researcher, sitting on a children’s chair in the circle of children, wasn’t seen as intrusive. The event, one where the class listens to a song selected by Edith, was recorded by Alejandra using her mobile phone. Therefore, we, as researchers, watched and, at the same time, gathered as much data as our lens determined was worthy of analysis.

Our analysis is structured in two layers in which we address each of Malone and Murris’ propositions (Reference MALONE, MURRIS and Murris2022). In the first layer, we ‘pay attention’ to our data and read it from a cognitive perspective on music, movement and educational purpose. In the second layer, we explore the possibilities opened when we ‘interrupt’ and consider ourselves as agents in the data (Ellingson & Sotirin, Reference ELLINGSON and SOTIRIN2020). Hence, our data are no longer a raw material waiting for our shaping but a dynamic construction in which the decentred human operates together with human and non-human beings (Gullion, Reference GULLION2018).

We think with theory in our analysis, and we engage with Jackson and Mazzei’s (Reference JACKSON and MAZZEI2023) analysis of texts as a ‘collective assemblage of enunciation’ (12), following Deleuze and Guattari (Reference DELEUZE and GUATTARI1987). Our research faces the contingency and movement of scientific knowledge when thinking with two theoretical paradigms, where their voices entangle and build ‘a brief arrest’ (Jackson & Mazzei, Reference JACKSON and MAZZEI2023, 12). In this engagement, we connect theoretical conceptualisations with the performative narration of a vignette.

A performative narration understands the vignette not as a representation of reality but as a causal recreation of a collectivity. In other words, ‘as performative, the accounts are repetitions that do not simply repeat the past but bring something different into existence’ (Jackson & Mazzei, Reference JACKSON and MAZZEI2023, 13). In consequence, our vignette cannot be considered as data but as an assemblage that merges agents such as the researchers, the traces of the experience in the vignette, the children’s movements or the educational research (Ellingson, Reference ELLINGSON2017). In this process, the vignette and its analysis contain our own material footprint and our worlding, and we are agents in the process of data co-construction. Following St. Pierre (Reference ST. PIERRE2019), we analyse the assemblage of agents (including theoretical conceptualisations) in writing, noting its connections with the researchers and our past research, as well as their cooperation with the vignette, thereby creating a new field in educational research (Jackson & Mazzei, Reference JACKSON and MAZZEI2023). The two-layered reading (cognitive and posthuman) offered in this paper enables us to zoom in on relations and interactions that are implicit in our classroom sessions with the participants and to deepen the role of the researchers who are entangled with/through the researched materialities (MacLure, Reference MACLURE2016).

Music on the move: two vignettes

Our vignettes recall the moment in which Edith, the teacher, carries out a musical listening activity in the classroom. The song is part of an educational toolbox she uses in the classroom, focused on the topic ‘Mystery in the Museum’ and consisted of a book, notebook, worksheets, songs and crafts. Through them, the children are expected to develop a series of skills and learnings.

1. What’s the song about?

It is the end of January, and the children have been working with the toolbox since they came back from the Christmas holidays, three weeks before. The children have learnt this song in the previous days through listening to it, dancing and singing.

The children are sitting cross-legged on the cork floor of the classroom, forming the circle of the assembly (Figure 1). The teacher, Edith, announces they are going to listen to the song. Dora claps. Whispers and murmurs similar to the song reach my ears. Nigel moves his head up and down, and Julia, cross-legged, sways. Susan and other children are humming the chorus of the song. Edith asks Laura: ‘How many words are in the song’s title?’ Before Edith finishes her question, Dora looks at her and says: ‘I know the song.’ ‘Dora, what’s the song about?’ asks Edith. Silence. Fiona looks at Dora moving her finger, mouthing and swaying. Edith changes her question and asks, ‘Does anybody remember the song?’ Duncan exclaims ‘Me, me!’ When Edith gives the floor to Duncan, he starts singing the song. Edith explains with slower, emphasised words: ‘I am not asking you to sing the song, but to tell us what’s the song about, what happens in the song’. Duncan shrugs and says: ‘Don’t know’. His knees are swinging.

Figure 1. What’s the song about?

2. Do not dance, we are listening to the song

Edith keeps encouraging the children to talk about the content of the song. She talks about a missing painting and a papyrus – words that are familiar to the children and that they repeat with conviction. Edith considers it necessary to listen to the song again in order to achieve the purpose of her activity.

‘Do not dance, we are listening to the song, because I think we are not…’. Duncan is bouncing, cross-legged. Fiona and Dora are moving too, swinging their bodies cross-legged. Julia claps. The music starts. Dora almost stands up, with one leg in front of her. Edith says: ‘We sit. Laura; we are listening to the lyrics of the song, the lyrics’. Dora’s arms keep swinging. The movement gradually fades out, losing momentum. Izzy repeats ‘The lyrics’. From my position on one of the children’s chairs, I can see a cluster of moving knees and arms, soft voices singing. More children are joining this community of movement (Figure 2). Duncan watches Dora and Izzy move and sing on their knees. Duncan suddenly goes on his knees, singing and pointing with his fingers. Susan’s moves mimic the characters and lyrics of the video. Julia’s slapping on her knees follows the beat while Nigel sings the song and crawls around. Nigel and Duncan throw themselves on the floor when the song ends. Some girls discuss how to produce the finger clicks in the song.

Figure 2. Do not dance.

Thinking with theory in music education: an otherwise analysis

The everyday activity of listening to a song in a classroom, as the culmination of a series of activities before moving onto another set of tasks, confronted us with a twofold reading. When we (Alejandra, José and Fernando) went back to our recordings, we, as music educators, took into account different musical outputs, such as the children’s coordination with music, their pitch accuracy or their rhythmic development. We also noticed their failure when they were not able to extract the meaning of the lyrics or to keep still while they listened. However, our posthuman research with this same group of children moved us to think differently and notice the material agentialities and affects emerging in the event. These simultaneous readings cohabit as diffractions in our approach to this complex event, in the same way in which they coexist in ourselves as researchers and educators.

‘Paying attention’ to movement, music and song: a cognitive reading

Edith’s approach to the song is to listen to the lyrics without dancing. However, as the children listen to the music, they move different parts of their bodies, in line with the idea that movement is an inherent part of children’s musical experience (Abril, Reference ABRIL, Colwell and Webster2011) and that music is learned in the relationship between body and mind (Philpott, Reference PHILPOTT and McPherson2016). Some of the children move while sitting on the floor, or sit up, or are even about to stand up. These movements are created by the children in a free and exploratory way (Abril, Reference ABRIL, Colwell and Webster2011). Through them, they display their understanding of musical discourse (rhythm, pitch, structure) (Faber, Reference FABER2017), which is similar to other suggestions for the use of movement in music. Children in our vignettes show age-appropriate skills in their movements, such as clapping to the beat or developing fine psychomotor skills when clicking their fingers (Kenney, Reference KENNEY2008). In this sense, children’s movement becomes a form of active listening in which they identify rhythmic, melodic and other formal patterns (Gault, Reference GAULT2016).

The teacher attributes a series of utilities to the song that transcend the merely musical and seek to develop cognitive skills through musical practice. This attribution is based on the theory that ‘whether children listen to the “music” of the rain, popular children’s songs, or make their own musical compositions, important skills, such as reading, writing, listening, and speaking, are developed’ (Paquette & Rieg, Reference PAQUETTE and RIEG2008, 231). Thus, Edith asks the children how many words are in the title of the song or what the song is about. However, in our vignettes, none of the children can respond adequately to Edith’s questions, even when they know how to count and have already listened to the song several times in previous sessions. In this sense, although there are studies that demonstrate the benefits of music education on children’s different cognitive skills (Schellenberg, Reference SCHELLENBERG and McPherson2016), Rauscher and Hinton (Reference RAUSCHER and HINTON2011) stress that these effects are of undetermined duration, require further elaboration and should not restrict music education to only serving extra-musical cognitive development. What happens in the classroom is related to studies in which children of similar ages to ours, after a music-based intervention, improved their phonological awareness but not their ability to identify letters (Gromko, Reference GROMKO2005), or in which the intervention on children of similar ages to ours did not show an improvement in vocabulary (Mehr et al., Reference MEHR, SCHACHNER, KATZ and SPELKE2013).

The children’s claim to know the song responds to their understanding of it (as seen in their movements, reactions or when Fiona mouths the song to her partner) in terms of the rhythm, tempo, melody or movements associated with the song and its lyrics (as happens to Duncan). This spontaneous response to music, even before it starts to sound, relates to musical practice as a form of meaning-making (Campbell, Reference CAMPBELL and Barrett2011) and to the individual and collective musical agency of the children (Karlsen, Reference KARLSEN2011). The children in our vignettes construct their understanding of music (when they say they know the song) in a holistic way, without separating the lyrics from the music, and through movement (Minton & Faber, Reference MINTON and FABER2016). As Philpott (Reference PHILPOTT and McPherson2016, 198) states, ‘the importance of the child’s body in music literacy should come as no surprise, that is, these meaning-making processes are rooted in the dynamic body’. In this sense, Philpott (Reference PHILPOTT and McPherson2016) claims that music is the paradigm of human cognition (199) in its merging of mind and body, thinking and perception.

‘Interruption’ as knowledge: a posthuman reading

The corked assemblage of the assembly

As we (Alejandra, José and Fernando) come back to the video recordings and rewrite the vignettes, we feel the need to consider the different relations entangled in it. Boldt (Reference BOLDT2021) sets out how a more-than-human approach allows us to rethink school spaces as assemblages and entanglements. Our event’s assemblage is made up of the children, their material world, the song when they sing it or the floor, as they crawl around or slide on it. The cork floor is no longer a defined space for them to move on but an active agent in the set of relations intertwining in the classroom. We feel how it offers an invitation for Duncan and Nigel to slide on it at the end of the song, in the same way as it embraces the children as they move and sway. Accepting these elements as an assemblage allows us to delve into the intricacy of the moment through the material (Daniels, Reference DANIELS2021a), where human and non-human bodies are levelled.

The children-song-cork assemblage exists insofar as all its elements converge and intertwine, none of them being more important than the others. They emerge in a non-child-centred ‘relation between a child and their sonic, embodied, mobile entanglements with a place – entanglements that resist the image of a sovereign human child’ (Land, Reference LAND2022, 74). The more-than-human desires in this assemblage (Buchanan, Reference BUCHANAN2021) act on/with the children who are unable to stop moving. The human desires in the event (not to move, listening while still) cohabit and are levelled with the matter’s desires (the song or the floor). Hence, we do not see the children as refusing to obey their teacher; rather, we view them as an assemblage in which the decentred children come-to-be in their interweaving with the more-than-human world.

The unpredictability of movements and agentialities in our event recalls the randomness of more-than-human bodies and affects (Hackett & Rautio, Reference HACKETT and RAUTIO2019). The song affects the children’s bodies to the same degree as the floor or Alejandra’s mobile phone’s camera. The children’s movements emerge in their relations with music and exist with the song, and the song comes-to-be in the children’s movements. Affects vibrate and resonate in the more-than-human bodies (Gershon, Reference GERSHON2020), triggering Fiona’s movements even before the song begins. The classroom assemblage is not just the sum of its parts but the agentialities of its elements (DeLanda, Reference DELANDA2006), their desires and affects. Reading our vignettes as an assemblage challenges a human-centred perspective, in which human beings organise the world according to their will. Music, as matter, is an agent and triggers desires and affects far beyond human intentions.

Embodied listening as thinking

Children are asked by the teacher to remain still while they listen to the song when they are told to ‘listen, not to dance’. Her reminders throughout our vignettes resonate with other ways of controlling movement and with a certain way of listening (‘Do not dance’, ‘we are listening to the song’). Edith, as part of the educational system, controls what movements are allowed in the classroom (Kirby, Reference KIRBY2020). The permitted movements are those that do not involve the children shifting their positions, and therefore, Edith allows children to sway and to move their head or finger. Edith and Alejandra’s bodies are part of this adult way of listening when they sit still and silently listen to the song. However, as the song goes on, the children’s bodies challenge these rules by becoming sliders on the floor or almost-standing dancers. Each body relates to/with the music independently, with its own desires and affects, and the adult’s homogeneous command is unable to grasp the network of happenstances unfolding in the assemblage. As stated by Hackett and Somerville (Reference HACKETT and SOMERVILLE2017), children’s embodiments become a way of being embedded in/with the world.

The aim of listening to the song without moving implies a mind/body divide that utterly defies the children’s way of relating to music. As MacRae et al. (Reference MACRAE, HACKETT, HOLMES and JONES2018, 511) explain, ‘when young children think in movement, the notion of intentionality does not work because firstly, thinking is happening through the body’. The children in our vignettes think through embodied movement (Fullagar, Reference FULLAGAR and Murris2021), and their movements and answers to Edith are part of the same unified nature of their body–mind relations (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016). The aims of counting words, explaining the lyrics’ meaning or listening still respond to a split of the cognitive mind and the sensitive body. However, when asked about the song, Duncan answers with the song itself, embracing the tune, its lyrics and their embodiment, in which movement is the continuous state of bodies (Daniels, Reference DANIELS2021b).

The aim of using the song for a linguistic task is therefore troubled by the consideration of ‘sound, vibration and resonance (music) as part of the material experience of children’ in their learning (Powell & Somerville, Reference POWELL and SOMERVILLE2020, 845). The song is part of the onto-epistemological way of being/knowing/doing in childhood, where mind (listening) and body (moving) merge (Bowman & Powell Reference BOWMAN, POWELL and Bresler2007). Lyrics are always sung and embodied (Hackett, Reference HACKETT2022), not thought out, and the title of the song is not just a sum of words but an assemblage of melody, rhythm, phonemes and movements.

Children’s deterritorialisation of adult’s intentionalities

Territories are created in the classroom assemblages through reiteration, routines and rituals (Brownell, Reference BROWNELL2019). The classroom assembly is a daily-created territory where children know what to expect, and routines are dictated by the teacher and delivered to the children as oral instructions, use of spaces and furniture or visual displays. The children in this territory may be considered to be knowledge consumers waiting for instructions, questions and information. Nonetheless, the material agency of the song is intertwined with the affects and desires of the moving bodies, and joy troubles the muted territories of the school (Gershon, Reference GERSHON2018). As Dernikos (Reference DERNIKOS2020) proposes, embodied affects can create lines of flight such as those in Fiona’s or Duncan’s movements, deterritorialising the striated spaces of the well-known. The territory of the (silent/still) listening in the assembly is deterritorialised in the assemblage of (sounding) music and movement. The expectation, murmurs and whispers when Edith announces the song, the bodies’ emerging movements or Fiona’s moving fingers even before the song starts to play highlight the bodies’ entanglement with the imagined matter of children’s embodiments (Daniels, Reference DANIELS2021b).

The Mystery at the Museum song in the assembly territory is imbued with a pedagogical intentionality, being part of a lesson plan that finds its purpose in the school territory. The chronologically ordered tasks – listening, counting, explaining – draw on a conceptualisation of teaching in which one step leads to another, fulfilling a final aim, thereby supporting a child in need of guidance and support for their learning (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016). However, this linearity of learning in the school’s territory, based on linguistic discourse, is deterritorialised in the sounding embodiments emerging in/with the cork floor that underpins the impossibility of linear learning in early childhood (Hackett, Reference HACKETT2021). The song, as agential matter, has become a ‘willful object’ (Dernikos, Reference DERNIKOS2018, 6) that denies the likelihood of meaning-making in silent and still minds separated from bodies. Wilful more-than-human bodies flee alien purposes imposed on them and show, in their reluctance, the exercise of power in institutions (Ahmed, Reference AHMED2014). Therefore, the song, bodies and movements challenge linear learnings through their emergence and their recall of the embodied/imagined/assembled children’s bodies.

The privilege given to linguistic/still/silent ways of knowledge in the school, over embodied/emergent/wilful matter, dwells in an adult/child binarism (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016). Our reading of the event shows how the children’s thinking bodies emerge in/through/with their relations with the world (Somerville & Powell, Reference SOMERVILLE and POWELL2019). They create unexpected lines of flight that trouble human-centred mind/body binaries and human/child intentionalities. Music in our vignettes cannot be understood as a means of serving territorialised adult ends, such as listening or counting. As a material agent, music comes-to-be in the deterritorialised, more-than-human assemblage (Daniels, Reference DANIELS2021b).

Conclusions

Paying attention to the relationality of sound, space or children invites us to rethink childhood from a material perspective that troubles an understanding of childhood from biological and psychological paradigms. In our research, a minor event is read from two different theoretical propositions on childhood, music learning and the school. These readings propose divergent constructs on childhood as a social, cultural and political artefact, raising issues about the limits of cognitive and developmental approaches to music and posing alternatives based on the materiality of sound and music. Our inquiry highlights different understandings of research on/with children and questions a universalising view of learning based on the assessment of skills (Malone et al., Reference MALONE, TESAR and ARNDT2020). In the pursuit of alternatives to research based on cognitive and developmental propositions, posthuman pedagogies (Snaza et al., Reference SNAZA, SONU, TRUMAN, ZALIWSKA, Snaza, Sonu, Truman and Zaliwska2016) open paths to challenge the linearity of learning and to consider how sound affects and is affected in the classroom (Dernikos et al., Reference DERNIKOS, LESKO, MCCALL, NICCOLINI, Dernikos, Lesko, McCall and Niccolini2020). Posthuman pedagogies attend to an otherwise ethic-onto-epistemology (Barad, Reference BARAD2007) based on material relationalities that reposition children and childhood (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016). Therefore, our research proposes posthuman pedagogies as divergent forms of understanding our event, delving into the emergence of its agents and aiming to avoid the constraints of cognitive approaches to music education.

Thinking with theory as a method for analysis (Jackson & Mazzei, Reference JACKSON and MAZZEI2023) implies that researchers do not know the questions or the answers beforehand. At the beginning of our inquiry, we asked ourselves about the possibilities offered by our diffractive reading of our vignette, but we were also open to unexpected answers arising as we read with theory (Jackson & Mazzei, Reference JACKSON and MAZZEI2023).

The relations overlooked by applying only a cognitive approach in our analysis was our first consideration. In our cognitive analysis, we explored movement as a natural reaction to music and its use as a support for developing non-musical skills, such as language. However, the children in our vignette were unable to answer their teacher’s questions, and a posthuman perspective has highlighted the impossibility of separating mind and body. The song, as matter, acts in/on/with the more-than-human bodies in an unpredictable way, different for each child. These bodies include sound, the floor or the relations of children and adults, entangled in a boundless assemblage of enacted, unique and emergent agencies. Each assemblage is unique and emergent and comes-to-be in the more-than-human bodies. Teachers cannot foresee the affects and desires assembling in classroom situations, but the dismissal of these deterritorialisations ignores the ways in which children come to know music by being, knowing and doing.

Developmental cognitive approaches to music education accept movement as a natural way to experience music and to develop motor and cognitive skills. We therefore asked ourselves what our posthuman reading added to this perspective. Humanistic and cognitive theories of movement generally draw on the mind–body divide, emphasising movement as representation (Minton & Faber, Reference MINTON and FABER2016). A posthuman reading challenges these meaning–symbol divides in its conceptualisation of our event as an assemblage, emerging in relations, affects and desires. In an assemblage, knowledge in music is constructed through an entanglement of bodies, sound and matter. Relegating music to support other areas of learning hinders children’s possibilities to discover the world and create meaning through aural sensoriality (Shannon, Reference SHANNON2020). Therefore, we question our vignettes’ adult intentionality of dividing the music discourse into sections, privileging the contents’ lyrics over the tune or the embodiment. From a posthuman perspective, such a divide fails because of its neglect of the children’s embodied ways to know music, where mind and body, music and lyrics, merge in an entangled form of musicianship.

Thinking with posthuman theories challenges the idea of songs as representational experiences unconnected to affects and embodiments. It also makes us wonder what may be understood as mistakes and knowledge in school music practices. As we saw in our vignettes, music’s embodied knowledge does not always align with the aims of adults/teachers. In the continuous dynamic of (de)territorialisations, children’s learning may flee pre-determined pedagogical goals. The song in our vignette is children’s knowledge that comes-to-be in the assemblage, where adults fail in the attempt to break it down into single elements governed by a linguistic logic (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016). Attending to sound and music as entangled with bodies and matter ‘create an invitation to explore and learn in ways that are valuable and meaningful to our students’ (Cooke, Reference COOKE2024, 315). Regarded from and adult-led cognitive and disembodied point of view, the children have not fulfilled the tasks they were set. As we move from an adult-centred and humanist lens towards a child-centred one in which children are not regarded as less human just for the mere reason of being children (Murris, Reference MURRIS2016), we begin to consider these mistakes as world-forming. A posthuman focus on children as part of a more-than-human world enables a wider and fairer understanding of the school’s practices, where the children integrate into material assemblages (Hackett, Reference HACKETT2022). Attention to the materiality of music in the school could lead to a new curriculum, one that is attuned to childhood ways of knowing that include otherwise ways of being/doing/knowing music (Snaza et al., Reference SNAZA, SONU, TRUMAN, ZALIWSKA, Snaza, Sonu, Truman and Zaliwska2016).

The implications of our research point to the necessity of reading music learning from different theoretical paradigms, given that the ways in which we build our conceptualisations of childhood are intimately interwoven with our pedagogical practices. Hence, our research proposes different readings on children and music in order to attend the complexity of the classroom events (Malone et al., Reference MALONE, TESAR and ARNDT2020). By doing so, researching on sound and music in childhood education becomes open and unpredictable. Our posthuman perspective has enabled us to approach the otherwise ways in which children learn and build worlds as they entangle with sound and matter. In this sense, posthuman pedagogies are sensitive to learning based on material relationalities, affects, material agencies and the more-than-human assemblages that decentre the human in emergent and unpredictable events. As pointed out by Snaza et al. (Reference SNAZA, APPELBAUM, BAYNE, CARLSON, MORRIS, ROTAS, SANDLIN, WALLIN and WAVER2014), research on childhood should become an open science that facilitates the challenge to cognitive practices in education.

Acknowledgements

This paper is part of the R&D&I project PID2019-104557GB-I00, funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033/, and the European Union ‘NextGenerationEU’ funds, through the Recovery, Transformation and Resilience Plan and the Ministry of Universities, within the framework of the Support for the Requalification of the Spanish University 2021–2023, and the R&D&I project PID2021-127124OB-I00, funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033/and by ‘ERDF A Way of Doing Europe’. Special recognition to Universidad Loyola Andalucía, in gratitude for the Research Assistant grant that made possible this research. We thank the children, parents and teachers who have taken part in this research, for their collaboration and trust, and Dr Hilary McQueen for her careful review of the text.

Competing interests

The authors report there are no competing interests to declare.

Alejandra Pacheco-Costa is currently a senior lecturer in music education in the Arts Education Department at the University of Sevilla (Spain). She has been a guest lecturer at Lancaster University (United Kingdom), the University of Ruse (Bulgaria), the University of Costa Rica and the University Pablo de Olavide (Spain). She has developed research on informal learning in music education, music and literacy and instrumental teaching in Spain in London (Institute of Education, now UCL Institute of Education, University of London), New York (Foundation for Iberian Music, CUNY), Costa Rica (INIE, University of Costa Rica) and Switzerland (Hochschule für Musik, Basel). She is a corresponding member of the Lancaster Literacy Research Centre and has published her research in books, monographs and scientific journals such as History of Education, Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, Journal of Early Childhood Research or Music Education Research, among others.

José Juan Roa-Trejo is a lecturer in music education in the Communication and Education Department at the Loyola University Andalucía. At this university, he teaches music education and teaching classes in the primary education and early childhood education degrees and also coordinates the bachelor’s degree in music creation and production. His research tries to delve into the definition of multimodal literacy. Through it, he seeks to describe how literacy practices related to sound and music are created and how children intra-act with sound to create meaning. This interest in the material/discursive power of sound brings him to participate in different international conferences and projects related to literacy, music and innovative educational practices.

Fernando Guzmán-Simón obtained his PhD in Spanish Language and Literature at the University of Seville (Spain), where he is a senior lecturer in the Department of Language Education. He has published several research papers on the assessment of academic writing in Spanish, as well as literacy practices in early childhood and primary education. At present, his research is framed in a broader project focused on the development of multimodal literacy in childhood from a posthuman and post-qualitative approach. This project aims to analyse the literacy events of students and their families and how they create mattering through human and more-than-human’s intra-actions.

References

ABRIL, C. R. (2011). Music, movement, and learning. In Colwell, R. & Webster, P. (eds.), MENC Handbook of Research on Music Learning, vol. 2: Applications (pp. 92129). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
AHMED, S. (2014). Willful Subjects. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
BARAD, K. (2007). Meeting the Universe Halfway: Quantum Physics and the Entanglement of Matter and Meaning. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BOLDT, G. (2021). Theorizing vitality in the literacy classroom. Reading Research Quarterly, 56(2), 207221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BOROVICA, T. (2019). Dancing the strata: Investigating affective flows of moving/dancing bodies in the exploration of bodily (un)becoming. Qualitative Inquiry, 25(1), 2636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BOWMAN, W. (2004). Cognition and the body: Perspectives from music education. In Bresler, L. (ed.), Knowing Bodies, Moving Minds. Towards Embodied Teaching and Learning (pp. 2950). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BOWMAN, W. & POWELL, K. (2007). The body in a state of music. In Bresler, L. (ed.), International Handbook of Research in Arts Education (pp. 10871106). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BRAIDOTTI, R. (2019). A theoretical framework for the critical posthumanities. Theory, Culture and Society, 36(6), 3161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BROWN, A. R. (2016). Engaging in a sound musicianship. In McPherson, G. E. (ed.), The Child as Musician. A Handbook for Musical Development (2nd ed., pp. 208220). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
BROWNELL, C. J. (2019). Sound the alarm!: Disrupting sonic resonances of an elementary English language arts classroom. Curriculum Inquiry, 49(5), 551572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BUCHANAN, I. (2021). Assemblage Theory and Method: An Introduction and Guide. London, UK: Bloomsbury Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CAMPBELL, P. S. (2011). Musical enculturation: Sociocultural influences and meanings of children’s experiences in and through music. In Barrett, M. S. (ed.), A Cultural Psychology of Music Education (pp. 6181). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
COOKE, C. (2024). Reverberations in music education: What does sound ‘do’ in our learning spaces? Music Education Research, 26(3), 304317.Google Scholar
COYLE, Y. & GÓMEZ GRACIA, R. (2014). Using songs to enhance L2 vocabulary acquisition in preschool children. ELT Journal, 68(3), 276285.Google Scholar
DANIELS, K. (2021a). Notions of agency in early literacy classrooms: Assemblages and productive intersections. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 21(4), 568589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DANIELS, K. (2021b). Movement, meaning and affect and young children’s early literacy practices. International Journal of Early Years Education, 29(1), 4155.Google Scholar
DAVIS, S. G. (2016). Children, popular music, and identity. In McPherson, G. E. (ed.), The Child as Musician. A Handbook for Musical Development (2nd ed, pp. 265283). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DELANDA, M. (2006). A New Philosophy of Society: Assemblage Theory and Social Complexity. London, UK: Continuum.Google Scholar
DELEUZE, G. & GUATTARI, F. (1987). A Thousand Plateaux. Capitalism and Schizophrenia. London, UK: Continuum.Google Scholar
DERNIKOS, B. P. (2018). ‘It’s like you don’t want to read it again’: Exploring affects, trauma and ‘willful’ literacies. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 0(0). https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798418756187.Google Scholar
DERNIKOS, B. P. (2020). Tuning into ‘fleshy’ frequencies: A posthuman mapping of affect, sound and de/colonized literacies with/in a primary classroom. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 20(1), 134157.Google Scholar
DERNIKOS, B. P., LESKO, N., MCCALL, S. D. & NICCOLINI, A. D. (2020). Feeling education. In Dernikos, B. P., Lesko, N., McCall, S. D. & Niccolini, A. D. (eds.), Mapping the Affective Turn in Education. Theory, Research, and Pedagogies (pp. 327). New York, USA: Routledge.Google Scholar
ELLINGSON, L. L. (2017). Embodiment in Qualitative Research. New York, USA: Routledge.Google Scholar
ELLINGSON, L. L. & SOTIRIN, P. (2020). Making Data in Qualitative Research. Engagement, Ethics, and Entanglements. New York, USA: Routledge.Google Scholar
FABER, R. (2017). Dance and early childhood cognition: The Isadora effect. Arts Education Policy Review, 118(3), 172182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FULLAGAR, S. (2021). Re-turning to embodied matters and movement. In Murris, K. (ed.), Navigating the Postqualitative, New Materialist and Critical Posthumanist Terrain Across Disciplines (pp. 117124). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
GALLAGHER, M. (2016). Sound as affect: Difference, power and spatiality. Emotion, Space and Society, 20, 4248.Google Scholar
GAULT, B. M. (2016). Listen Up! Fostering Musicianship through Active Listening. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
GERSHON, W. S. (2018). Sound Curriculum. Sonic Studies in Educational Theory, Method & Practice. New York, USA: Routledge.Google Scholar
GERSHON, W. S. (2020). Reverberations and reverb: Sound possibilities for narrative, creativity, and critique. Qualitative Inquiry, 26(10), 11631173.Google Scholar
GROMKO, J. E. (2005). The effect of music instruction on phonemic awareness in beginning readers. Journal of Research in Music Education, 53(3), 199209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GROSZ, E. (2008). Chaos, Territory, Art. Deleuze and the Framing of the Earth. New York, USA: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
GULLION, J. S. (2018). Diffractive Ethnography. Social Sciences and the Ontological Turn. New York, USA: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
GUYOTTE, K. W., FLINT, M. A., KIDD, B. G., POTTS, G. A., IRWIN, A. J. & BENNETT, L. A. (2020). Meanwhile: Posthuman intra-actions in/with a post-qualitative readings class. Qualitative Inquiry, 26(1), 109121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HACKETT, A. (2021). More-Than-Human Literacies in Early Childhood. London, UK: Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
HACKETT, A. (2022). Beyond transparency: More-than-human insights into the emergence of young children’s language. Literacy, 56(3), 244252.Google Scholar
HACKETT, A. & RAUTIO, P. (2019). Answering the world: Young children’s running and rolling as more-than-human multimodal meaning making. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 32(8), 10191031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HACKETT, A. & SOMERVILLE, M. (2017). Posthuman literacies: Young children moving in time, place and more-than-human worlds. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 17(3), 374391.Google Scholar
HARGREAVES, D. & LAMONT, A. (2017). The Psychology of Musical Development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
HICKEY-MOODY, A. C. (2020). New materialism, ethnography, and socially engaged practice: Space-time folds and the agency of matter. Qualitative Inquiry, 26(7), 724732.Google Scholar
JACKSON, A. Y. & MAZZEI, L. A. (2023) Thinking with Theory in Qualitative Research. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
KARLSEN, S. (2011). Using musical agency as a lens: Researching music education from the angle of experience. Research Studies in Music Education, 33(2), 107121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KENNEY, S. (2008). Birth to six. General Music Today, 22(1), 3234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KIRBY, P. (2020). Marshmallow claps and frozen children: Sitting on the carpet in the modern ‘on-task’ primary classroom. Pedagogy, Culture & Society, 28(3), 445461.Google Scholar
KOFF, S. R. (2000). Toward a definition of dance education. Childhood Education, 77(1), 2732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
KUBY, C. R. & RUCKER, T. G. (2020). (Re)Thinking children as fully (in)human and literacies as otherwise through (re)etymologizing intervene and inequality. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 20(1), 1343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LAND, N. (2022). Movement belongs to all of us? Thinking interdisciplinarity with early childhood studies and kinesiology. Journal of Pedagogy, 13(1), 7187.Google Scholar
MACLURE, M. (2013). The wonder of data. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies, 13(4), 228232.Google Scholar
MACLURE, M. (2016). The refrain if the a-grammatical child: Finding another language in/for qualitative research. Cultural Studies ↔ Critical Methodologies, 16(2), 173182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MACRAE, C., HACKETT, A., HOLMES, R. & JONES, L. (2018). Vibrancy, repetition and movement: Posthuman theories for reconceptualising young children in museums. Children’s Geographies, 16(5), 503515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MALONE, K. (2019). Worlding with Kin: Diffracting childfish sensorial ecological encounters through moving image. Video Journal of Education and Pedagogy, 4(1), 6980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MALONE, K. & MURRIS, K. (2022). Worlding. In Murris, K. (ed.), A Glossary for Doing Postqualitative, New Materialist and Critical Posthumanist Research Across Disciplines (pp. 144145). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
MALONE, K., TESAR, M. & ARNDT, S. (2020). Theorising Posthuman Childhood Studies. Singapore, Singapore: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MANNING, E. (2014). Wondering the world directly – or, how movement outruns the subject. Body & Society, 20(3–4), 162188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MARTÍN ESCOBAR, M. J. (2005). Del Movimiento a la Danza en la educación musical. Educatio Siglo XXI, 23, 125140.Google Scholar
MEHR, S. A., SCHACHNER, A., KATZ, R. C. & SPELKE, E. S. (2013). Two randomized trials provide no consistent evidence for nonmusical cognitive benefits of brief preschool music enrichment. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e82007.Google ScholarPubMed
MINTON, S. C. & FABER, R. (2016). Thinking with the Dancing Brain. Embodying Neuroscience. Lanham, Maryland, USA: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
MULLEN, G. (2015). More than words: Using nursery rhymes and songs to support domains of child development. Journal of Childhood Studies, 42(2), 4253.Google Scholar
MURRIS, K. (2016). The Posthuman Child. Educational Transformation Through Philosophy with Picturebooks. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
MURRIS, K. & BOZALEK, V. (2019). Diffraction and response-able reading of texts: The relational ontologies of Barad and Deleuze. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 32(7), 872886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PAHL, K., STEADMAN-JONES, R. & VASUDEVAN, L. (2022). Collaborative Research in Theory and Practice. the Poetics of Letting Go. Bristol, UK: Bristol University Press.Google Scholar
PAQUETTE, K. R. & RIEG, S. A. (2008). Using music to support the literacy development of young English language learners. Early Childhood Education Journal, 36(3), 227232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
PARR, A. (2010). The Deleuze Dictionary. Edinburgh, Scotland: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
PERRY, M. & MEDINA, C. L. (2015). Methodologies of Embodiment. Inscribing Bodies in Qualitative Research. New York, USA: Routledge.Google Scholar
PHILPOTT, C. (2016). Music literacy: Music as language. In McPherson, G. E. (ed.), The Child as Musician. A Handbook for Musical Development (2nd ed., pp. 192207). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
POWELL, S. & SOMERVILLE, M. (2020). Drumming in excess and chaos: Music, literacy and sustainability in early years learning. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 20(4), 839861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RAUSCHER, F. H. & HINTON, S. C. (2011). Music instruction and its diverse extra-musical benefits. Music Perception, 29(2), 215226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SCHELLENBERG, G. E. (2016). Music and non-musical abilities. In McPherson, G. E. (ed.), The Child as Musician. A Handbook for Musical Development (2nd ed., pp. 149176). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
SCHRADER, A. (2023). Diffraction as cross-disciplinary methodology between science and arts. In Bayley, A. & Chan, J. (eds.), Diffracting New Materialisms: Emerging Methods in Artistic Research and Higher Education (pp. 1540). Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
SHANNON, D. B. (2020). Neuroqueer(ing) noise: beyond ‘mere inclusion’ in a neurodiverse early childhood classroom. Canadian Journal of Disability Studies, 9(5), 489514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SLATER, J., TIERNEY, A. & KRAUS, N. (2014). At-risk elementary school children with one year of classroom music instruction are better at keeping a beat. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e77250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SNAZA, N., APPELBAUM, P., BAYNE, S., CARLSON, D., MORRIS, M., ROTAS, N., SANDLIN, J., WALLIN, J. & WAVER, J. (2014). Toward a posthumanist education. Journal of Curriculum Theorizing, 30(2), 3955.Google Scholar
SNAZA, N. & SONU, D. (2016). Borders, bodies, and the politics of attention. In Snaza, N., Sonu, D., Truman, S. E. & Zaliwska, Z. (eds.), Pedagogical Matters. New Materialisms and Curriculum Studies (pp. 2942). New York, USA: Peter Lang.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
SNAZA, N., SONU, D., TRUMAN, S. E. & ZALIWSKA, Z. (2016). Introduction. Re-attuning to the materiality of education. In Snaza, N., Sonu, D., Truman, S. E. & Zaliwska, Z. (eds.), Pedagogical Matters. New Materialisms and Curriculum Studies (pp. xvixxxiii). New York, USA: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
SOMERVILLE, M. & POWELL, S. (2019). Thinking posthuman with mud: And children of the Anthropocene. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 51(8), 829840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ST. PIERRE, E. A. (2019). Post qualitative inquiry in an ontology of immanence. Qualitative Inquiry, 25(1), 316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WARGO, J. M. (2018). Earwitnessing (in)equity: Tracing the intra-active encounters of ‘being-in-resonance-with’ sound and the social contexts of education. Educational Studies, 54(4), 382395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
WIENS, K. F. (2015). Music, movement and the brain. Canadian Music Educator, 57(1), 3437.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. What’s the song about?

Figure 1

Figure 2. Do not dance.