No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Sociocultural discourse in science: Flawed assumptions and bias in the CLASH model
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 11 May 2017
Abstract
In this commentary, we contest Van Lange and colleagues' central claim that “countries closer to the equator are generally more violent.” We point to the lack of credible empirical evidence for this assertion and suggest that the CLASH model uses the language of science to lend false credibility to a problematic sociocultural discourse.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Burke, M., Hsiang, S. M. & Miguel, E. (2015) Climate and conflict. Annual Review of Economics
7:577–617.Google Scholar
Cohen, W. B. (2003) The French encounter with Africans: White responses to blacks, 1530–1880. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A. & Coie, J. D. (1987) Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
53(6):1146–58.Google Scholar
Easterly, W. & Levine, R. (2003) Tropics, germs, and crops: How endowments influence economic development. Journal of Monetary Economics
50(1):3–39. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3932(02)00200-3.Google Scholar
Fienberg, S. E. & Resnick, D. P. (1997) Reexamining the bell curve. In: Intelligence, genes, and success: Scientists respond to the bell curve, ed. Devlin, B., Fienberg, S. E., Resnick, D. P. & Roeder, K., pp. 3–18. Springer Science+Business Media.Google Scholar
Glass, D. C., Snyder, M. L. & Hollis, J. F. (1974) Time urgency and the type A coronary-prone behavior pattern. Journal of Applied Social Psychology
4(2):125–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1974.tb00663.x.Google Scholar
Harrison, F. V. (1995) The persistent power of “race” in the culture and political economy of racism. Annual Review of Anthropology
24:47–74.Google Scholar
Karlberg, L., Krakau, I. & Undén, A.-L. (1998) Type A behavior intervention in primary health care reduces hostility and time pressure: A study in Sweden. Social Science & Medicine
46(3):397–402. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(97)00169-X.Google Scholar
Krug, E. G., Mercy, J. A., Dahlberg, L. L. & Zwi, A. B. (2002) The world report on violence and health. The Lancet
360(9339):1083–88. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11133-0.Google Scholar
Montesquieu, Charles de Secondat, baron de (1989). The spirit of the laws. In: Montesquieu: The spirit of the laws, part of Cambridge texts in the history of political thought ed. and trans. Cohler, A. M., Basia, C., Miller, B. C. & Stone, H., pp. 233–234. Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1748.)Google Scholar
Sachs, J. D. (2001) Tropical underdevelopment. National Bureau of Economic Research Working Paper Series, Paper No. 8119. National Bureau of Economic Research. doi: 10.3386/w8119.Google Scholar
Walker, J., Wilson, P. R., Chappell, D. & Weatherburn, D. (1990) A comparison of crime in Australia and other countries. Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice
23:1–8. Canberra: Australian Institute of Criminology.Google Scholar
Target article
Aggression and violence around the world: A model of CLimate, Aggression, and Self-control in Humans (CLASH)
Related commentaries (28)
A climate of confusion
Aggression, predictability of the environment, and self-regulation: Reconciliation with animal research
An alternative interpretation of climate data: Intelligence
Bullying when it's hot? The CLASH model and climatic influences on bullying
CLASH's life history foundations
Climate is not a good candidate to account for variations in aggression and violence across space and time
Culture matters for life history trade-offs
Dimensions of environmental risk are unique theoretical constructs
Does distance from the equator predict self-control? Lessons from the Human Penguin Project
Hell on earth? Equatorial peaks of heat, poverty, and aggression
Inconsistent with the data: Support for the CLASH model depends on the wrong kind of latitude
More than just climate: Income inequality and sex ratio are better predictors of cross-cultural variations in aggression
Postcolonial geography confounds latitudinal trends in observed aggression and violence
Pragmatic prospection emphasizes utility of predicting rather than mere predictability
Reply to Van Lange et al.: Proximate and ultimate distinctions must be made to the CLASH model
Russian data refute the CLASH model
Sociocultural discourse in science: Flawed assumptions and bias in the CLASH model
Stuck in the heat or stuck in the hierarchy? Power relations explain regional variations in violence
The CLASH model in broader life history context
The CLASH model lacks evolutionary and archeological support
The importance of being explicit
The paradoxical effect of climate on time perspective considering resource accumulation
The role of adolescence in geographic variation in violent aggression
The role of climate in human aggression and violence: Towards a broader conception
Using foresight to prioritise the present
Warm coffee, sunny days, and prosocial behavior
Where the psychological adaptations hit the ecological road
Why the CLASH model is an unconvincing evolutionary theory of crime
Author response
The Logic of Climate and Culture: Evolutionary and Psychological Aspects of CLASH