Hostname: page-component-f554764f5-nwwvg Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-04-15T14:15:20.293Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Societal inferences from the physical world

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 April 2025

Rodney Tompkins
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA [email protected] [email protected] https://madlab.ucsd.edu/
Julian Jara-Ettinger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA [email protected] https://compdevlab.yale.edu/ Wu-Tsai Institute, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Adena Schachner*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA [email protected] [email protected] https://madlab.ucsd.edu/
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

Moffett points to humans' use of physical markers to signal group identity as crucial to human society. We characterize the developmental and cognitive bases of this capacity, arguing that it is part of an early-emerging, intuitive socio-physical interface which allows the inanimate world to encode rich social meaning about individuals' identities, and the values of the society as a whole.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Article purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Bian, L., & Baillargeon, R. (2022). When are similar individuals a group? Early reasoning about similarity and in-group support. Psychological Science, 33(5), 752764. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976211055185CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, G., & Kizzire, J. (2016). Whose heritage? Public symbols of the Confederacy. https://www.splcenter.org/whose-heritage. Accessed 30 May 2024.Google Scholar
Brey, E., & Shutts, K. (2015). Children use nonverbal cues to make inferences about social power. Child Development, 86(1), 276286. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12334Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y. C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). The perception of history: Seeing causal history in static shapes induces illusory motion perception. Psychological Science, 27(6), 923930. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616628525Google ScholarPubMed
Dukler, N., & Liberman, Z. (2022). Children use race to infer who is “in charge.”. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 221, 105447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105447Google ScholarPubMed
Gehl, J. (2013). Cities for people. Island Press.Google Scholar
Gweon, H., Asaba, M., & Bennett-Pierre, G. (2017). Reverse-engineering the process: Adults’ and preschoolers’ ability to infer the difficulty of novel tasks. Proceedings of the 39th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Henderson, K., Powers, S., Claibourn, M., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Trawalter, S. (2021). Confederate monuments and the history of lynching in the American South: An empirical examination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 118(42), e2103519118.Google ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. (2015). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hurwitz, E., Brady, T., & Schachner, A. (2019). Detecting social transmission in the design of artifacts via inverse planning. Proceedings of the 41st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Jara-Ettinger, J., & Schachner, A. (2024). Traces of our past: The social representation of the physical world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 33(5), 334340. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214241268145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinzler, K. D. (2021). Language as a social cue. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 241264. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103034Google ScholarPubMed
Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., & Kinzler, K. D. (2016). The origins of social categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(7), 556568. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.004Google Scholar
Lopez-Brau, M., & Jara-Ettinger, J. (2023). People can use the placement of objects to infer communicative goals. Cognition, 239, 105524. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2023.105524Google ScholarPubMed
Lopez-Brau, M., Colombatto, C., Jara-Ettinger, J., & Scholl, B. (2021). Attentional prioritization for historical traces of agency. Journal of Vision, 21(9), 2748. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.9.2748Google Scholar
Lopez-Brau, M., Kwon, J., & Jara-Ettinger, J. (2022). Social inferences from physical evidence via Bayesian event reconstruction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 151(9), 20292042. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001182Google ScholarPubMed
Öner, G., & Soley, G. (2023). Children use epistemic states flexibly to make diagnostic social inferences. Developmental Psychology, 59(12), 22772286. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001653Google ScholarPubMed
Pelz, M., Schulz, L., & Jara-Ettinger, J. (2020). The signature of all things: Children infer knowledge states from static images. Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Pesowski, M. L., Quy, A. D., Lee, M., & Schachner, A. (2020). Children use inverse planning to detect social transmission in the design of artifacts. Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shutts, K., & Kalish, C. W. (2021). Intuitive sociology. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 61, 335374. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2021.05.004Google ScholarPubMed
Soley, G., & Spelke, E. S. (2016). Shared cultural knowledge: Effects of music on young children's social preferences. Cognition, 148, 106116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.017Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. (2022). What babies know. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tompkins, R., Vasquez, K., Gerdin, E., Dunham, Y., & Liberman, Z. (2023). Expectations of intergroup empathy bias emerge by early childhood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001505Google ScholarPubMed
Weatherhead, D., Nancekivell, S. E., & Baron, A. S. (2022). Wearing your knowledge on your sleeve: Young children's reasoning about clothing as a marker of group-specific knowledge. Cognitive Development, 62, 101177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2022.101177CrossRefGoogle Scholar