Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T18:47:24.543Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evolution after mirror neurons: Tapping the shared manifold through secondary adaptation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2014

Matthew M. Gervais*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Center for Behavior, Evolution, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095. [email protected]://sites.google.com/site/matthewmgervais/

Abstract

Cook et al. laudably call for careful comparative research into the development of mirror neurons. However, they do so within an impoverished evolutionary framework that does not clearly distinguish ultimate and proximate causes and their reciprocal relations. As a result, they overlook evidence for the reliable develop of mirror neurons in biological and cultural traits evolved to work through them.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atkinson, Q. D. & Whitehouse, H. (2011) The cultural morphospace of ritual form: Examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evolution and Human Behavior 32(1):5062.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2012) A hierarchical model of the evolution of human brain specializations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109:10733–40.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. & Broesch, J. (2012) Prepared social learning about dangerous animals in children. Evolution and Human Behavior 33(5):499508.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Tal, J., Ludemann, P., Toda, S., Rahn, C. W., Pêcheux, M. G., Azuma, H. & Vardi, D. (1992) Maternal responsiveness to infants in three societies: The United States, France, and Japan. Child Development 63(4):808–21.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J. & Henrich, J. (2011) The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 108(Suppl. 2):10918–25.Google Scholar
Brand, R. J. & Shallcross, W. L. (2008) Infants prefer motionese to adult-directed action. Developmental Science 11(6):853–61.Google Scholar
Bryant, G. A. & Barrett, H. C. (2007) Recognizing intentions in infant-directed speech evidence for universals. Psychological Science 18(8):746–51.Google Scholar
Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (1996) The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In: Reaching into thought: The minds of the Great Apes, ed. Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., & Parker, S. T., pp. 371403. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C. & Lenzi, G. L. (2003) Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100(9):5497–502.Google Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Allcock, B., Thomas, C. & Bard, K. A. (2011) Aping expressions? Chimpanzees produce distinct laugh types when responding to laughter of others. Emotion 11(5):1013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ionica, C. & Suomi, S. J. (2009b) Reciprocal face-to-face communication between rhesus macaque mothers and their newborn infants. Current Biology 19(20):1768–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A. P. (1992) The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review 99(4):689.Google Scholar
Gallese, V. (2003) The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology 36(4):171–80.Google Scholar
Garcia, J. & Koelling, R. A. (1966) Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science 4(1):123–24.Google Scholar
Gervais, M. & Wilson, D. S. (2005) The evolution and functions of laughter and humor: A synthetic approach. The Quarterly Review of Biology 80(4):395430.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Vrba, E. S. (1982) Exaptation – a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8(1):415.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. (2010) The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33(2–3):6183.Google Scholar
Kircher, T., Pohl, A., Krach, S., Thimm, M., Schulte-Rüther, M., Anders, S. & Mathiak, K. (2013) Affect-specific activation of shared networks for perception and execution of facial expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8(4):370377.Google Scholar
Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M. & Chartrand, T. L. (2003) The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 27(3):145–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N., Sterelny, K., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W. & Uller, T. (2011) Cause and effect in biology revisited: Is Mayr's proximate-ultimate dichotomy still useful? Science 334(6062):1512–16.Google Scholar
Mancini, G., Ferrari, P. F. & Palagi, E. (2013) Rapid facial mimicry in Geladas. Scientific Reports 3(1527):16.Google Scholar
Mui, R., Haselgrove, M., Pearce, J. & Heyes, C. (2008) Automatic imitation in budgerigars. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275(1651): 2547–53.Google Scholar
Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (1984) Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories and their implications. In: Culture Theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion, ed. Shweder, R. A. & Levine, R. A., pp. 276320. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M. & Vick, S. J. (2007) New developments in understanding emotional facial signals in chimpanzees. Current Directions in Psychological Science 16(3):117–22.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C. & Dapretto, M. (2008) Mirroring others' emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage 39(4):2076–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preston, S. D. & de Waal, F. (2002) Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(1):120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Range, F., Huber, L. & Heyes, C. (2011) Automatic imitation in dogs. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278(1703):211–17.Google Scholar
Russell, J. L., Lyn, H., Schaeffer, J. A. & Hopkins, W. D. (2011) The role of socio-communicative rearing environments in the development of social and physical cognition in apes. Developmental Science 14(6):1459–70.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E. & Herrmann, E. (2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation. Current Anthropology 53(6):673–92.Google Scholar
Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M. & Umiltà, C. (1996) Face preference at birth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22(4):892903.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G, Wrangham, R. & Boesch, C. (2001) Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour 138(11):1481–516.Google Scholar
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G. & Aiguier, C. J. (2003) Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40:655–64.Google Scholar