Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T15:35:42.535Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Confounding the origin and function of mirror neurons

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 April 2014

Giacomo Rizzolatti*
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Brain Center for Social and Motor Cognition (BCSMC), Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia (IIT), 43125 Parma, Italy. [email protected]

Abstract

Cook et al. argue that mirror neurons originate in sensorimotor associative learning and that their function is determined by their origin. Both these claims are hard to accept. It is here suggested that a major role in the origin of the mirror mechanism is played by top-down connections rather than by associative learning.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Damasio, A. & Meyer, K. (2008) Behind the looking-glass. Nature 450:167–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B. & Keysers, C. (2007) The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage 35(4):1674–84.Google Scholar
Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1998) Early exploration of the visual cortex. Neuron 20:401–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (1997) The cognitive neuroscience of action. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J. & Frith, C. D. (2007b) The mirror-neuron system: A Bayesian perspective. NeuroReport 18:619–23.Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982) Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198(4312):7578.Google Scholar
Peeters, R., Simone, L., Nelissen, K., Fabbri-Destro, M., Vanduffel, W., Rizzolatti, G. & Orban, G. A. (2009) The representation of tool use in humans and monkeys: Common and uniquely human features. Journal of Neuroscience 29:11523–39.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Sinigaglia, C. (2010) The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(4):264–74. doi: 10.1038/nrn2805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochat, M. J., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., Escola, L., Intskirveli, I., Grammont, F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G. & Umiltà, M. A. (2010) Responses of mirror neurons in area F5 to hand and tool grasping observation. Experimental Brain Research 204(4):605–16.Google Scholar
Van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C. & Bekkering, H. (2008) You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. NeuroImage 43:808–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.057.Google Scholar