Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T09:06:36.100Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Putting revenge and forgiveness in an evolutionary context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2012

Michael E. McCullough
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL 33124-0751. [email protected]://www.psy.miami.edu/faculty/mmccullough
Robert Kurzban
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104. [email protected]://www.psych.upenn.edu/~kurzban/ Economic Science Institute, Chapman University, Orange, CA 92866
Benjamin A. Tabak
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California–Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563. [email protected]

Abstract

In this response, we address eight issues concerning our proposal that human minds contain adaptations for revenge and forgiveness. Specifically, we discuss (a) the inferences that are and are not licensed by patterns of contemporary behavioral data in the context of the adaptationist approach; (b) the theoretical pitfalls of conflating proximate and ultimate causation; (c) the role of development in the production of adaptations; (d) the implications of proposing that the brain's cognitive systems are fundamentally computational in nature; (e) our preferred method for considering the role of individual differences in computational systems; (f) applications of our proposal to understanding conflicts between groups; (g) the possible implications of our views for understanding the operation of contemporary criminal justice systems; and (h) the question of whether people ever “genuinely” forgive.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, M. L. (2010) Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33:245313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W. & Matthews, D. (2002) Adaptationism – How to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25:489504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aureli, F., Cozzolino, R., Cordischi, C. & Scucchi, S. (1992) Kin-oriented redirection among Japanese macaques: An expression of a revenge system? Animal Behaviour 44:283–91.Google Scholar
Ballard, K. & Knutson, B. (2009) Dissociable neural representations of future reward magnitude and delay during temporal discounting. NeuroImage 45:143–50.Google Scholar
Balliet, D. (2010) Conscientiousness and forgivingness: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences 48:259–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, H. C. & Kurzban, R. (2006) Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review 113:628–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L. & Draper, P. (1991) Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development 62:647–70.Google Scholar
Blumstein, D. T., Ferando, E. & Stankowich, T. (2009) A test of the multipredator hypothesis: Yellow-bellied marmots respond fearfully to the sight of novel and extinct predators. Animal Behaviour 78:873–78.Google Scholar
Bshary, A. & Bshary, R. (2010) Self-serving punishment of a common enemy creates a public good in reef fishes. Current Biology 20:14.Google Scholar
Bshary, R. & Grutter, A. S. (2005) Punishment and partner switching cause cooperative behavior in a cleaning mutualism. Biology Letters 1:396–99.Google Scholar
Burnette, J. L., McCullough, M. E., Van Tongeren, D. R. & Davis, D. E. (2012) Forgiveness results from integrating information about relationship value and exploitation risk. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 38:345–56.Google Scholar
Burnham, T. C. (2003) Engineering altruism: A theoretical and experimental investigation of anonymity and gift giving. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 50:133–44.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2009) How can evolutionary psychology successfully explain personality and individual differences? Perspectives on Psychological Science 4:359–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, P. (2006) The architecture of the mind. Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cherniak, C., Mokhtarzada, Z., Rodriguez-Esteban, R. & Changizi, K. (2004) Global optimization of cerebral cortex layout. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 101:1081–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curry, O. S., Price, M. E. & Price, J. G. (2008) Patience is a virtue: Cooperative people have lower discount rates. Personality and Individual Differences 44:780–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (2005) Carpe diem: Adaptation and devaluing the future. Quarterly Review of Biology 80:5560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. (1978) Animal signals: Information or manipulation? In: Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, 1st edition, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B., pp. 282309. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Delton, A. W., Krasnow, M. M., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2011) Evolution of direct reciprocity under uncertainty can explain human generosity in one-shot encounters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108:13335–40.Google Scholar
DeScioli, P. (2008) Investigations into the problems of moral cognition. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
DeScioli, P., Bruening, R. & Kurzban, R. (2011a) The omission effect in moral cognition: Toward a functional explanation. Evolution and Human Behavior 32:204–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeScioli, P. & Kurzban, R. (2009a) Mysteries of morality. Cognition 112:281–99.Google Scholar
DeScioli, P. & Kurzban, R. (2009b) The alliance hypothesis for human friendship. PLoS One 4:e5802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeScioli, P. & Kurzban, R. (2012) The company you keep: Friendship decisions from a functional perspective. In: Social judgment and decision making, ed. Krueger, J. I.. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
DeScioli, P., Gilbert, S. & Kurzban, R. (2012) Indelible victims and persistent punishers in moral cognition. Psychological Inquiry 23:143–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeScioli, P., Kurzban, R., Koch, E. N. & Liben-Nowell, D. (2011b) Best friends: Alliances, friend ranking, and the MySpace social network. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6:68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J. (2004) Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin 130:920–58.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D. F.eds. (2005) Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fehr, R., Gelfand, M. J. & Nag, M. (2010) The road to forgiveness: A meta-analytic synthesis of its situational and dispositional correlates. Psychological Bulletin 136:894914.Google Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1988) Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. Norton.Google Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E. & Del Giudice, M. (2012) When do adaptive developmental mechanisms yield maladaptive outcomes? Developmental Psychology 48:628–42.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2010) Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000) Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development 71:5765.Google Scholar
Gray, K., Young, L. & Waytz, A. (in press) Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry.Google Scholar
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J., Delton, A. & Robertson, T. (2011) The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100:1015–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guala, F. (2012) Reciprocity: Weak or strong? What punishment experiments do (and do not) demonstrate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35(1):159.Google Scholar
Gullo, M. J. & Dawe, S. (2008) Impulsivity and adolescent substance use: Rashly dismissed as “all-bad”? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32:1507–28.Google Scholar
Hagen, E. H. & Hammerstein, P. (2006) Game theory and human evolution: A critique of some recent interpretations of experimental games. Theoretical Population Biology 69:339–48.Google Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. & de Waal, F. B. M., eds. (1992) Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hill, E. M. & Chow, K. (2006) Life-history theory and risky drinking. Addiction 97:401–13.Google Scholar
Hill, E. M., Jenkins, J. & Farmer, L. (2008) Family unpredictability, future discounting, and risk taking. Journal of Socio-Economics 37:1381–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holtzman, N. S. & Strube, M. J. (2010) Narcissism and attractiveness. Journal of Research in Personality 44:133–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoover, J. P. & Robinson, S. K. (2007) Retaliatory mafia behavior by a parasitic cowbird favors host acceptance of parasitic eggs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104:4479–83.Google Scholar
Hoyt, W. T., Fincham, F., McCullough, M. E., Maio, G. & Davila, J. (2005) Responses to interpersonal transgressions in families: Forgivingness, forgivability, and relationship-specific effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89:375–94.Google Scholar
Jensen, K., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2007) Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spiteful. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104:13046–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurzban, R. (2010a) Does the brain consume additional glucose during self-control tasks. Evolutionary Psychology 8:244–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurzban, R. (2010b) Why everyone (else) is a hypocrite: Evolutionary psychology and the modular mind. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P. & Fein, D. (2012) Hamilton vs. Kant: Pitting adaptations for altruism against adaptations for moral judgment. Evolution and Human Behavior 32(4):323–33.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P. & O'Brien, E. (2007) Audience effects on moralistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior 28:7584.Google Scholar
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2001) Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98:15387–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough, M. E. (2001) Forgiveness: Who does it and how do they do it? Current Directions in Psychological Science 10, 194–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, M. E. (2008) Beyond revenge: The evolution of the forgiveness instinct. Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
McCullough, M. E., Bellah, C. G., Kilpatrick, S. D. & Johnson, J. L. (2001) Vengefulness: Relationships with forgiveness, rumination, well-being, and the Big Five. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27:601–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, M. E. & Hoyt, W. T. (2002) Transgression-related motivational dispositions: Personality substrates of forgiveness and their links to the Big Five. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28:1556–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCullough, M. E., Pedersen, E. J., Schroder, J., Tabak, B. & Carver, C. S. (2012) Childhood exposure to neglect, conflict, and crime predict unprovoked and retaliatory defection in an iterated prisoner's dilemma. Unpublished manuscript. University of Miami.Google Scholar
McCullough, M. E. & Worthington, E. L. Jr. (1999) Religion and the forgiving personality. Journal of Personality 67:1141–64.Google Scholar
Molden, D. C., Hui, C. M., Scholer, A. A., Meier, B. P., Noreen, E. E., D'Agostino, P. R. & Martin, V. (in press) The motivational versus metabolic effects of carbohydrates on self-control. Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Navon, D. (1984) Resources-a theoretical soup stone? Psychological Review 91:216–34.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2006) The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals. American Psychologist 20:622–31.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2009) Ecological influences on human behavioural diversity: A review of recent findings. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 24:618–24.Google Scholar
Petersen, M. B., Sell, A., Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2010) Evolutionary psychology and criminal justice: A recalibrational theory of punishment and reconciliation. In: Human morality and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative perspectives, ed. Høgh-Olesen, H., pp. 72131. Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1999) How the mind works. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 882:119–27.Google Scholar
Riek, B. M. & Mania, E. W. (2012) The antecedents and consequences of interpersonal forgiveness: A meta-analytic review. Personal Relationships 19:304–25.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. (1960) The strategy of conflict. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Scott-Philips, T. C. (2008) On the correct application of animal signalling theory to human communication. In: Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on the Evolution of Language, 7th edition, ed. Smith, A. D. M., Smith, K. & Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., pp. 275–82. World Scientific.Google Scholar
Sell, A., Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2009) Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106(35):15073–78.Google Scholar
Smith, J. M. (1982) Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994) The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In: Mapping the mind, ed. Hirschfeld, L. & Gelman, S., pp. 3967. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. R., Cushman, F. A. & Hauser, M. D. (2005) Evolving the psychological mechanisms for cooperation. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 36:499518.Google Scholar
Szymanska, E. J. (2011) Retaliation versus vigilantism: Why do we choose to punish? Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Tiger, L. (1969) Men in groups. Random House.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1990a) On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality 58:1768.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 19136. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2008) The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In: Handbook of emotions, 3rd edition, ed. Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M. & Barrett, L. F., pp. 114–37. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., Cosmides, L. & Barrett, H. C. (2003) The second law of thermodynamics is the first law of psychology: Evolutionary developmental psychology and the theory of tandem, coordinated inheritances. Psychological Bulletin 129:858–65.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Mouden, C. E. & Gardner, A. (2011) Sixteen common misconceptions about the evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior 32:231–62.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (1992) Adaptation: Current usages. In: Keywords in evolutionary biology, ed. Keller, E. F. & Lloyd, E. A., pp. 1218. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966) Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1998) Consilience: The unity of knowledge. Knopf.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. & Daly, M. (1985) Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: The young male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology 6:5973.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yi, R., Buchhalter, A. R., Gatchalian, K. M. & Bickel, W. K. (2007) The relationship between temporal discounting and the prisoner's dilemma game in intranasal abusers of prescription opioids. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 87:9497.Google Scholar