Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:51:07.790Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cross-linguistic differences and their impact on L2 sentence processing*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2009

CARRIE N. JACKSON*
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University
PAOLA E. DUSSIAS
Affiliation:
The Pennsylvania State University
*
Address for correspondence: Carrie N. Jackson, Department of Germanic and Slavic Languages and Literatures, 311 Burrowes Building, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA[email protected]

Abstract

Using a self-paced reading task, the present study investigates how highly proficient second language (L2) speakers of German with English as their native language process unambiguous wh-subject-extractions and wh-object-extractions in German. Previous monolingual research has shown that English and German exhibit different processing preferences for the type of wh-question under investigation, due in part to the robust case-marking system in German – a morphosyntactic feature that is largely absent in English (e.g., Juffs and Harrington, 1995; Fanselow, Kliegl and Schlesewsky 1999; Meng and Bader, 2000; Juffs, 2005). The results revealed that the L2 German speakers utilized case-marking information and exhibited a subject-preference similar to German native speakers. These findings are discussed in light of relevant research regarding the ability of L2 speakers to adopt native-like processing strategies in their L2.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This study was funded in part by grants from Language Learning – A Journal of Research in Language Studies and from the Children Youth and Families Consortium at Pennsylvania State University. We would like to thank Susan Bobb, Mark Louden and Helena Ruf for their help in collecting the L2 speaker data, and Sonja Kotz and other members of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Science in Leipzig, Germany, for their help in collecting the German native speaker data. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the Center for Language Science at Penn State. We are grateful to the attendees for their insightful comments. Finally, we would like to thank the anonymous Bilingualism reviewers for their comments on earlier versions of this article. All errors are, of course, our own.

References

Bader, M. & Meng, M. (1999). Subject–object ambiguities in German embedded clauses: An across-the-board comparison. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28, 121143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, G. N. & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1988). Thematic roles and language comprehension. In Wilkins, W. (ed.), Syntax and semantics, pp. 263288. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. & Felser, C. (2006). Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E. (2001). Sentence parsing in fluent Spanish–English bilinguals. In Nicol, J. L. (ed.), One mind, two languages, pp. 159176. Malden, MA: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E. (2003). Syntactic ambiguity resolution in L2 learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 529557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dussias, P. E. & Piñar, P. in press. Effects of reading span and plausibility in the reanalysis of wh-gaps by Chinese–English L2 speakers. Second Language Research, 25.Google Scholar
Dussias, P. E. & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 101116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fanselow, G., Kliegl, R. & Schlesewsky, M. (1999). Processing difficulty and principles of grammar. In Kemper, S. & Kliegl, R. (eds.), Constraints on language: Aging, grammar, and memory, pp. 171202. Boston, MA: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Felser, C., Roberts, R., Marinis, T. & Gross, R. (2003). The processing of ambiguous sentences by first and second language learners of English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 453489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández, E. M. (1999). Processing strategies in second language acquisition: Some preliminary results. In Klein, E. & Martohardjono, G. (eds.), The development of second language grammars: A generative approach, pp. 217240. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández, E. M. (2003). Bilingual sentence processing: Relative clause attachment in English and Spanish. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. D. (1993). Empty categories in sentence processing: A question of visibility. In Altmann, G. & Shillcock, R. (eds.), Cognitive models of speech processing, pp. 351400. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. D. (1998). Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 285319.Google Scholar
Frazier, L. & Clifton, C. Jr. (1989). Successive cyclicity in the grammar and the parser. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 93126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. (1997). Examining second language reading: An on-line look. In Sorace, A., Heycock, C. & Shillcock, R. (eds.), Proceedings of the GALA 1997 Conference on Language Acquisition, pp. 474–478. Edinburgh: Human Communications Research Center.Google Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. (2002). An on-line look at sentence processing in the second language. In Heredia, R. & Altarriba, J. (eds.), Bilingual sentence processing, pp. 218236. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, C. & Pynte, J. (1997). Syntactic ambiguity resolution while reading in second and native languages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 50A, 119148.Google Scholar
Gass, S. (1987). The resolution of conflicts among competing systems: A bidirectional perspective. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 329350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorrell, P. (2000). The subject-before-object preference in German clauses. In Hemforth, & Konieczny, (eds.), pp. 25–65.Google Scholar
Grewendorf, G. (1988). Aspekte der deutschen Syntax [Aspects of German syntax]. Tübingen: Gunther Narr.Google Scholar
Harrington, M. (1987). Processing transfer: Language-specific processing strategies as a source of interlanguage variation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 351377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemforth, B. & Konieczny, L. (eds.) (2000). German sentence processing. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemforth, B., Konieczny, L. & Scheepers, C. (2000). Syntactic attachment and anaphor resolution: The two sides of relative clause attachment. In Crocker, M. W., Pickering, M. & Clifton, C.. (eds.), Architectures and mechanisms for language processing, pp. 259281. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hoover, M. L. & Dwivedi, V. D. (1998). Syntactic processing in skilled bilinguals. Language Learning, 48, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopp, H. (2006). Syntactic features and reanalysis in near-native processing. Second Language Research, 22, 369397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. N. (2007). The use and non-use of semantic information, word order, and case markings during comprehension by L2 learners of German. Modern Language Journal, 91 (3), 418432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, N. (2004). Morphological insensitivity in second language processing. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 603634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. (1998). Main verb versus reduced relative clause ambiguity resolution in second language sentence processing. Language Learning, 48, 107147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. (2005). The influence of first language on the processing of wh-movement in English as a second language. Second Language Research, 21, 121151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A. & Harrington, A. (1995). Parsing effects in second language sentence processing: Subject and object asymmetries in wh-extraction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 17, 482516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A. & Woolley, J. D. (1982). Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 228238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ladiere, D. (1998). Disassociating syntax from morphology in a divergent L2 end-state grammar. Second Language Research, 14, 359375.Google Scholar
Lenerz, J. (1977). Zur Abfolge nominaler Satzglieder im Deutschen [On the sequence of nominal phrases in German]. Tübingen: Gunther Narr.Google Scholar
Marinis, T., Roberts, L., Felser, C. & Clahsen, H. (2005). Gaps in second language processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 5378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meng, M. & Bader, M. (2000). Mode of disambiguation and garden-path strength. Language and Speech, 43, 4374.Google Scholar
Miyao, M. & Omaki, , A. (2006). No ambiguity about it: Korean learners of Japanese have a clear attachment preference. In Bamman, D., Magnitskaia, T. & Zaller, C.. (eds.), A supplement to the proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 12 pp. http://www.bu.edu/linguistics/APPLIED/BUCLD/supp30.html (retrieved 6 November 2008).Google Scholar
Papadopoulou, D. & Clahsen, H. (2003). Parsing strategies in L1 and L2 sentence processing: A study of relative clause attachment in Greek. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 501528.Google Scholar
Pritchett, B. L. (1992). Grammatical competence and parsing performance. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Ritterbusch, R., LaFond, L. & Agustin, M. (2006). Learner difficulties with German case: Implications from an action research study. Die Unterrichtspraxis, 39, 3045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasaki, Y. (1991). English and Japanese interlanguage comprehension strategies – an analysis based on the competition model. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 4773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlesewsky, M., Fanselow, G., Kliegl, R. & Krems, J. (2000). Preferences for grammatical functions in the processing of locally ambiguous wh-questions in German. In Hemforth, & Konieczny, (eds.), pp. 65–94.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Eschmann, A. & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime v1.1. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.Google Scholar
Su, I-R. (2001). Transfer of sentence processing strategies: A comparison of L2 learners of Chinese and English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 83112.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. (2001). The neural basis of lexicon and grammar in first and second language: The declarative/procedural model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 4, 105122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. (2003). Second language acquisition and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N. (2006). Incremental interpretation in second language sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9, 7181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. N., Möbius, P. & Kim, C. (2001). Native and non-native processing of English wh-questions: Parsing strategies and plausibility constraints. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 509540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zubin, D. (1977). The semantic bases of case alternation in German. In Fasold, R. W. & Shuy, R. W.. (eds.), Studies in language variation: Semantics, syntax, phonology, pragmatics, social situations, ethnographic approaches, pp. 8899. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar