Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T20:04:35.541Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Future-oriented objects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2019

Brandon W. Goulding
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, CanadaN2L 2V9. [email protected]@uwaterloo.cahttps://uwaterloo.ca/psychology/people-profiles/ori-friedman
Ori Friedman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, ON, CanadaN2L 2V9. [email protected]@uwaterloo.cahttps://uwaterloo.ca/psychology/people-profiles/ori-friedman

Abstract

Hoerl & McCormack suggest that saving tools does not require temporal reasoning. However, we identify a class of objects that are only possessed (i.e., saved) in anticipation of future needs. We propose that investigating these future-oriented objects may help identify temporal reasoning in populations where this ability is uncertain.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atance, C. M., Louw, A. & Clayton, N. S. (2015) Thinking ahead about where something is needed: New insights about episodic foresight in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 129:98109. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.09.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atance, C. M., Metcalf, J. L. & Thiessen, A. J. (2017) How can we help children save? Tell them they can (if they want to). Cognitive Development 43:6779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atance, C. M. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005) My future self: Young children's ability to anticipate and explain future states. Cognitive Development 20(3):341–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2005.05.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atance, C. M. & O'Neill, D. K. (2005) Preschoolers’ talk about future situations. First Language 25(1):518. https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723705045678.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beran, M. J., Hopper, L. M., de Waal, F. B., Sayers, K. & Brosnan, S. F. (2016) Chimpanzee food preferences, associative learning, and the origins of cooking. Learning & Behavior 44(2):103108. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0206-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coolidge, F. L. & Wynn, T. (2016) An introduction to cognitive archaeology. Current Directions in Psychological Science 25(6):386–92. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0963721416657085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kabadayi, C. & Osvath, M. (2017) Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science 357(6347):202204. doi:10.1126/science.aam8138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCormack, T. & Atance, C. M. (2011) Planning in young children: A review and synthesis. Developmental Review 31(1):131. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.02.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalf, J. L. & Atance, C. M. (2011) Do preschoolers save to benefit their future selves? Cognitive Development 26(4):371–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.09.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulcahy, N. J. & Call, J. (2006) Apes save tools for future use. Science 312(5776):1038–40. doi:10.1126/science.1125456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osvath, M. & Gärdenfors, P. (2005) Oldowan culture and the evolution of anticipatory cognition. Lund University Cognitive Studies 122:116. www.lucs.lu.se/LUCS/122/LUCS.122.pdf.Google Scholar
Russell, J., Alexis, D. & Clayton, N. (2010) Episodic future thinking in 3-to 5-year-old children: The ability to think of what will be needed from a different point of view. Cognition 114(1):5671. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shick, K. D. (1987) Modeling the formation of Early Stone Age artifact concentrations. Journal of Human Evolution 16(7–8):789807. https://doi.org/10.1016/0047-2484(87)90024-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Busby, J. (2005) Making decisions with the future in mind: Developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learning and Motivation 36(2):110–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2005.02.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (2007a) The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30(3):299313. doi:10.1017/s0140525X07001975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M. & von Gehlen, R. (2011) Children's capacity to remember a novel problem and to secure its future solution. Developmental Science 14(1):2633. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00950.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, N. & Schick, K. (2018) An overview of the cognitive implications of the Oldowan Industrial Complex. Azania: Archaeological Research in Africa, 53(1):339. https://doi.org/10.1080/0067270X.2018.1439558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F. & Rosati, A. G. (2015) Cognitive capacities for cooking in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282(1809):20150229. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wobber, V., Hare, B. & Wrangham, R. (2008) Great apes prefer cooked food. Journal of Human Evolution 55(2):340–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.03.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed