Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T03:18:50.020Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Differential Effects of Worry and Imagery After Exposure to a Stressful Stimulus: A Pilot Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2009

Gillian Butler
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Adrian Wells
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Hilary Dewick
Affiliation:
University of Oxford

Abstract

Imagery appears to be associated with higher levels of anxiety than does worry. Borkovec has argued that worry could be a way of avoiding distressing imagery and the associated affect. Thus worry could suppress emotional activation, interfere with emotional processing, and contribute to the maintenance of anxiety. This hypothesis suggests that short and long-term effects of worrying after experiencing a distressing stimulus should differ from the effects of engaging in imagery. In the short term, imagery should maintain anxiety while worry should not do so, or should do so less. In the longer term, worry should be a less successful way of reducing anxiety associated with the stimulus than imagery, and should be followed by a greater number of intrusive cognitions (indicating the relative failure of emotional processing). These predictions were tested by asking subjects to worry, engage in imagery or “settle down” after watching a distressing video. The results were broadly consistent with the hypothesis. Other interpretations are also considered.

Type
Main Section
Copyright
Copyright © British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Third Ed. Revised). Washington, D.C: APA.Google Scholar
Borkovec, T. D. and Hu, S. (1990). The effect of worry on cardiovascular response to phobic imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 6973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borkovec, T. D. and Inz, J. (1990). The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: a predominance of thought activity. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 153158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borkovec, T. D., Shadick, R. N. and Hopkins, M. (1991). The nature of normal and pathological worry. In Rapee, R. and Barlow, D. H. (Eds). Chronic Anxiety and Mixed Anxiety-Depression. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Clark, D. M., Ball, S. and Pape, D. (1991). An experimental investigation of thought suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy 29, 253257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deffenbacher, J. L. (1978). Worry, emotionality and task-generated interference in test anxiety: an empirical test of attentional theory. Journal of Educational Psychology 70, 248254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F. and Buss, A. M. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness and trait anxiety. Psychological Reports 43, 522527.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B. and Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin 991, 2035.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, P. J. (1985). The cognitive psychophysiology of emotion: fear and anxiety. In Tuma, A. H. and Maser, J. D. (Eds). Anxiety and the Anxiety Disorders. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L.Metzger, R. L. and Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy 28, 487495.Google Scholar
Morris, L. W., Davis, M. A. and Hutchings, C. H. (1981). Cognitive and emotional components of anxiety: literature review and a revised worry-emotionality scale. Journal of Educational Psychology 73, 541555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachman, S. (1980). Emotional processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy 18, 5160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, A. (1977). Verbalizer-visualizer: a cognitive style dimension. Journal of Mental Imagery 1, 109126.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R. and Jacobs, G. A. (1977). Self-Evaluation Questionnaire. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R. and White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53, 513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, A. (1985). Relationship between private self-consciousness and anxiety scores in threatening situations. Psychological Reports 57, 10631066.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, A. (1991). Effects of dispositional self-focus, appraisal and attention instructions on responses to a threatening stimulus. Anxiety Research 3, 291301.Google Scholar
Wells, A. (1994a). Attention and the control of worry. In Davey, G. C. L. and Tallis, F. (Eds). Worrying: Perspectives on Theory, Assessment and Treatment, pp 91114. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wells, A. (1994b). A multi-dimensional measure of worry: development and preliminary validation of the Anxious Thoughts Inventory. Anxiety, Stress and Coping 6, 289299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, A. and Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and Emotion: A Clinical Perspective. Hove, Sussex: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wine, J. D. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin 76, 92104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.