No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The influence of private interests on research in behavioural public policy: A system-level problem
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 30 August 2023
Abstract
Chater & Loewenstein argue that i-frame research has been coopted by private interests opposed to system-level reform, leading to ineffective interventions. They recommend that behavioural scientists refocus on system-level interventions. We suggest that the influence of private interests on research is problematic for wider normative and epistemic reasons. A system-level intervention to shield research from private influence is needed.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press
References
Adams, P. J. (2011). Ways in which gambling researchers receive funding from gambling industry sources. International Gambling Studies, 11(2), 145–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, L. M. (2016). Unequal democracy: The political economy of the New Gilded Age (2nd ed.). Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bright, L. K., & Heesen, R. (2023). To be scientific is to be communist. Social Epistemology, 37(3), 249–258. doi:10.1080/02691728.2022.2156308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christiano, T. (2012). Money in politics. In Estlund, D. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of political philosophy (pp. 241–260). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holman, B., & Bruner, J. P. (2015). The problem of intransigently biased agents. Philosophy of Science, 82(5), 956–968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolodny, N. (2014). Rule over none II: Social equality and the justification of democracy. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 42(4), 287–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lexchin, J., Bero, L. A., Djulbegovic, B., & Clark, O. (2003). Pharmaceutical industry sponsorship and research outcome and quality: Systematic review. BMJ, 326(7400), 1167–1170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovett, A. (2024) [Forthcoming]. Democratic failures and the ethics of democracy. University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Lovett, A., & Zuehl, J. (2022). The possibility of democratic autonomy. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 50(4), 467–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oreskes, N., & Conway, E. M. (2011). Merchants of doubt: How a handful of scientists obscured the truth on issues from tobacco smoke to global warming. Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Pinto, M. F. (2017). To know or better not to: Agnotology and the social construction of ignorance in commercially driven research. Science & Technology Studies, 30(2), 53–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viehoff, D. (2014). Democratic equality and political authority. Philosophy and Public Affairs, 42(4), 337–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weatherall, J. O., O'Connor, C., & Bruner, J. P. (2020). How to beat science and influence people: Policymakers and propaganda in epistemic networks. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 71(4), 1157–1186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Target article
The i-frame and the s-frame: How focusing on individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray
Related commentaries (33)
An inconvenient truth: Difficult problems rarely have easy solutions
Behavioral market design
Behavioral mechanism design
Behavioral public policy in practice: Misconceptions and opportunities
Behavioral winter: Disillusionment with applied behavioral science and a path to spring forward
Community-engaged research is best positioned to catalyze systemic change
Conspiracy theory
Don't throw the individual perspective out while waiting for systemic change
Expectations, opportunities, and awareness: A case for combining i- and s-frame interventions
i-Frame interventions enhance s-frame interventions
Individual-level solutions may support system-level change â if they are internalized as part of one's social identity
It's always both: Changing individuals requires changing systems and changing systems requires changing individuals
Misdiagnosing the problem of why behavioural change interventions fail
Moral psychology biases toward individual, not systemic, representations
Moving from i-frame to s-frame focus in equity, diversity, and inclusion research, practice, and policy
Nudges, regulations, and behavioral public choice
Nudging is being framed
On Skinner's pendulum: A framework for assessing s-frame hope
Optimizing behavior change through integration of individual- and system-level intervention approaches
Real systemic solutions to humanity's problems require a radical reshaping of the global political system
Structural problems require structural solutions
The influence of private interests on research in behavioural public policy: A system-level problem
The psychology and policy of overcoming economic inequality
The real cause of our complicity: The preoccupation with human weakness
The social sciences are increasingly ill-equipped to design system-level reforms
The “hearts-and-minds frame”: Not all i-frame interventions are ineffective, but education-based interventions can be particularly bad
Unpacking the nudge muddle
Use behavioral research to improve the feasibility and effectiveness of system-level policy
Using effective psychological techniques to subvert a US sociopolitical context
When nudges have societal-level impact
Why a group-level analysis is essential for effective public policy: The case for a g-frame
Wise interventions consider the person and the situation together
“More effective” is not necessarily “better”: Some ethical considerations when influencing individual behaviour
Author response
Where next for behavioral public policy?