Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T15:23:06.482Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Vocal coordination and vocal imitation: A role for mirror neurons?1

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2014

John D. Newman*
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Comparative Ethology, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health (NIH), Poolesville, MD 20837. [email protected]

Abstract

Some birds and mammals have vocal communication systems in which coordination between individuals is important. Examples would include duetting or antiphonal calling in some birds and mammals, rapid exchanges of the same vocalization, and vocal exchanges between paired individuals and other nearby pairs. Mirror neurons may play a role in such systems but become functional only after experience.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

1.

© John D. Newman 2014. This is a work of the U.S. Government and is not subject to copyright protection in the United States.

References

Brenowitz, E. A., Arnold, A. P. & Levin, R. N. (1985) Neural correlates of female song in tropical duetting birds. Brain Research 343:104–12.Google Scholar
Geissmann, T. (2002) Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biological Reviews 77:5776.Google Scholar
George, I., Alcaix, S., Henry, L., Richard, J.-P., Cousillas, H. & Hausberger, M. (2010) Neural correlates of experience-induced deficits in learned vocal communication. PLoS ONE 5(12):e14347. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0014347.Google Scholar
Hickok, G. (2010) The role of mirror neurons in speech and language processing. Brain and Language 112:12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCowan, B. & Newman, J. D. (2000) The role of learning in chuck call recognition by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Behaviour 137:279300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, C. T., DiMauro, A., Pistorio, A., Hendry, S. & Wang, X. (2010) Vocalization induced cFos expression in marmoset cortex. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 4:115.Google Scholar
Miller, C. T. & Wang, X. (2006) Sensory-motor interactions modulate a primate vocal behavior: Antiphonal calling in common marmosets. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 192:2738.Google Scholar
Müller, A. E. & Anzenberger, G. (2002) Duetting in the titi monkey Callicebus cupreus: Structure, pair specificity and development of duets. Folia Primatologica 73:104–15.Google Scholar
Newman, J. D. & Bernhards, D. E. (1997) The affiliative vocal subsystem of squirrel monkeys. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 807:546–48. [Annals Vol. 807: The integrative neurobiology of affiliation, ed. C.S. Carter, B. Kirkpatrick & I. Lederhendler. Wiley.]Google Scholar
Soltis, J., Bernhards, D., Donkin, H. & Newman, J. D. (2002) Squirrel monkey chuck call: Vocal response to playback chucks based on acoustic structure and affiliative relationship with the caller. American Journal of Primatology 57:119–30.Google Scholar