No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Learning from instruction
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984
References
Adrian, E. D. (1928) The basis of sensations: The action of the sense organs. W. W. Norton. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. & Caplan, D. (1979) Neurolinguistics must be computational. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2:449–84. [EPS]Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1966) Discussions of papers by Israel Goldiamond, B. F. Skinner, and Arthur W. Staats. In: Problem solving: Research, method and theory, ed. Kleinmuntz, B.. Wiley. [EH]Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1969) The reward-value of indifferent stimulation. In: Reinforcement and behavior, ed. Tapp, J. T.. Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Bitterman, M. E. (1975) The comparative analysis of learning. Science 188:699–709. [NEW]Google Scholar
Bloomfield, T. M. (1969) Behavioral contrast and the peak shift. In: Animal discrimination learning, ed. Gilbert, R. M. & Sutherland, N. S.. Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Bridgman, P. W. (1952) The nature of some of our physical concepts. Philosophical Library. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1952) The conceptual framework of psychology. University of Chicago Press. [RMH]Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (1981) The validity and utility of expert judgment. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago. [RMH]Google Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R. & Rees, E. (1982) Expertise in problem solving. In: Advances in the psychology of human intelligence, vol. 1, ed. Sternberg, R. J.. Erlbaum Associates. [EH]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959) Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal behavior. Language 35:26–58. [PJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1969) Comments on Harman. In: Language and philosophy, ed. Hook, S.. New York University Press. [EPS]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1980) Rules and representations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:1–61. [EPS]Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Haviland, S. E. (1977) Comprehension and the given-new contract. In: Discourse production and comprehension, ed. Freedle, R. O.. Ablex. [EH]Google Scholar
Clark, J. H. (1963) Adaptive machines in psychiatry. In: Nerve, brain and memory models, ed. Wiener, J. & Schade, J. P.. Amsterdam. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Cohen, L. J. (in press) Semantics and the computational metaphor. In: Logic, methodology and philosophy of Science, vol. 7, ed. Marcus, R. Barcan, Dorn, G. & Weingartner, P.. North-Holland. [LJC]Google Scholar
Dawes, R. M. & Corrigan, B. (1974) Linear models in decision making. Psychological Bulletin 81:95–106. [RMH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1978) Skinner skinned. In: Brainstorms, by Dennett, D. C.. Bradford/MIT Press. [EPS]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1981) Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. MIT Press. [PJ, GK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodwell, P. C. (1977) Criteria for a neuropsychological theory of perception. Cahiers de psychologic 20:175–82. [PCD]Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J. (1980) Overconfidence in judgment. In: New directions for methodology of social and behavioral science: Fallible judgment in behavioral research, vol. 4, ed. Shweder, R.. Jossey-Bass. [RMH]Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J. & Hogarth, R. M. (1982) Prediction, diagnosis, and causal thinking in forecasting. Journal of Forecasting 1:23–36. [RMH]Google Scholar
Einhorn, H. J. & Hogarth, R. M. (1983) Diagnostic inference and causal judgment: A decision making framework. University of Chicago, Center for Decision Research. [RMH]Google Scholar
Epstein, W., ed. (1977) Stability and constancy in visual perception: Mechanisms and processes. Wiley. [SG]Google Scholar
Fantino, E., Dunn, R. & Meek, W. (1979) Percentage reinforcement and choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 32:335–40. [SG]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1980) Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:63–109. [LJC]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. & Garrett, M. F. (1974) The psychology of language. McGraw-Hill. [EPS]Google Scholar
Freeman, W. J. (1979) EEG analysis gives model of neuronal template-matching mechanism for sensory search with olfactory bulb. Biological Cybernetics 35:221–34. [SG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauld, A. & Shotter, J. (1977) Human action and its psychological investigation. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [JGR]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1975) A neural model of attention, reinforcement, and discrimination learning. International Review of Neurobiology 18:263–327. [SG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossberg, S. (1978a) Behavioral contrast in short-term memory: Serial binary memory models or parallel continuous memory models? Journal of Mathematical Psychology 17:199–219. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1978b) A theory of human memory: Self-organization and performance of sensory-motor codes, maps, and plans. In: Progress in theoretical biology, vol. 5, ed. Rosen, R. & Snell, F.. Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1980) How does a brain build a cognitive code? Psychological Review 87:1–51. [SG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossberg, S. (1982a) Processing of expected and unexpected events during conditioning and attention: A psychophysiological theory. Psychological Review 89:529–72. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1982b) A psychophysiological theory of reinforcement, drive, motivation, and attention. Journal of Theoretical Neurobiology 1:286–369. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1982c) Studies of mind and brain: Neural principles of learning, perception, development, cognition, and motor control. Reidel Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1983a) The adaptive self-organization of serial order in behavior: Speech and motor control. In: Perception of speech and visual form: Theoretical issues, models, and research, ed. Schwab, E. C. & Nusbaum, H.. Academic Press. [SG]Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1983b) Some psychophysiological and pharmacological correlates of a developmental, cognitive, and motivational theory. In: Brain and information: Event related potentials, ed. Karrer, R., Cohen, J. & Tueting, P.. New York Academy of Sciences. [SG]Google Scholar
Held, R. (1961) Exposure-history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases 132:26–32. [SG]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H. & Cable, C. (1976). Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2:235–311. [rBFS]Google Scholar
Hinson, J. M. & Staddon, J. E. R. (1978) Behavioral competition: A mechanism for schedule interactions. Science 202:432–34. [SG]Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (1981) The design of a robot mind: A theoretical approach to some issues in intelligence. In: Intelligence and learning, ed. Friedman, M. P., Das, J. P. & O'Connor, N.. Plenum Press. [EH]Google Scholar
Hunt, E. & Pixton, P. (1982) A general model for simulating information processing experiments. Proceedings of the 4th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 164–66. [EH]Google Scholar
Julià, P. (1983) Explanatory models in linguistics: A behavioral perspective. Princeton University Press. [PJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Killeen, P. (1982) Incentive theory, 2: Models for choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:217–32. [SG]Google Scholar
Larkin, J. H., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P. & Simon, H. A. (1980) Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems. Science 208:1335–42. [EH]Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1936) Principles of topological psychology. McGraw-Hill. [MK, taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Näätänen, R., Hukkanen, S. & Järvilechto, T. (1980) Magnitude of stimulus deviance and brain potentials. In: Progress in brain research, vol. 54, Motivation, motor and sensory processes of the brain, ed. Kornhuber, H. H. and Deecke, L.. Elsevier-North Holland. [SG]Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. & Ross, L. (1980) Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Prentice-Hall. [RMH, EPS]Google Scholar
Paige, J. M. & Simon, H. A. (1966). Cognitive processes in solving algebra word problems. In: Problem solving: Research, method, and theory. ed. Kleinmuntz, B.. Wiley. [EH]Google Scholar
Pelham, G. F. (1982) The psychological writings of B. F. Skinner. Doctoral dissertation, University of Bradford, England. [RH]Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1963) Conjectives and refutations: The growth of scientific knowledge. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [PCD]Google Scholar
Pounds, W. (1969) The process of problem finding. Industrial Management Review 11(1):1–19. [GK]Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1980) Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundation of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:11–169. [EPS]Google Scholar
Raaheim, K. (1961) Problem solving: A new approach. Acta Universitatis Bergensis, Series Humaniorum Litterarum, no. 5 [KR]Google Scholar
Rein, J. G. & Svartdal, F. (1979) Limitations of Skinner's concept of an “operant.” Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 20:65–70. [JGR]Google Scholar
Rogers, C. (1961) On becoming a person: A therapist's view of psychotherapy. Houghton Mifflin. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Roughead, W. G. & Scandura, J. M. (1968) “What is learned” in mathematical discovery. Journal of Educational Psychology 59:283–89. [JMS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salaman, R. A. (1957) Tradesmen's tools. In: A history of technology, vol. 3. Oxford University Press. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1964) An analysis of exposition and discovery modes of problem solving instruction. Journal of Experimental Education 33:145–48. [JMS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1969) New directions for theory and research on rule learning. II. Empirical research. Acta Psychologica 29:101–33. [JMS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1970) The role of rules in behavior: Toward an operational definition of what (rule) is learned. Psychological Review 77:516–33. [JMS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1971) Deterministic theorizing in structural learning: Three levels of empiricism. Journal of Structural Learning 3:21–53. [JMS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1973) Structural learning, vol. 1, Theory and research. Gordon & Breach Science Publishers. [JMS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1976) ed. Structural learning, vol. 2, Issues and approaches. Gordon & Breach Science Publishers. [JMS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1981) Problem solving in schools and beyond: Transitions from the naive to the neophyte to the master. Educational Psychologist 16:139–50. [JMS]Google Scholar
Seandura, J. M. (1982) Structural (cognitive task) analysis. I. Background and empirical research. Journal of Structural Learning 7:101–14. [JMS]Google Scholar
Scandura, J. M. & Scandura, A. B. (1980) Structural learning and concrete operations: An approach to Piagetian conservation. Praeger. [JMS]Google Scholar
Schick, K. (1974) Operants. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 15:413–23. [JGR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W. & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977) Controlled and automatic human information processing. 1. Detection, search and attention. Psychological Review 84:1–66. [EH]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1966) Preface to paperback ed. of The behavior of organisms. 1st ed. 1938, Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Squires, K., Wickens, C., Squires, N. & Donchin, E. (1976) The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. Science 193:1142–46. [SG]Google Scholar
Stabler, E. P. (1983) How are grammars represented? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:391–421. [EPS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., ed. (1982) Handbook of human intelligence. Cambridge University Press. [KR]Google Scholar
Stinessen, L. (1983) Intentionality, problem solving, and rule-governed behavior. Doctoral dissertation, University of Trondheim. [JGR]Google Scholar
Taube, M. (1961) Computers and common sense: The myth of thinking machines. Columbia University Press. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Tecee, J. J. (1972) Contingent negative variation (CNV) and psychological processes in man. Psychological Review 77:73–108. [SG]Google Scholar
Thomas, L. (1983) The youngest science: Notes of a medicine watcher. Viking Press. [RMH]Google Scholar
Verplanck, W. S. (1962) Unaware of where's awareness: Some verbal operants–notates, monents, and notants. In: Behavior and awareness. Duke University Press. [WSV]Google Scholar
Wallach, H. & Karsh, E. B. (1963) Why the modification of stereoscopic depth-perception is so rapid. American Journal of Psychology 76:413–20. [SG]Google Scholar
Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (1982) Language acquisition: The state of the art. Cambridge University Press. [EPS]Google Scholar