Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T15:26:25.276Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Experiential effects on mirror systems and social learning: Implications for social intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2014

Simon M. Reader*
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, McGill University, Montréal, QC H3A 1B1, Canada. [email protected]://biology.mcgill.ca/faculty/reader/ Department of Biology and Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, 3508 TB Utrecht, The Netherlands.

Abstract

Investigations of biases and experiential effects on social learning, social information use, and mirror systems can usefully inform one another. Unconstrained learning is predicted to shape mirror systems when the optimal response to an observed act varies, but constraints may emerge when immediate error-free responses are required and evolutionary or developmental history reliably predicts the optimal response. Given the power of associative learning, such constraints may be rare.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, L. & Chappell, J. (2002) Inhibition of optimal behavior by social transmission in the guppy depends on shoaling. Behavioral Ecology 13:827–31.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brown, E. C. & Brüne, M. (2012) The role of prediction in social neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6:147. doi:10.3389/Fnhum.2012.00147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W. (2002) Imitation of novel complex actions: What does the evidence from animals mean? Advances in the Study of Behavior 31:77105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, C. A. & Whiten, A. (2002) Evolutionary perspectives on imitation: Is a comparative psychology of social learning possible? Animal Cognition 5:193208.Google Scholar
Church, R. M. (1957) Transmission of learned behavior between rats. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 54:163–65.Google Scholar
Davies, N. B. & Welbergen, J. A. (2009) Social transmission of a host defense against cuckoo parasitism. Science 324:1318–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, E. H., Avargues-Weber, A., Chittka, L. & Leadbeater, E. (2013) Learning by observation emerges from simple associations in an insect model. Current Biology 23:727–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. & Shultz, S. (2007) Evolution in the social brain. Science 317:1344–47.Google Scholar
Dunlap, A. S. & Stephens, D. W. (2009) Components of change in the evolution of learning and unlearned preference. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 276:3201–208.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (1994) Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews 69:207–31.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2012a) Grist and mills: On the cultural origins of cultural learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 367:2181–91.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2012c) What's social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology 126:193202.Google Scholar
Horn, G. (2004) Pathways of the past: The imprint of memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5:108–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, T. D. (1982) The selective costs and benefits of learning: An evolutionary analysis. Advances in the Study of Behaviour 12:65106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katsnelson, E., Motro, U., Feldman, M. W. & Lotem, A. (2008) Early experience affects producer-scrounger foraging tendencies in the house sparrow. Animal Behaviour 75:1465–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I. & Laland, K. N. (2009) Adaptive trade-offs in the use of social and personal information. In: Cognitive ecology II, ed. Dukas, R. & Ratcliffe, J. M., pp. 249–71. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Keysers, C. & Perrett, D. I. (2004) Demystifying social cognition: A Hebbian perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(11):501507.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Bateson, P. (2001) The mechanisms of imitation. Cybernetics and Systems 32:195224.Google Scholar
Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. (2007) Social learning in insects – From miniature brains to consensus building. Current Biology 17:R703–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lefebvre, L. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (1996) Is social learning an adaptive specialization? In: Social learning in animals: The roots of culture, ed. Heyes, C. M. & Galef, B. G. Jr., pp. 107–28. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lindeyer, C. M., Meaney, M. J. & Reader, S. M. (2013) Early maternal care predicts reliance on social learning about food in adult rats. Developmental Psychobiology 55:168–75.Google Scholar
Miller, N. & Dollard, J. (1941) Social learning and imitation. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Mineka, S. & Cook, M. (1988) Social learning and the acquisition of snake fear in monkeys. In: Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives, ed. Zentall, T. & Galef, B. G. Jr., pp. 5173. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Morand-Ferron, J., Doligez, B., Dall, S. R. X. & Reader, S. M. (2010) Social information use. In: Encyclopedia of animal behavior, vol. 3, ed. Breed, M. D. & Moore, J., pp. 242–50. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mottley, K. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2000) Experimental evidence that group foragers can converge on predicted producer–scrounger equilibria. Animal Behaviour 60:341–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mui, R., Haselgrove, M., Pearce, J. & Heyes, C. (2008) Automatic imitation in budgerigars. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275(1651): 2547–53.Google Scholar
Offerman, T. & Schotter, A. (2009) Imitation and luck: An experimental study on social sampling. Games and Economic Behavior 65:461502.Google Scholar
Pigliucci, M. & Kaplan, J. (2000) The fall and rise of Dr Pangloss: Adaptationism and the Spandrels paper 20 years later. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15:6670.Google Scholar
Range, F., Huber, L. & Heyes, C. (2011) Automatic imitation in dogs. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278(1703):211–17.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. & Biro, D. (2010) Experimental identification of social learning in wild animals. Learning and Behavior 38:265–83.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., Hager, Y. & Laland, K. N. (2011) The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 366:1017–27.Google Scholar
Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W. J. E., Morgan, T. J. H., Webster, M. M. & Laland, K. N. (2011) Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15:6876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rieucau, G. & Giraldeau, L. A. (2009) Persuasive companions can be wrong: The use of misleading social information in nutmeg mannikins. Behavioral Ecology 20:1217–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar