Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T15:11:21.904Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The rich detail of cultural symbol systems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 August 2014

Dwight W. Read*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology and Department of Statistics, University of California–Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095. [email protected]://ucla.academia.edu/DwightRead

Abstract

The goal of forming a science of intentional behavior requires a more richly detailed account of symbolic systems than is assumed by the authors. Cultural systems are not simply the equivalent in the ideational domain of culture of the purported Baldwin Effect in the genetic domain.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Balikci, A. (1970) The Netsilik Eskimo. Doubleday.Google Scholar
Bennardo, G. & Read, D. (2007) Cognition, algebra, and culture in the Tongan kinship terminology. Journal of Cognition 7(1):4988.Google Scholar
Damas, D. (1969) The diversity of Eskimo societies. In: Man the hunter, ed. Lee, R. B. & DeVore, I., pp. 111–17. Aldine.Google Scholar
Damas, D. (1972) Central Eskimo systems of food sharing. Ethnology 11(3):220–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973) The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M. J. (2005) Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leaf, M. & Read, D. (2012) The conceptual foundation of human society and thought: Anthropology on a new plane. Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Rasmussen, K. (1931) The Netsilik Eskimo: Social life and spiritual culture (Report of the Fifth Thule Expedition, vol. 8). Gyldendalske Boghandel.Google Scholar
Read, D. (1984) An algebraic account of the American kinship terminology. Current Anthropology 25(4):417–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, D. (2001) What is kinship? In: The cultural analysis of kinship: The legacy of David Schneider and its implications for anthropological relativism, ed. Feinberg, R. & Ottenheimer, M., pp. 78117. University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Read, D. (2005) Some observations on resilience and robustness in human systems. Cybernetics and Systems 36:773802.Google Scholar
Read, D. (2007) Kinship theory: A paradigm shift. Ethnology 46(4):329–64.Google Scholar
Read, D. (2010) The algebraic logic of kinship terminology structure. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(5):399400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, D. (2012) How culture makes us human: Primate evolution and the formation of human societies. Left Coast Press.Google Scholar
Read, D. (2013) A new approach to forming a typology of kinship terminology systems: From Morgan and Murdock to the present. Structure and Dynamics 6(1). Available at: http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/0ss6j8sh.Google Scholar
Read, D. & Behrens, C. (1990) KAES: An expert system for the algebraic analysis of kinship terminologies. Journal of Quantitative Anthropology 2:353–93.Google Scholar
Read, D., Lane, D. & van der Leeuw, S. (2009) The innovation innovation. In: Complexity perspectives in innovation and social change, ed. Lane, D., Pumain, D., van der Leeuw, S. & West, G., pp. 4384. Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Read, D., Leaf, M. & Fischer, M. D. (2013) What are kinship terminologies, and why do we care? A computational approach to analyzing symbolic domains. Social Science Computer Review 31(1):1644.Google Scholar
Simpson, G. G. (1953) The Baldwin effect. Evolution 7:110–17.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. & Smith, S. A. (1994) Inuit sex-ratio variation. Current Anthropology 35(5):595659.Google Scholar
Van de Velde, F. (1956) Les règles du partage des phoquespris par la chase aux aglus. Anthropologica 3:514.Google Scholar