Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T21:29:27.739Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Choice, optimal foraging, and the delay-reduction hypothesis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Edmund Fantino
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University ol California, San Diego, La Jolla, Calif. 92093
Nureya Abarca
Affiliation:
Escuela de Psicologia, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile

Abstract

Behaving organisms are continually choosing. Recently the theoretical and empirical study of decision making by behavioral ecologists and experimental psychologists have converged in the area of foraging, particularly food acquisition. This convergence has raised the interdisciplinary question of whether principles that have emerged from the study of decision making in the operant conditioning laboratory are consistent with decision making in naturally occurring foraging. One such principle, the “parameter-free delay-reduction hypothesis, ” developed in studies of choice in the operant conditioning laboratory, states that the effectiveness of a stimulus as a reinforcer may be predicted most accurately by calculating the decrease in time to food presentation correlated with the onset of the stimulus, relative to the length of time to food presentation measured from the onset of the preceding stimulus. Since foraging involves choice, the delay-reduction hypothesis may be extended to predict aspects of foraging. We discuss the strategy of assessing parameters of foraging with operant laboratory analogues to foraging. We then compare the predictions of the delay-reduction hypothesis with those of optimal foraging theory, developed by behavioral ecologists, showing that, with two exceptions, the two positions make comparable predictions. The delay-reduction hypothesis is also compared to several contemporary pscyhological accounts of choice. Results from several of our experiments with pigeons, designed as operant conditioning simulations of foraging, have shown the following: The more time subjects spend searching for or traveling between potential food sources, the less selective they become, that is, the more likely they are to accept the less preferred outcome; increasing time spent procuring (“handling”) food increases selectivity; how often the preferred outcome is available has a greater effect on choice then how often the less preferred outcome is available; subjects maximize reinforcement whether it is the rate, amount, or probability of reinforcement that is varied; there are no significant differences between subjects performing under different types of deprivation (open vs. closed economies). These results are all consistent with the delay-reduction hypothesis. Moreover, they suggest that the technology of the operant conditioning laboratory may have fruitful application in the study of foraging, and, in doing so, they underscore the importance of an interdisciplinary approach to behavior.

Type
Target article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1985

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abarca, N. & Fantino, E. (1982) Choice and foraging. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:117–23. [MNB, TC. tarEF, BFP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abarca, N., Fantino, E. & Ito, M. (1985) Percentage reward in an operant analogue to foraging. Animal Behaviour 33. In press. [tarEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abs, M., ed. (1983) Physiology and behaviour of the pigeon. Academic Press. [DMW]Google Scholar
Allison, J., Miller, M. & Wozny, M. (1979) Conservation in behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108:434. [BFP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, J. (1974) Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behavior 49:227–62. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amsel, A. (1967) Partial reinforcement effects on vigor and persistence. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 1, ed. Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T.. Academic Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
Arnold, S. J. (1981) The microevolution of feeding behavior. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T.. Garland Press. [MBS]Google Scholar
Autor, S. M. (1960) The strength of conditioned reinforcers as a function of frequency and probability of reinforcement. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University. [tarEF]Google Scholar
Autor, S. M. (1969) The strength of conditioned reinforcers as a function of frequency and probability of reinforcement. In: Conditioned reinforcement, ed. Hendry, D. P.. Dorsey Press. [taEF]Google Scholar
Balsam, P. (1984) Relative time in trace conditioning. In: Timing and time perception, ed. Gibbon, J. & Allan, L.. New York Academy of Sciences. [BLB, rEF]Google Scholar
Barker, L. M., Best, M. R. & Domjan, M., eds. (1977) Learning mechanisms in food selection. Baylor University Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
Barnard, C. J. (1984) Snap decisions on survival. New Scientist 102(1411):2427. [CJB]Google Scholar
Barnard, C. J. & Brown, C. A. J. (1981) Prey size selection and competition in the common shrew (Sorex araneus L.). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 8:239–43. [CJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnard, C. J. & Brown, C. A. J. (in press a) Risk-sensitive foraging in common shrews (Sorex araneus L.). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology.. [CJB]Google Scholar
Barnard, C. J. & Brown, C. A. J. (in press b) Competition affects risk-sensitivity in foraging shrews. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. [CJB]Google Scholar
Bauer, S. J. & Sokolowski, M. B. (1984) Larval foraging behaviour in isofemale lines of Drosophila melanogaster and D. pseudobscura. Journal of Heredity 75:131–34. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, W. M. (1972) Choice in a continuous procedure. Psychonomic Science 28:263–65. [MD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, W. M. (1982) Choice, changeover, and travel. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:3549. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, W. M. (1983) Studying foraging in the psychological laboratory. In: Animal cognition and behavior, ed. Mellgren, R. L.. North-Holland. [taEF, BFF]Google Scholar
Belovsky, G. E. (1978) Diet optimization in a generalist herbivore: The moose. Theoretical Population Biology 14:105–34. [TC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, A. B. (1983) The foraging behaviour of lacewing larvae on vertical rods. Animal Behaviour 31:9901004. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bovet, P. (1979) La valeur adaptative des comportements aléatoires. L'Année psychologique 79:505–25. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broom, D. M. (1981) Biology of behaviour: Mechanisms, functions and applications. Cambridge University Press. [JH]Google Scholar
Brown, B. L., Hemmes, N. S., Coleman, D. A. Jr, Hassin, A. & Goldhammer, E. (1982) Specification of the stimulus-reinforcer relation in multiple schedules: Delay and probability of reinforcement. Animal Learning and Behavior 10:365–76. [BLB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. G. B. (1969) Seed selection by pigeons. Behaviour 34:115–31. [SJS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunswick, E. (1939) Probability as a determiner of rat behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology 25:175–97. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capaldi, E. J. (1967) A sequential hypothesis of instrumental learning. In: The psychology of learning & motivation, vol. 1, ed. Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T.. Academic Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
Caraco, T. (1980) On foraging time allocation in a stochastic environment. Ecology 61:119–28. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caraco, T.(1981) Energy budgets, risk and foraging preferences in dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 8:213–17. [CJB, tarEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caraco, T. (1982) Aspects of risk-aversion in foraging white-crowned sparrows. Animal Behaviour 30:719–27. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caraco, T. (1983) White-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys): Foraging preferences in a risky environment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 12:6369. [CJB, taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caraco, T., Martindale, S. & Whittam, T. S. (1980) An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behaviour 28:820–30. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, D. & Fantino, E. (1981) The delay-reduction hypothesis of conditioned reinforcement and punishment: Observing behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 35:93108. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, L. & Davison, M. (1982) On the effects of component durations and component reinforcer rates in multiple schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 37:417–39. [MD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, L. & Davison, M. (1983) On the effects of food deprivation and component reinforcer rates on multiple-schedule performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 40:239–51. [MD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charnov, E. L. (1976a) Optimal foraging: Attack strategy of a mantid. American Naturalist 110:141–51. (TC, taEF, AK, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charnov, E. L. (1976b) Optimal foraging: The marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology 9:129–36. [taEF, AK, JMK, PRK, JNM, RLM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collier, G. H., Hirsch, E. & Hamlin, P. H. (1972) The ecological determinants of reinforcement in the rat. Physiology and Behavior 9:705–16. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collier, G. H. & Rovee-Collier, C. K. (1981) A comparative analysis of optimal foraging behavior: Laboratory simulation. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T.. Garland Press.[taEF, JMK, MBS]Google Scholar
Collier, G. H. & Rovee-Collier, C. K. (1983) An ecological perspective of reinforcement and motivation. In: Handbook of behavioural neurobiology, vol. 6, ed. Satinoff, E. & Teitelbaum, P.. Plenum Press. [taEF, NR]Google Scholar
Cook, R. M. & Cockrell, B. J. (1978) Predator ingestion rate and its bearing on feeding time and the theory of optimal diets. Journal of Animal Ecology 47:529–47. [JH, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowie, R. J. (1977) Optimal foraging in great tits (Parus major). Nature 268:137–39. [taEF, AK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, N. B. & Houston, A. I. (1981) Owners and satellites: The economics of territory defence in the pied wagtail, Motacilla alba. Journal of Animal Ecology 50:157–80. [CJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davison, M. (1969) Preference for mixed-interval versus fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:247–52. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davison, M. (1972) Preference for mixed-interval versus fixed-interval: Number of component intervals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 17:169–76. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davison, M. (in press) The analysis of concurrent-chain performance. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior, vol. 5: The effects of delay and of intervening events, ed. Commons, M. L., Nevin, J. A., Rachlin, H. C. & Mazur, J. E.. Ballinger. [MD]Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1977) Escape, avoidance, punishment: Where do we stand? Journal of the Experimental Analyses of Behavior 28:8395. [JAD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1983) Observing and conditioned reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:693728. [tarEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Domjan, M. & Burkhard, B. (1982) The principles of learning and behavior. Brooks/Cole. [JAD]Google Scholar
Donovan, W. J. (1978) Structure and function of the pigeon visual system. Physiological Psychology 6:403–37. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, R. & Fantino, E. (1982) Choice and the relative immediacy of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:321–26. [tarEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, R. M. (1982) Choice, relative reinforcer duration, and the changeover ratio. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:313–19. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrman, L. & Parsons, P. A. (1976) The genetics of behavior. Sinauer Associates. [MBS]Google Scholar
Elliffe, D. & Davison, M. (submitted) Performance in continuously availablemultiple schedules. [MD]Google Scholar
Elner, R. W. & Hughes, R. N. (1978) Energy maximization in the diet of the shore crab, Carcinus maenas. Journal of Animal Ecology 47:103–16. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emlen, J. M. (1966) The role of time and energy in food preference. American Naturalist 100:611–17. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, R. (in press) Simulation research in the analysis of behavior. In: Control of human behavior: Issues in applied behavior analysis research methodology, vol. 4, ed. Poling, A., Fuqua, R. W. & Ulrich, R.. Scott, Foresman. [MSa]Google Scholar
Erichsen, J. T., Krebs, J. R. & Houston, A. L. (1980) Optimal foraging and cryptic prey. Journal of Animal Ecology 49:271–76. [AK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essock, S. & Reese, E. P. (1974) Preference for and effects of variable — as opposed to fixed — reinforcer duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:8997. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estes, W. K. & Skinner, B. F. (1941) Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology 29:390400. [BLB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eward, J. M. (1913) Is the appetite of swine a reliable indication of physiological needs? Proceedings of the Iowa Academy of Sciences 22:375403. [TWC]Google Scholar
Falconer, D. S. (1970) Introduction to quantitative genetics. Oliver and Boyd. [MBS]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. (1967) Preference for mixed versus fixed-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 10:3543. [tarEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantino, E. (1969) Choice and rate of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:723–30. [tarEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantino, E. (1977) Conditioned reinforcement: Choice and information. In: Handbook of operant behavior, ed. Honig, W. K. & Staddon, J. E. R.. Prentice-Hall. [taEF]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. (1981a) Contiguity, response strength, and the delay-reduction hypothesis. In: Advances in analysis of behavior, vol. 2: Predictability, correlation and contiguity, ed. Harzem, P. & Zeiler, M. D.. John Wiley & Sons. [taEF]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. (1981b) Is maximization theory general, and is it refutable? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:390–91. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fantino, E. (1982) Effects of initial-link length on responding in terminal link. Behaviour Analysis Letters 2:6570. [tarEF]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. (1984) Operant conditioning simulations of foraging and the delay-reduction hypothesis. Paper presented at Foraging Behavior Conference, Providence, R.I. [rEF]Google Scholar
Fantino, E., Abarca, N. & Dunn, R. (in press) The delay-reduction hypothesis: Extensions to foraging and three-alternative choice. In: Quantitative analyses of behavior, vol. 5: Beinforcement value: Delay and intervening events, ed. Commons, M. L., Mazur, J. E., Nevin, J. A., & Rachlin, H.. Erlbaum. [tarEF]Google Scholar
Fantino, E. & Davison, M. (1983) Choice: Some quantitative relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 40:113. [tarEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantino, E. & Dunn, R. (1983) The delay-reduction hypothesis: Extension to three-alternative choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 9:132–46. [TC, tarEF, AIH]Google Scholar
Fantino, E., Dunn, R. & Meck, W. (1979) Percentage reinforcement and choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 32:335–40. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fantino, E. & Logan, C. (1979) The experimental analysis of behavior: A biological perspective. W. H. Freeman. [tarEF]Google Scholar
Felton, M. & Lyon, D. O. (1966) The post-reinforcement pause. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9:131–34. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferster, C. B. & Skinner, B. F. (1957) Schedules of reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galef, B. C. Jr. (1976) Social transmission of acquired behavior: A discussion of tradition and social learning in vertebrates. In: Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 6., ed. Rosenblatt, J., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C. & Shaw, E.. Academic Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
Carcia, J., Kimmeldorf, D. J. & Koelling, R. A. (1955) Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science 122:157–58. [BLB]Google Scholar
Gass, C. L. (1984) Behavioural foundations of adaptation. In: Perspectives in ethology, vol. 6, ed. Bateson, P. P. C. & Klopfer, P. H.. Plenum Press. [CLG]Google Scholar
Gass, C. L. & Montgomerie, R. D. (1981) Hummingbird foraging behavior: Decision-making and energy regulation. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D.. Garland Press. [CLG, DMW]Google Scholar
Gass, C. L. & Sutherland, G. D. (in press) Specialization by territorial hummingbirds on experimentally enriched patches of flowers: Profitability and learning. Canadian Journal of Zoology. [CLG]Google Scholar
George, C. (1971) Choix et apprentissage en situation aléatoire. Paris: C.N.R.S. [PB]Google Scholar
Gibb, J. (1960) Populations of tits and goldcrests and their food supply in pine plantations. Ibis 102:163208. [JH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbon, J. (1977) Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychological Review 84:279325. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbon, J. & Balsam, P. (1981) Spreading association in time. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory, ed. Lucurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. & Gibbon, J.. Academic Press. [BLB, taEF]Google Scholar
Gibbon, J., Berryman, R. & Thompson, R. L. (1974) Contingency spaces and measures in classical and instrumental conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:585605. [JAD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gill, F. B. & Wolf, L. L. (1977) Nonrandom foraging by sunbirds in a patchy environment. Ecology 58:1284–96. [JMK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciller, P. S. (1980) The control of handling time and its effects on the foraging strategy of a heteropteran predator, Notonecta. Journal of Animal Ecology 49:699712. [JNM]Google Scholar
Goodwin, D. (1970) Pigeons and doves of the world. British Museum. [DMW]Google Scholar
Goodwin, D. (1983) Behaviour. In: Physiology and behaviour of the pigeon, ed. Abs, M.. Academic Press. [DMW]Google Scholar
Goss-Custard, J. D. (1977) Optimal foraging and the size selection of worms by redshank, Tringa totanus, in the field. Animal Behaviour 25:1029. [CJB, taEF, JH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. C. (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptionist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 205:581–98. [JNM]Google Scholar
Granda, A. M. & Maxwell, M. A., eds. (1979) Neural mechanisms of behavior in the pigeon. Plenum Press. [DMW]Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1982) The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford University Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
Gray, L. (1981) Genetic and experiential differences affecting foraging behavior. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T.. Garland Press. [MBS]Google Scholar
Gray, R. (1984) Faith and foraging: A critique of optimal foraging theory. Paper presented at the Foraging Behavior Conference, Providence, R.I. [JH]Google Scholar
Griffiths, D. (1982) Tests of alternative models of prey consumption by predators, using ant-lion larvae. Journal of Animal Ecology 52:363–73. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, W. G. & Williams, C. L. (1983) Suckling isn't feeding, or is it? A search for developmental continuities. In: Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 13, ed. Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., Beer, C. & Busnel, M.-C.. Academic Press. [NR]Google Scholar
Hall-Johnson, E. & Poling, A. (1984) Preference in pigeons given a choice between sequences of fixed-ratio schedules: Effects of ratio values and duration of food delivery. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 42:127–35. [rEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanson, J. & Green, L. (1984a) Tests of optimal foraging using an operant analogue. Paper presented at the Association for Behavior Analysis meeting, Nashville, Tenn. [JH]Google Scholar
Hanson, J. & Green, L. (1984b) Patch choice by pigeons. Paper presented at the Foraging Behavior Conference, Providence, R.I. [JH]Google Scholar
Hanson, J. & Green, L. (1984c) Foraging decisions: Patch choice by pigeons. In preparation. [JH]Google Scholar
Hanson, S. J. & Timberlake, W. (1983) Regulation during challenge: A general model of learned performance under schedule constraint. Psychological Review 56:261–82. [BFP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1964a) Secondary reinforcement and the rate of primary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7:2736. [tarEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrnstein, R. J. (1964b) Aperiodicity as a factor in choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7:179–82. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrnstein, R. J. & Loveland, D. H. (1974) Hunger and contrast in a multiple schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21:511–17. [MD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. & Vaughan, W. (1980) Melioration and behavioral allocation. In: Limits to action: The allocation of individual behavior, ed. Staddon, J. E. R.. Academic Press. [CJB]Google Scholar
Hewitt, S. K., Fulker, D. W. & Hewitt, C. A. (1983) Genetic architecture of olfactory discriminative avoidance conditioning in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Comparative Psychology 97:5258. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsch, J. & McCauley, L. A. (1977) Successful replication of and selective breeding for classical conditioning in the blowfly Phormia regina. Animal Behaviour 25:784–85. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honig, W. K. (1966) Operant behavior: Areas of research and application. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taEF]Google Scholar
Honig, W. K. & Staddon, J. E. R. (1977) Handbook of operant behavior. Prentice-Hall. [taEF]Google Scholar
Houston, A. I. (1980) Godzilla v. the creature from the black lagoon: Ethology v. psychology. In: Analysis of motivational processes, ed. Toates, F. M. & Halliday, T. R.. Academic Press. [MSn]Google Scholar
Houston, A. I. (unpub.) Choice and preference. [AIH]Google Scholar
Houston, A. L., Krebs, J. R. & Erichsen, J. T. (1980) Optimal prey choice and discrimination time in the great tit (Parus major L). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 6:169–75. [AK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houston, A. I. & McNamara, J. M. (1981) How to maximize reward rate on two variable interval paradigms. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 35:367–96. [AIH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houston, A. I. & McNamara, J. M. (1982) A sequential approach to risk-taking. Animal Behaviour 30:1260–61. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, D. J. & Hartl, D. L. (1980) Optimal foraging in glossophagine bats: When to give up. American Naturalist 115:696704. [JMK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, R. N. (1979) Optimal diets under the energy maximization premise: The effects of recognition time and learning. American Naturalist 113:209–21. [TC, AK, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, I. & Davison, M. (in press) Determination of a behavioral transfer function: White noise analysis of session to session response ratio dynamics on concurrent VI–VI schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. [SEGL]Google Scholar
Hursh, S. R. (1980) Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 34:219–38. [MNB, taEF, BFP, MSa]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hursh, S. R. & Fantino, E. (1973) Relative delay of reinforcement and choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 19:437–50. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ito, M. & Asaki, K. (1982) Choice behavior of rats in a concurrent-chains schedule: Amount and delay of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 37:383–92. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ito, M. & Fantino, E. (1983) Choice, foraging and reinforcer duration. In preparation. [taEF]Google Scholar
Iwasa, Y., Higashi, M. & Yamamura, N. (1981) Prey distribution as a factor determining the choice of optimal foraging strategy. American Naturalist 117:710–23. [RFC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, H. M., Barnes, R. A. & Barrera, F. J. (1981) Why autoshaping depends on trial spacing. In: Autoshaping and conditioning theory, ed. Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. & Gibbon, J.. Academic Press. [taEF]Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M. & Shattuck, D. (1981) Contingency in fear conditioning: A reexamination. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 17:159–62. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagel, J. H., McDonald, D. N., Battalio, R. C., White, S. & Green, L. (in preparation). Effects of varying energy budgets on risk preferences in rats. [CJB]Google Scholar
Kamil, A. C. (1983) Optimal foraging theory and the psychology of learning. American Zoologist 23:291302. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D. (1981) Foraging Behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches. Garland Press. [taEF, RFG, MBS]Google Scholar
Kamil, A. C. & Yoerg, S. I. (1982) Learning and foraging behaviour. In: Perspectives in ethology, vol. 5, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P. H.. Plenum Press. [CLG]Google Scholar
Kay, D., Hemmes, N. S. & Brown, B. L. (1984) Choice measures and the assessment of temporal control in autoshaping. Proceedings and Abstracts of the 1984 Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association 55:32. [BLB]Google Scholar
Kendall, S. B. (1974) Preference for intermittent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:463–73. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killeen, P. R. (1968) On the measurement of reinforcement frequency in the study of preference. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11:263–69. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Killeen, P. R. (1982a) Incentive theory. In: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, ed. Bernstein, D. J.. University of Nebraska Press. [PRK, SEGL]Google Scholar
Killeen, P. R. (1982b) Incentive theory 2: Models for choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:217–32. [taEF, PRK, SEGL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killeen, P. R. (1984) Incentive theory 3: Adaptive clocks. In: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol. 423: Timing and time perception, ed. Gibbon, J. & Allan, L.. New York Academy of Sciences. [PRK]Google Scholar
Killeen, P. R., Smith, J. P. & Hanson, S. J. (1981) Central place foraging in Rattus norvegicus. Animal Behaviour 29:6470. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korber, E. J. (1983) The foraging behavior of a Columba livia flock as a function of resource allocation and patch size. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association Meeting, Philadelphia. [DMW]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. (1973) Behavioral aspects of predation. In: Perspectives in ethology, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P. H.. Plenum Press. [RLM]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. (1978) Optimal foraging: Decision rules for predators. In: Behavioural ecology, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B.. Blackwell. [taEF]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (1981) An introduction to behavioural ecology. Blackwell. [taEF]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Erichsen, J. T., Webber, M. I. & Charnov, E. L. (1977) Optimal prey selection in the great tit (Parus major). Animal Behavior 25:3038. [CJB, taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Houston, A. I. & Charnov, E. L. (1981) Some recent developments in optimal foraging. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D.. Garland Press. [taEF]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Kacelnik, A. & Taylor, P. (1978) Test of optimal sampling by foraging great tits. Nature 275:2731. [CJB, PB, SEGL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Kacelnik, A. & Taylor, P. (1981) Correlation and contiguity in foraging behaviour. In: Advances in analysis of behaviour, vol. 2: Predictability, correlation, and contiguity, ed. Harzem, P. & Zeiler, M. D.. John Wiley & Sons. [CJB, taEF]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & McClecry, R. H. (1984) Optimization in behavioural ecology. In: Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, 2d ed., ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B.. Blackwell. [TC]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Ryan, J. C. & Charnov, E. L. (1974) Hunting by expectation or optimal foraging? A study of patch use by chickadees. Animal Behavior 22:953–64. [rEF, RFG, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Stephens, D. W. & Sutherland, W. J. (1983) Perspectives in optimal foraging. In: Perspectives in ornithology, ed. Brush, A. H. & Clarke, G. A.. Cambridge University Press. [CJB, taEF, DMW]Google Scholar
Lea, S. E. G. (1976) Titration of schedule parameters by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 25:4354. [SEGL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lea, S. E. G. (1979) Foraging and reinforcement schedules in the pigeon: Optimal and non-optimal aspects of choice. Animal Behavior 27:875–86. [taEF, AIH, JMK, BFP, SJS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, S. E. G. & Dow, S. M. (1984) The integration of reinforcements over time. In: Timing and time perception, ed. Gibbon, J. & Allan, L.. New York Academy of Sciences. [rEF, SEGL]Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L. (1983) Equilibrium distribution of feral pigeons at multiple food sources. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 12:1117. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1970) The units of selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 1:118. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, J. R. (1983) The role of foraging time constraints and variable prey encounter in optimal diet choice. American Naturalist 122:191209. [TC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. (1977) The choice axiom after twenty years. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 15:215–33. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacArthur, R. H. & Pianka, E. R. (1966) On optimal use of a patchy environment. American Naturalist 100:603–9. [taEF, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuire, T. R. & Hirsch, J. (1977) Behavior genetic analysis of Phormia regina: Conditioning, reliable individual differences and selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 74:5193–97. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J. (1974) The psychology of animal learning. Academic Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
McNair, J. N. (1979) A generalized model of optimal diets. Theoretical Population Biology 15:159–70. [TC, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNair, J. N. (1981) A stochastic foraging model with predator training effects. 2. Optimal diets. Theoretical Population Biology 19:147–62. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNair, J. N. (1983) A class of patch-use strategies. American Zoologist 23:303–13. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McSweeney, F. K. (1975) Concurrent responding as a function of body weight. Animal Learning and Behavior 3:264–70. [MD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. (1974) Models in ecology. Cambridge University Press. [JNM]Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. (1978) Optimization theory in evolution. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 9:3156. [SEGL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazur, J. E. (1981) Optimization theory fails to predict performance of pigeons in a two-response situation. Science 214:823–25. [SEGL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellgren, R. L. (1982) Foraging in a simulated natural environment: There's a rat loose in the lab. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 38:93100. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metz, H. A. J., Dienske, H., de Jonge, G. & Putters, F. A. (1983) Continuous-time Markov chains as models for animal behaviour. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology 45:643–58. [PB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, R. D. & Zeigler, H. P. (1979) Food preferences in the pigeon (Columba livia). Physiology and Behavior 22:1171–82. [SJS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, J. (1982) Choice and multiple reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 37:115–22. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morse, D. H. (1980) Behavioral mechanisms in ecology. Harvard University Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
Murton, R. K., Coombs, C. F. B. & Thearle, R. J. P. (1972) Ecological studies of the feral pigeon Columba livia var. 2. Flock behaviour and social organization. Journal of Applied Ecology 9:875–89. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Navarick, D. J. & Fantino, E. (1976) Self-control and general models of choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2:7587. [taEF]Google Scholar
Norborg, J., Osborne, S. & Fantino, E. (1983) Duration of components and response rates on multiple fixed-ratio schedules. Animal Learning and Behavior 11:5159. [rEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Normile, H. J. & Barraco, R. A. (1984) Relation between food and water intake in the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Psychology 98:7690. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oaten, A. (1977) Optimal foraging in patches: A ease for stochasticity. Theoretical Population Biology 12:263–85. [RFG, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okubo, A. (1980) Diffusion and ecological problems: Mathematical models. Springer Verlag. [PB]Google Scholar
Olton, D. S., Handelman, G. E. & Walker, J. A. (1981) Spatial memory and food searching strategies. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D.. Garland Press. [tarEF]Google Scholar
Parizkova, J. (1977) Body fat and physical fitness. Martinus Nijhoff B.V. [NR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peck, J. W. (1980) Homeostatic analyses and relations between nutrition and ecology. In: Analysis of motivational processes, ed. Toates, F. M. & Halliday, T. R.. Academic Press. [NR]Google Scholar
Peden, B. F. & Rohe, M. S. (1984) Effects of search cost on foraging and feeding: A three-component chain analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. In press. [rEF, BFP]Google Scholar
Picker, M. & Poling, A. (1982) Choice as a dependent measure in autoshaping: Sensitivity to frequency and duration of food presentation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 37:393406. [BLB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietrewicz, A. T. & Kamil, A. C. (1977) Visual detection of cryptic prey by blue jays (Cyanocitta cristate). Science 195:580–82. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pietrewicz, A. T. & Kamil, A. C. (1979) Search image formation in the blue jay (Cyanocitta cristata). Science 204:1332–33. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietrewicz, A. T. & Kamil, A. C. (1981) Search images and the detection of cryptic prey: An operant approach. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches. ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D.. Garland Press. [JNM, RLM]Google Scholar
Pliskoff, S. S., Cicerone, R. & Nelson, T. D. (1978) Local response-rate constancy on concurrent variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 29:431–46. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pliskoff, S. S. & Fetterman, J. G. (1981) Undermatching and overmatching: The fixed-ratio changeover requirement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 36:2127. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulliam, H. R. (1974) On the theory of optimal diets. American Naturalist 108:5974. [TC, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pyke, C. H. (1984) Optimal foraging theory: How useful is it? Paper presented at the Foraging Behavior Conference, Providence, R.I. [JH]Google Scholar
Pyke, G. H., Pulliam, H. R. & Charnov, E. L. (1977) Optimal foraging: A selective review of theory and tests. Quarterly Review of Biology 52:137–54. [taEF, RFG, JH, MBS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, W. G., Harris, W. A. & Benzer, S. (1974) Conditioned behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 71:708–12. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachlin, H., Battalio, R., Kagel, J. & Green, L. (1981) Maximization theory in behavioral psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:371417. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rappoport, D. J. (1981) Foraging behavior of Stentor coeruleus: A microeconomic interpretation. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological ethological, and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. B.. Garland Press. [taEF]Google Scholar
Rashotte, M. E., O'Connell, J. M. & Beidler, D. L. (1982) Associative influence on the foraging behavior of the pigeon (Columba livia). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 8:142–53. [taEF, MSa]Google Scholar
Regal, P. J. (1978) Behavioral differences between reptiles and mammals: An analysis of activity and mental capacities. In: Behavior and neurology of lizards, ed. Greenberg, N. & McLean, P.. U.S. Government Printing Office. [JMK]Google Scholar
Reichman, O. J. (1981) Factors influencing foraging in desert rodents. In: Foraging behavior: Ecological, ethological, and psychological approaches, ed. Kamil, A. C. & Sargent, T. D.. Garland Press. [JMK]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1968) Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 66:15. [rEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1970) Informational variables in Pavlovian conditioning. In: Current issues in animal learning, ed. Reynierse, J. H.. University of Nebraska Press. [rEF]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [BLB]Google Scholar
Rider, D. P. (1983) Preference for mixed versus constant delays of reinforcement: Effect of probability of the short, mixed delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 39:257–66. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodgers, W. & Rozin, P. (1966) Novel food preferences in thiamine-deficient rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 61:14. [ENR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowland, N. (1982) Failure by deprived hamsters to increase food intake: Some behavioral and physiological determinants. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 96:591603. [NR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. & Kalat, J. W. (1971) Specific hungers and poisoning as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review 78:459–86. [JMK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoener, T. W. (1971) Theory of feeding strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 2:369404. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1983) Memory in food-hoarding birds. Scientific American 247:102–10. [CLG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1984a) Learning and behavioural ecology. In: Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, 2d ed., ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B.. Blackwell. [CJB]Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1984b) Handling time and choice in pigeons. Submitted. [SJS]Google Scholar
Shull, R. L., Spear, D. J. & Bryson, A. E. (1981) Delay or rate of food delivery as a determiner of response rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 35:129–43. [SEGL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taEF]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950) Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review 57:193216. [MNB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolowski, M. B. (1980) Foraging strategies of Drosophila melanogaster: A chromosomal analysis. Behavior Cenetics 10:291302. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolowski, M. B., Hansell, R. I. C. & Rotin, D. (1983) Larval foraging behavior. 2. Selection in the sibling species Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans. Behavior Cenetics 13:169–77. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squires, N. & Fantino, E. (1971) A model for choice in simple concurrent and concurrent-chains schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 15:2738. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. (1964) Reinforcement as input: Cyclic variable-interval schedule. Science 145:410–12. [SEGL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staddon, J. E. R. (1979) Operant behavior as adaptation to constraint. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108:4687. [BFP]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. (1980) Optimality analyses of operant behavior and their relation to optimal foraging. In: Limits to action: The allocation of individual behavior, ed. Staddon, J. E. R.. Academic Press. [CJB, tarEF]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. (1983) Adaptive behavior and learning. Cambridge University Press. [tarEF]Google Scholar
Stephens, D. W. (1982) Stochasticity in foraging theory: Risk and information. D.Phil. thesis, Oxford University. [taEF]Google Scholar
Thompson, D. B. A. & Barnard, C. J. (1984) Prey selection by plovers: Optimal foraging in mixed species groups. Animal Behaviour 32:554–63. [CJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1980) A molar equilibrium theory of learned performance. In: The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 14, ed. Bower, G. H.. Academic Press. [BFP]Google Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1984) A temporal limit on the effect of future food on current performance in an analogue of foraging and welfare. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 41:117–24. [MNB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timberlake, W. & Allison, J. (1974) Response deprivation: An empirical approach to instrumental performance. Psychological Review 81:146–64. [BFP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, L. (1960) The natural control of insects in pinewoods. I. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds. Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie 13:265343. [RLM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. & Brunswick, E. (1935) The organism and causal texture of the environment. Psychological Review 42:4377. [PB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waage, J. K. (1979) Foraging for patchily-distributed hosts by the parasitoid, Nemeritis canescens. Journal of Animal Ecology 48:353–71. [JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waddington, K. D. (1982) Optimal diet theory: Sequential and simultaneous encounter models. Oikos 39:278–80. [rEF]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waddington, K. D. & Holden, R. (1979) Optimal foraging: On flower selection by bees. American Naturalist 114:179–96. [TC]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahlsten, D. (1972) Genetic experiments with animal learning: A critical view. Behavioural Biology 7:143–82. [MBS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahlsten, D. (1978) Behavioral genetics and animal learning. In: Psychopharmacology of aversively motivated behavior, ed. Anisman, H. & Bignami, G.. Plenum Press. [MBS]Google Scholar
Wasserman, E. A., Franklin, S. R. & Hearst, E. (1974) Pavlovian appetitive contingencies and approach versus withdrawal to conditioned stimuli in pigeons. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 86:616–27. [BLB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, G. E. & Hall, D. J. (1974) Optimal foraging and size selection of prey by the bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus). Ecology 55:1042–52. [taEF, JNM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, E. E., Mittelbach, G. G. & Hall, D. J. (1981) The role of foraging profitability and experience in habitat use by the bluegill sunfish. Ecology 62:116–25. [CLG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, J. S. (1981) Discrete-trial choice in pigeons: Effects of reinforcer magnitude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 35:2329. [taEF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, P. T. (1967) Palatability: The hedonic response to foodstuff. In: Handbook of physiology: Alimentary canal, ed. Code, C. F.. Waverly Press. [TWC]Google Scholar
Zeigler, H. P., Green, H. L. & Lehrer, R. (1971) Patterns of feeding behavior in the pigeon. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 76:468–77. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeigler, H. P., Green, H. L. & Siegel, J. (1972) Food and water intake and weight regulation in the pigeon. Physiology and Behavior 8:127–34. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zweers, G. A. (1982a) Drinking of the pigeon Columba livia L.). Behaviour 80:274317. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zweers, G. A. (1982b) Pecking of the pigeon (Columba livia L.). Behaviour 81:173230. [DMW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar