Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T18:56:38.889Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why imaginary worlds? The psychological foundations and cultural evolution of fictions with imaginary worlds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2021

Edgar Dubourg
Affiliation:
Département d’études Cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. [email protected]
Nicolas Baumard
Affiliation:
Département d’études Cognitives, Institut Jean Nicod, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris, France. [email protected]

Abstract

Imaginary worlds are extremely successful. The most popular fictions produced in the last few decades contain such a fictional world. They can be found in all fictional media, from novels (e.g., Lord of The Rings and Harry Potter) to films (e.g., Star Wars and Avatar), video games (e.g., The Legend of Zelda and Final Fantasy), graphic novels (e.g., One Piece and Naruto), and TV series (e.g., Star Trek and Game of Thrones), and they date as far back as ancient literature (e.g., the Cyclops Islands in The Odyssey, 850 BCE). Why such a success? Why so much attention devoted to non-existent worlds? In this paper, we propose that imaginary worlds co-opt our preferences for exploration, which have evolved in humans and nonhuman animals alike, to propel individuals toward new environments and new sources of reward. Humans would find imaginary worlds very attractive for the very same reasons, and under the same circumstances, as they are lured by unfamiliar environments in real life. After reviewing research on exploratory preferences in behavioral ecology, environmental esthetics, neuroscience, and evolutionary and developmental psychology, we focus on the sources of their variability across time and space, which we argue can account for the variability of the cultural preference for imaginary worlds. This hypothesis can, therefore, explain the way imaginary worlds evolved culturally, their shape and content, their recent striking success, and their distribution across time and populations.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akchelov, E., & Galanina, E. (2016). Virtual World of Video Games. 2016 8th International Conference on Games and Virtual Worlds for Serious Applications (VS-GAMES), 14. https://doi.org/10.1109/VS-GAMES.2016.7590379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alberti, J. (2013). “I Love You, Man”: Bromances, the construction of masculinity, and the continuing evolution of the romantic comedy. Quarterly Review of Film and Video, 30(2), 159172. https://doi.org/10.1080/10509208.2011.575658.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alves, I., Arenas, M., Currat, M., Sramkova Hanulova, A., Sousa, V. C., Ray, N., & Excoffier, L. (2016). Long-distance dispersal shaped patterns of human genetic diversity in Eurasia. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 33(4), 946958. https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msv332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, M. M., Schjoedt, U., Price, H., Rosas, F. E., Scrivner, C., & Clasen, M. (2020). Playing with fear: A field study in recreational horror. Psychological Science, 31(12), 1497–1510. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620972116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
André, J.-B., & Baumard, N. (2020). Cultural evolution by capital accumulation. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, e18. https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2020.19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
André, J.-B., Baumard, N., & Boyer, P. (2020). The Mystery of Symbolic Culture: What fitness costs? What fitness benefits? 18. https://osf.io/kdh7t/download?format=pdf.Google Scholar
Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(4), 547569. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X98001277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakhtin, M. M., & Emerson, C. (1984). Problems of Dostoevsky's poetics. University of Minnesota Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balling, J. D., & Falk, J. H. (1982). Development of visual preference for natural environments. Environment and Behavior, 14(1), 528. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916582141001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 248254. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartle, R. A. (2004). Designing virtual worlds. New Riders Pub.Google Scholar
Baumann, O., Dames, P., Kühnel, D., & Walz, B. (2002). Distribution of serotonergic and dopaminergic nerve fibers in the salivary gland complex of the cockroach Periplaneta americana. BMC Physiology, 15, 9.Google Scholar
Baumard, N. (2017). Phenotypic plasticity. What scientific term or concept ought to be more widely known? https://www.edge.org/response-detail/27196.Google Scholar
Baumard, N. (2019). Psychological origins of the industrial revolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, e189. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1800211X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumard, N., Huillery, E., & Zabro, L. (in press). The cultural evolution of love in history. Nature Human Behaviour.Google Scholar
Behrens, T. E. J., Muller, T. H., Whittington, J. C. R., Mark, S., Baram, A. B., Stachenfeld, K. L., & Kurth-Nelson, Z. (2018). What is a cognitive map? Organizing knowledge for flexible behavior. Neuron, 100(2), 490509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Goode, M. K., & Most, R. K. (1980). Maternal stimulation and infant exploratory competence: Cross-sectional, correlational, and experimental analyses. Child Development, 51(4), 1168. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benson-Amram, S., & Holekamp, K. E. (2012). Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279(1744), 40874095. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.1450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, A. I. (1977). Science-fiction fans in socio-economic perspective: Factors in the social consciousness of a genre. Science Fiction Studies, 4(3), 232246.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1950). Novelty and curiosity as determinants of exploratory behaviour. British Journal of Psychology. General Section, 41(1–2), 6880. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1950.tb00262.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception & Psychophysics, 8(5), 279286. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besson, A. (2015). Constellations: Des mondes fictionnels dans l'imaginaire contemporain. CNRS éditions.Google Scholar
Besson, A. (2021). Les pouvoirs de l'enchantement: Usages politiques de la fantasy et de la science-fiction. Vendémiaire.Google Scholar
Blanchard, T. C., & Gershman, S. J. (2018). Pure correlates of exploration and exploitation in the human brain. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(1), 117126. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-017-0556-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanco, N. J., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2019). Adaptive flexibility in category learning? Young children exhibit smaller costs of selective attention than adults. Developmental Psychology, 55(10), 20602076. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanco, N. J., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2020). Attentional mechanisms drive systematic exploration in young children. Cognition, 202, 104327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanco, N. J., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2021). Systematic exploration and uncertainty dominate young children's choices. Developmental Science, 24(2), e13026. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, A. B., & Wilson, L. A. (Eds.). (2010). George Lucas's blockbusting: A decade-by-decade survey of timeless movies, including untold secrets of their financial and cultural success (1st ed). itBooks.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2010). How pleasure works: The new science of why we like what we like (1st ed). W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Bogin, B. (1997). Evolutionary hypotheses for human childhood. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40, 27.Google Scholar
Bolt, J., & van Zanden, J. L. (2020). Maddison style estimates of the evolution of the world economy. A new 2020 update. https://www.rug.nl/ggdc/historicaldevelopment/maddison/releases/maddison-project-database-2020?lang=enGoogle Scholar
Bonawitz, E. B., van Schijndel, T. J. P., Friel, D., & Schulz, L. (2012). Children balance theories and evidence in exploration, explanation, and learning. Cognitive Psychology, 64(4), 215234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.12.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boon-Falleur, M., Baumard, N., & André, J.-B. (2020). Risk-seeking or impatient? Disentangling variance and time in hazardous behaviors. Evolution and Human Behavior, 42(5), 453–460. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/phtm8.Google Scholar
Borel, A., Ajzenherc, Y., Moncel, M.-H., Saint Jalme, M., & Krief, S. (2016). Do orangutans share early human interest in odd objects? Current Anthropology, 57(6), 828837. https://doi.org/10.1086/688855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (2010). Outline of a theory of practice (25. printing). Cambridge Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Bown, N. J., Read, D., & Summers, B. (2003). The lure of choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 16(4), 297308. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, B. (2018). The evolution of stories: From mimesis to language, from fact to fiction. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 9(1), e1444. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1444.Google ScholarPubMed
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2018). Minds make societies: How cognition explains the world humans create. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Brändle, F., Wu, C. M., & Schulz, E. (2020). What are we curious about? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 685–687. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.05.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broglio, C., Rodríguez, F., & Salas, C. (2003). Spatial cognition and its neural basis in teleost fishes. Fish and Fisheries, 4(3), 247–255. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1467-2979.2003.00128.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Matsumoto, M., & Hikosaka, O. (2010). Dopamine in motivational control: Rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron, 68(5), 815834. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchsbaum, D., Bridgers, S., Skolnick Weisberg, D., & Gopnik, A. (2012). The power of possibility: Causal learning, counterfactual reasoning, and pretend play. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 22022212. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bunzeck, N., & Düzel, E. (2006). Absolute coding of stimulus novelty in the human substantia nigra/VTA. Neuron, 51(3), 369379. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, D. M. (2015). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind (Fifth edn). Pearson.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, J. C. (2000). Personality and emotional correlates of right-wing authoritarianism. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 28(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2000.28.1.1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2013). Animal curiosity. Current Biology, 23(11), R469R470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canet, F. (2016). Quantitative approaches for evaluating the influence of films using the IMDb database. Communication & Society, 29(2), 151172. https://doi.org/10.15581/003.29.2.151-172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carbone, E., Meneghetti, C., & Borella, E. (2019). The influence of personality traits and facets on visuo-spatial task performance and self-assessed visuo-spatial inclinations in young and older adults. PLoS ONE, 14(8), e0220525. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0220525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carbone, E., Meneghetti, C., & Borella, E. (2020). The role of personality in route learning in young and older adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 166, 110187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. (2012). Literary Darwinism: Evolution, human nature, and literature. Routledge. https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/e/9780203505274.Google Scholar
Carter, G. G., Forss, S., Page, R. A., & Ratcliffe, J. M. (2018). Younger vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus) are more likely than adults to explore novel objects. PLoS ONE, 13(5), e0196889. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0196889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cashdan, E., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (2016). Why go there? Evolution of mobility and spatial cognition in women and men: An introduction to the special issue. Human Nature, 27(1), 115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-015-9253-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catania, A. C. (1980). Freedom of choice: A behavioral analysis. In Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 14, pp. 97145). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60160-7.Google Scholar
Catania, A. C., & Sagvolden, T. (1980). Preference for free choice over forced choice in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34(1), 7786. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1980.34-77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cave, T. (1999). Pré-histoires: Textes troublés au seuil de la modernité. Libr. Droz.Google Scholar
Chakroun, K., Mathar, D., Wiehler, A., Ganzer, F., & Peters, J. (2020). Dopaminergic modulation of the exploration/exploitation trade-off in human decision-making. eLife, 9, e51260. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.51260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambon, V., Thero, H., Findling, C., & Koechlin, E. (2018). Believing in one's power: A counterfactual heuristic for goal-directed control (Preprint). Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1101/498675.Google Scholar
Chapman, B. B., Hulthén, K., Wellenreuther, M., Hansson, L.-A., Nilsson, J.-Å., & Brönmark, C. (2014). Patterns of animal migration. In Hansson, L.-A. & Åkesson, S. (Eds.), Animal movement across scales (pp. 1135). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199677184.003.0002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charnov, E. L. (1976). Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9(2), 129136. https://doi.org/10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chau, A. (2018). From Nobel to Hugo: Reading Chinese science fiction as world literature. Modern Chinese Literatre and Culture, 30(1), 27.Google Scholar
Chavez, L., Ruiz, C., Curras, R., & Hernandez, B. (2020). The role of travel motivations and social media use in consumer interactive behaviour: A uses and gratifications perspective. Sustainability, 12(21), 8789. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12218789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claidière, N., Scott-Phillips, T. C., & Sperber, D. (2014). How Darwinian is cultural evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1642), 20130368. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2007). The role of attraction in cultural evolution. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 7(1–2), 89111. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853707X171829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2018). A nice surprise? Predictive processing and the active pursuit of novelty. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 17(3), 521534. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-017-9525-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clasen, M. (2010). The horror! The horror!. The Evolutionary Review, 1, 112119.Google Scholar
Clasen, M., Kjeldgaard-Christiansen, J., & Johnson, J. A. (2018). Horror, personality, and threat simulation: A survey on the psychology of scary media. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 14(3), 213–230. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000152.Google Scholar
Clasen, M., & Platts, T. K. (2019). Evolution and slasher films. In Vanderbeke, D. & Cooke, B. (Eds.), Evolution and popular narrative (pp. 23–42). Brill Rodopi. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004391161_003.Google Scholar
Clay, D. (2007). The islands of the odyssey. Journal of Medieval and Early Modern Studies, 37(1), 141161. https://doi.org/10.1215/10829636-2006-013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., & Yu, A. J. (2007). Should I stay or should I go? How the human brain manages the trade-off between exploitation and exploration. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 933942. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, V. D., Mitz, A. R., & Averbeck, B. B. (2019). Subcortical substrates of explore-exploit decisions in primates. Neuron, 103(3), 533545, e5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, V. D., Tran, V. L., Turchi, J., & Averbeck, B. B. (2014). Dopamine modulates novelty seeking behavior during decision making. Behavioral Neuroscience, 128(5), 556566. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, A., & Fisher, M. (2009). The Texas billionaire's pregnant bride: An evolutionary interpretation of romance fiction titles. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 3(4), 386401. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0099308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowston, K., & Fagnot, I. (2018). Stages of motivation for contributing user-generated content: A theory and empirical test. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 109, 89101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2017.08.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutting, J. E., Brunick, K. L., DeLong, J. E., Iricinschi, C., & Candan, A. (2011). Quicker, faster, darker: Changes in Hollywood film over 75 years. I-Perception, 2(6), 569576. https://doi.org/10.1068/i0441aap.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damerius, L. A., Graber, S. M., Willems, E. P., & van Schaik, C. P. (2017). Curiosity boosts orangutan problem-solving ability. Animal Behaviour, 134, 5770. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daugherty, T., Eastin, M. S., & Bright, L. (2008). Exploring consumer motivations for creating user-generated content. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 8(2), 1625. https://doi.org/10.1080/15252019.2008.10722139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daw, N. D., O'Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans. Nature, 441(7095), 876879. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Courson, B., & Baumard, N. (2019). Quantifying the scientific revolution (preprint). SocArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/9ex8q.Google Scholar
Defeyter, M. A., & German, T. P. (2003). Acquiring an understanding of design: Evidence from children's insight problem solving. Cognition, 89(2), 133155. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00098-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Giudice, M. (2014). Middle childhood: An evolutionary-developmental synthesis. Child Development Perspectives, 8(4), 193200. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delle, S. (2015). The power of imagination and the fan creation of muggle quidditch. In Brenner, L. S. (Ed.), Playing harry potter: Essays and interviews on fandom and performance (pp. 75–85). McFarland & Company, Inc., Publishers.Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G. (2013). The neuromodulator of exploration: A unifying theory of the role of dopamine in personality. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Openness/intellect: A dimension of personality reflecting cognitive exploration. In Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Cooper, M. L. & Larsen, R. J. (Eds.), APA Handbook of personality and social psychology, volume 4: Personality processes and individual differences. (pp. 369399). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14343-017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubey, R., & Griffiths, T. L. (2020). Reconciling novelty and complexity through a rational analysis of curiosity. Psychological Review, 127(3), 455476. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubourg, E., & Baumard, N. (in press). Why and how did narrative fictions evolve? Fictions as entertainment technology. Frontiers in Psychology.Google Scholar
Dubourg, E., Thouzeau, V., de Dampierre, C., & Baumard, N. (2021). Exploratory preferences explain the cultural success of imaginary worlds in modern societies. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/d9uqs.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (2017). Cognitive and network constraints in real life and literature. In Kenna, R., MacCarron, M. & MacCarron, P. (Eds.), Maths meets myths: Quantitative approaches to ancient narratives (pp. 719). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-39445-9_2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Düzel, E., Bunzeck, N., Guitart-Masip, M., & Düzel, S. (2010). Novelty-related motivation of anticipation and exploration by dopamine (NOMAD): Implications for healthy aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 660669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.08.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eco, U. (1997). The role of the reader: Explorations in the semiotics of texts (First Midland Book ed. 1984 [Nachdr.]). Indiana Univ. Pr.Google Scholar
Ekman, S. (2013). Here be dragons: Exploring fantasy maps and settings. Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
Eliassen, S., Jørgensen, C., Mangel, M., & Giske, J. (2007). Exploration or exploitation: Life expectancy changes the value of learning in foraging strategies. Oikos, 116(3), 513523. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2006.0030-1299.15462.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elza, C. (2014). Alice in Cartoonland: Childhood, gender, and imaginary space in early Disney animation. Animation, 9(1), 726. https://doi.org/10.1177/1746847714520936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Englerova, K., Klement, D., Frynta, D., Rokyta, R., & Nekovarova, T. (2019). Reactions to novel objects in monkeys: What does it mean to be neophobic? Primates, 60(4), 347353. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00731-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
English, S., Fawcett, T. W., Higginson, A. D., Trimmer, P. C., & Uller, T. (2016). Adaptive use of information during growth can explain long-term effects of early life experiences. The American Naturalist, 187(5), 620632. https://doi.org/10.1086/685644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, R. A., Patai, E. Z., Julian, J. B., & Spiers, H. J. (2017). The cognitive map in humans: Spatial navigation and beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 20(11), 10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Etchells, P. (2019). Lost in a good game: Why we play video games and what they can do for us. Icon Books Ltd.Google Scholar
Fagan, J. F. (1970). Memory in the infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 9(2), 217226. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(70)90087-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, J. H., & Balling, J. D. (2010). Evolutionary influence on human landscape preference. Environment and Behavior, 42(4), 479493. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916509341244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrara, E., Banerjee, A., & Orozco, V. (2019). MTV Shuga: Changing social norms and behaviors with entertainment education in Nigeria. Working Paper. https://www.povertyactionlab.org/publication/entertaining-way-behavioral-change-fighting-hiv-mtv.Google Scholar
Fishelov, D. (1995). The structure of generic categories: Some cognitive aspects. Journal of Literary Semantics, 24(2), 117–126. https://doi.org/10.1515/jlse.1995.24.2.117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FitzGibbon, L., Lau, J. K. L., & Murayama, K. (2020). The seductive lure of curiosity: Information as a motivationally salient reward. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 35, 2127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.05.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flinn, M. V., & Ward, C. V. (2005). Ontogeny and evolution of the social child. In Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D. F. (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 1944). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Frank, M. J., Doll, B. B., Oas-Terpstra, J., & Moreno, F. (2009). Prefrontal and striatal dopaminergic genes predict individual differences in exploration and exploitation. Nature Neuroscience, 12(8), 10621068. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Nettle, D. (2020). Integration of plasticity research across disciplines. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, 157162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.10.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E., Panchanathan, K., & Nettle, D. (2016). Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 7680. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.08.011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genette, G. (1997). Paratexts: Thresholds of interpretation. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When openness to experience and conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: An interactional approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(3), 513524. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershman, S. J. (2018). Deconstructing the human algorithms for exploration. Cognition, 173, 3442. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.12.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gessey-Jones, T., Connaughton, C., Dunbar, R., Kenna, R., MacCarron, P., O'Conchobhair, C., & Yose, J. (2020). Narrative structure of A Song of Ice and Fire creates a fictional world with realistic measures of social complexity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(46), 28582–28588. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2006465117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gocłowska, M. A., Ritter, S. M., Elliot, A. J., & Baas, M. (2019). Novelty seeking is linked to openness and extraversion, and can lead to greater creative performance. Journal of Personality, 87(2), 252266. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goody, J. (1986). The logic of writing and the organization of society (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511621598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. (2020). Childhood as a solution to explore–exploit tensions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1803), 20190502. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A., Griffiths, T. L., & Lucas, C. G. (2015). When younger learners can be better (or at least more open-minded) than older ones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(2), 8792. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414556653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., O'Grady, S., Lucas, C. G., Griffiths, T. L., Wente, A., Bridgers, S., … Dahl, R. E. (2017). Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 78927899. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1700811114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, G., Breazeal, C., & Engel, S. (2015). Can children catch curiosity from a social robot? Proceedings of the Tenth Annual ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction – HRI, 15, 9198. https://doi.org/10.1145/2696454.2696469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, J., & Oudeyer, P.-Y. (2018). Towards a neuroscience of active sampling and curiosity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(12), 758770. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-018-0078-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P.-Y., Lopes, M., & Baranes, A. (2013). Information-seeking, curiosity, and attention: Computational and neural mechanisms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 585593. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottschall, J. (2012). The storytelling animal: How stories make us human. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Gottschall, J. & Wilson, D. S. (Eds.) (2005). The literary animal: Evolution and the nature of narrative. Northwestern University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, S. J. (2008). A biological homage to mickey mouse. Ecotone, 4(1–2), 333340. https://doi.org/10.1353/ect.2008.0045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gozli, D. G. (2018). Reaching into the unknown: Actions, goal hierarchies, and explorative agency. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodal, T. (2010). High on crime fiction and detection. Projections, 4(2), 64–85. https://doi.org/10.3167/proj.2010.040205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grodal, T. (2017). How film genres are a product of biology, evolution and culture – An embodied approach. Palgrave Communications, 3(1), 17079. https://doi.org/10.1057/palcomms.2017.79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guarino, S., Yeater, D., Lacy, S., Dees, T., & Hill, H. M. (2017). Responses to familiar and unfamiliar objects by belugas (Delphinapterus leucas), bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), and Pacific white-sided dolphins (Lagenorhynchus obliquidens). Animal Cognition, 20(5), 823827. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1103-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunderson, V. M., & Sackett, G. P. (1984). Development of pattern recognition in infant pigtailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina). Developmental Psychology, 20(3), 418426. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.3.418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haggard, P., & Chambon, V. (2012). Sense of agency. Current Biology, 22(10), R390R392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hakimi, S., Hejazi, E., & Lavasani, M. G. (2011). The relationships between personality traits and students’ academic achievement. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 29, 836845. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.11.312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayden, B. Y., & Niv, Y. (2021). The case against economic values in the orbitofrontal cortex (or anywhere else in the brain).Behavioral Neuroscience, 135(2), 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000448.Google Scholar
Healy, S., Dekort, S., & Clayton, N. (2005). The hippocampus, spatial memory and food hoarding: A puzzle revisited. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 20(1), 1722. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2004.10.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, T. R. (1984). A cognitive analysis of preference for field-and-forest environments. Landscape Research, 9(1), 1016. https://doi.org/10.1080/01426398408706092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzog, T. R. (1985). A cognitive analysis of preference for waterscapes. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 5(3), 225241. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-4944(85)80024-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzog, T. R., & Bryce, A. G. (2007). Mystery and preference in within-forest settings. Environment and Behavior, 39(6), 779796. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916506298796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzog, T., & Smith, G. A. (1988). Danger, mystery, and environmental preference. Environment and Behavior, 20(3), 320344. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916588203004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B., van de Koppel, J. M. H., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1982). Exploration ranges of Aka pygmies of the Central African Republic. Man, 17(3), 418. https://doi.org/10.2307/2801706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hills, T. (2004). Dopamine and glutamate control area-restricted search behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(5), 12171225. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1569-03.2004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, T. T. (2006). Animal foraging and the evolution of goal-directed cognition. Cognitive Science, 30(1), 341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, T. T., & Stroup, W. (2004). Cognitive exploration and search behavior in the development of endogenous representations. San Diego: American Educational Research Association.Google Scholar
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., & Goldstone, R. L. (2010). The central executive as a search process: Priming exploration and exploitation across domains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 590609. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., Lazer, D., Redish, A. D., & Couzin, I. D. (2015). Exploration versus exploitation in space, mind, and society. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(1), 4654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A., & Barden, L. A. (1985). The evolution of the teddy bear. Animal Behaviour, 33(4), 13711373. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80205-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horvitz, J. C., Stewart, T., & Jacobs, B. L. (1997). Burst activity of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons is elicited by sensory stimuli in the awake cat. Brain Research, 759(2), 251258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00265-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, R. N. (2007). Neotic preferences in laboratory rodents: Issues, assessment and substrates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 31(3), 441464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.11.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K. L., Lee, S. S., Telzer, E. H., Gabard-Durnam, L. J., Goff, B., Flannery, J., & Tottenham, N. (2015). Exploration–exploitation strategy is dependent on early experience: Exploration–exploitation. Developmental Psychobiology, 57(3), 313321. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ikemi, M. (2005). The effects of mystery on preference for residential façades. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(2), 167173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.04.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R. (2018). Cultural evolution: People's motivations are changing, and reshaping the world (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108613880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, L. F. (2003). The evolution of the cognitive map. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 62(2), 128139. https://doi.org/10.1159/000072443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacquet, P. O., Safra, L., Wyart, V., Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2019). The ecological roots of human susceptibility to social influence: A pre-registered study investigating the impact of early-life adversity. Royal Society Open Science, 6(1), 180454. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.180454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkins, H. (Ed.) (1998). The children's culture reader. New York University Press.Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. (2006). Convergence culture: Where old and new media collide. New York University Press.Google Scholar
Jiang, Q. (2013). Translation and the development of science fiction in twentieth-century China. Science-Fiction Studies, 40(1), 116–132. https://doi.org/10.5621/sciefictstud.40.1.0116.Google Scholar
Jobling, I. (2001). The psychological foundations of the hero-ogre story: A cross-cultural study. Human Nature, 12(3), 247272. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-001-1009-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jourde, P. (1991). Géographies imaginaires de quelques inventeurs de mondes au XXe siècle: Gracq, borges, michaux, Tolkien. J. Corti.Google Scholar
Jullier, L. (2018). Cinéma et psychologie cognitive: Le “tournant corporel” des études audiovisuelles. Ekphrasis. Images, Cinema, Theory, Media, 20(2), 2034. https://doi.org/10.24193/ekphrasis.20.2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kakade, S., & Dayan, P. (2002). Dopamine: Generalization and bonuses. Neural Networks, 15(4–6), 549559. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-6080(02)00048-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, H., & Gangestad, S. W. (2015). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (2nd ed., pp. 88–114). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125563.evpsych102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9(4), 156–185.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, H., & Robson, A. J. (2002). The emergence of humans: The coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational transfers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(15), 1022110226. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.152502899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaplan, S. (1987). Aesthetics, affect, and cognition: Environmental preference from an evolutionary perspective. Environment and Behavior, 19(1), 332. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916587191001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, S. (1988). Perception and landscape: Conceptions and misconceptions. In Nasar, J. L. (Ed.), Environmental aesthetics (1st ed., pp. 4555). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571213.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, K., & Naug, D. (2015). Energetic state regulates the exploration–exploitation trade-off in honeybees. Behavioral Ecology, 26(4), 10451050. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, C., & Hayden, B. Y. (2015). The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron, 88(3), 449460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2012). The goldilocks effect: Human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS ONE, 7(5), e36399. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiverstein, J., Miller, M., & Rietveld, E. (2019). The feeling of grip: Novelty, error dynamics, and the predictive brain. Synthese, 196(7), 28472869. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-017-1583-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knutson, B., & Cooper, J. C. (2006). The lure of the unknown. Neuron, 51(3), 280282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konzack, L. (2018). Transmediality. In Wolf, M. J. P. (Ed.), The Routledge companion to imaginary worlds (pp. 134–140). Routledge.Google Scholar
Korobkova, E., Emonet, T., Vilar, J. M. G., Shimizu, T. S., & Cluzel, P. (2004). From molecular noise to behavioural variability in a single bacterium. Nature, 428(6982), 574578. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koster, R., Seow, T. X., Dolan, R. J., & Düzel, E. (2016). Stimulus novelty energizes actions in the absence of explicit reward. PLoS ONE, 11(7), e0159120. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krebs, R. M., Schott, B. H., Schütze, H., & Düzel, E. (2009). The novelty exploration bonus and its attentional modulation. Neuropsychologia, 47(11), 22722281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krueger, P. M., Wilson, R. C., & Cohen, J. D. (2017). Strategies for exploration in the domain of losses. Judgment and Decision Making, 14, 104–117.Google Scholar
Kuipers, G., & de Kloet, J. (2009). Banal cosmopolitanism and The Lord of the Rings: The limited role of national differences in global media consumption. Poetics, 37(2), 99118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2009.01.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavocat, F. (2016). Fait et fiction: Pour une frontière. Éditions du Seuil.Google Scholar
Le Heron, C., Kolling, N., Plant, O., Kienast, A., Janska, R., Ang, Y.-S., … Apps, M. A. J. (2020). Dopamine modulates dynamic decision-making during foraging. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(27), 5273–5282. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2586-19.2020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, E. S. (1966). A theory of migration. Demography, 3(1), 47–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lévi, J. (1995). La chine romanesque: Fictions d'Orient et d'Occident. Seuil.Google Scholar
Li, W., Li, X., Huang, L., Kong, X., Yang, W., Wei, D., … Liu, J. (2015). Brain structure links trait creativity to openness to experience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(2), 191–198. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liquin, E. G., & Gopnik, A. (2019). Learning from approach-avoid decisions: Children explore and learn more than adults. Paper presented at the Baltimore, MD: Society for Research in Child Development.Google Scholar
Liquin, E. G., & Lombrozo, T. (2020a). A functional approach to explanation-seeking curiosity. Cognitive Psychology, 119, 101276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2020.101276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liquin, E. G., & Lombrozo, T. (2020b). Explanation-seeking curiosity in childhood. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 35, 1420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.05.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Louwerse, M. M., & Benesh, N. (2012). Representing spatial structure through maps and language: Lord of the Rings encodes the spatial structure of middle earth. Cognitive Science, 36(8), 15561569. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Louwerse, M. M., & Zwaan, R. A. (2009). Language encodes geographical information. Cognitive Science, 33(1), 5173. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2008.01003.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, H. (1994). From historicity to fictionality: The Chinese poetics of narrative. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Lu, X. (2000). A brief history of Chinese fiction. University press of the Pacific.Google Scholar
Lucas, C. G., Bridgers, S., Griffiths, T. L., & Gopnik, A. (2014). When children are better (or at least more open-minded) learners than adults: Developmental differences in learning the forms of causal relationships. Cognition, 131(2), 284299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.010.Google ScholarPubMed
Lucian of Samosata. (150 C.E.). True history.Google Scholar
MacDonald, D. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (1999). Reproductive interests and forager mobility. Current Anthropology, 40(4), 501524. https://doi.org/10.1086/200047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A. (2018). Evaluating whether stories can promote social cognition: Introducing the social processes and content entrained by narrative (SPaCEN) framework. Discourse Processes, 55(5–6), 454479. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2018.1448209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(3), 173192. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00073.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, M. d. J. D., & Baumard, N. (2020). The rise of prosociality in fiction preceded democratic revolutions in early modern Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(46), 28684–28691. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2009571117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martins, M. d. J. D., & Baumard, N. (2021). Loving, fast and slow: A quantitative history of passion and tenderness in early modern Europe. Working Paper.Google Scholar
Maspons, J., Molowny-Horas, R., & Sol, D. (2019). Behaviour, life history and persistence in novel environments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 374(1781), 20180056. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2018.0056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCrae, R. R. (1993). Openness to experience as a basic dimension of personality. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 13(1), 3955. https://doi.org/10.2190/H8H6-QYKR-KEU8-GAQ0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDevitt, M. A., Pisklak, J. M., Spetch, M., & Dunn, R. (2018). The influence of outcome delay on suboptimal choice. Behavioural Processes, 157, 279285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.10.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehlhorn, K., Newell, B. R., Todd, P. M., Lee, M. D., Morgan, K., Braithwaite, V. A., … Gonzalez, C. (2015). Unpacking the exploration–exploitation tradeoff: A synthesis of human and animal literatures. Decision, 2(3), 191215. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mell, H., Baumard, N., & André, J.-B. (2021). Time is money. Waiting costs explain why selection favors steeper time discounting in deprived environments. Evolution and Human Behavior, 42(4), 379–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellmann, K. (2012). Is storytelling a biological adaptation? Preliminary thoughts on how to pose that question. In Gansel, C. & Vanderbeke, D. (Eds.), Telling stories/geschichten erzählen (pp. 30–49). De Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110268683.30.Google Scholar
Menadue, C. B., & Jacups, S. (2018). Who reads science fiction and fantasy, and how do they feel about science? Preliminary findings from an online survey. SAGE Open, 8(2), 215824401878094. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244018780946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meneghetti, C., Grimaldi, F., Nucci, M., & Pazzaglia, F. (2020). Positive and negative wayfinding inclinations, choice of navigation aids, and How they relate to personality traits. Journal of Individual Differences, 41(1), 4552. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercier, H. (2016). The argumentative theory: Predictions and empirical evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(9), 689700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mettke-Hofmann, C., Winkler, H., & Leisler, B. (2002). The significance of ecological factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology, 108(3), 249272. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2002.00773.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. (2001). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Miner, E. J., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Gaulin, S. J. C. (2014). Sex difference in travel is concentrated in adolescence and tracks reproductive interests. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1796), 20141476. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1476.Google ScholarPubMed
Montello, D. R. (2005). Navigation. In Shah, P. & Miyake, A. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking (1st ed., pp. 257294). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511610448.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, T. J. H., Suchow, J. W., & Griffiths, T. L. (2020). Experimental evolutionary simulations of learning, memory and life history. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1803), 20190504. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morin, O. (2016). Reasons to be fussy about cultural evolution. Biology & Philosophy, 31(3), 447458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-016-9516-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morin, O., Acerbi, A., & Sobchuk, O. (2019). Why people die in novels: Testing the ordeal simulation hypothesis. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 62. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0267-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D., & Hartas, L. (2004). The art of game worlds. Ilex.Google Scholar
Muffato, V., Meneghetti, C., & De Beni, R. (2016). Not all is lost in older adults’ route learning: The role of visuo-spatial abilities and type of task. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 47, 230241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.07.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muffato, V., Toffalini, E., Meneghetti, C., Carbone, E., & De Beni, R. (2017). Individual visuo-spatial factors and familiar environment knowledge: A structural equation modeling analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 113, 96102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakawake, Y., & Sato, K. (2019). Systematic quantitative analyses reveal the folk-zoological knowledge embedded in folktales. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 161. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-019-0375-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nave, G., Rentfrow, J., & Bhatia, S. (2020). We are what we watch: Movie plots predict the personalities of those who “like” them (preprint). PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/wsdu8.Google Scholar
Neidhardt, F. C., & Curtiss, R. (Eds.). (1996). Escherichia coli and Salmonella: Cellular and molecular biology (2nd ed). ASM Press.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2005a). The wheel of fire and the mating game: Explaining the origins of tragedy and comedy. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 3(1), 3956. https://doi.org/10.1556/JCEP.3.2005.1.3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D. (2005b). Exploring the psychological foundations of drama. In Gottschall, J. & Wilson, D. S. (Eds.), The literary animal: Evolution and the nature of narrative (pp. 56–75). Northwestern University Press. https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvw1d5h1.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2019). State-dependent cognition and its relevance to cultural evolution. Behavioural Processes, 161, 101107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.01.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, C. T. (2019). Games and the art of agency. The Philosophical Review, 128(4), 423462. https://doi.org/10.1215/00318108-7697863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nitsche, M. (2008). Video game spaces: Image, play, and structure in 3D game worlds. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, P., & Inglehart, R. (2004). Sacred and secular: Religion and politics worldwide (1st ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511791017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omar, B., & Dequan, W. (2020). Watch, share or create: The influence of personality traits and user motivation on TikTok Mobile video usage. International Journal of Interactive Mobile Technologies (IJIM), 14(04), 121. https://doi.org/10.3991/ijim.v14i04.12429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Organization for Economic Co-Operation and Development (2017). Indicator A1 To what level have adults studied? Education at a glance 2017. OECD Indicators. https://doi.org/10.1787/eag-2017-7-enGoogle Scholar
Oudeyer, P.-Y., Gottlieb, J., & Lopes, M. (2016). Intrinsic motivation, curiosity, and learning. In Progress in brain research (Vol. 229, pp. 257284). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2016.05.005.Google Scholar
Paige, N. (2018). The artifactuality of narrative form. Poetics Today, 39(1), 4165. https://doi.org/10.1215/03335372-4265059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paige, N. D. (2011). Before fiction: The ancien régime of the novel (1st ed). University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Paige, N. D. (2020). Technologies of the novel. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pavel, T. G. (2017). Univers de la fiction: Postface inédite. Points Essais.Google Scholar
Pazzaglia, F., Meneghetti, C., & Ronconi, L. (2018). Tracing a route and finding a shortcut: The working memory, motivational, and personality factors involved. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 225. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peeler, D. F., & Nowakowski, R. S. (1987). Genetic factors and the measurement of exploratory activity. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 48(1), 90103. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-1047(87)90619-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piantadosi, S. T., & Kidd, C. (2016). Extraordinary intelligence and the care of infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(25), 68746879. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1506752113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piccardi, L., Leonzi, M., D'Amico, S., Marano, A., & Guariglia, C. (2014). Development of navigational working memory: Evidence from 6- to 10-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 205217. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works (norton pbk). Norton.Google Scholar
Platt, M. L., & Brannon, E. M. (1996). Differences in feeding ecology predict differences in performance between golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia) and Wied's mannosets (Callithrix kuhli) on spatial and visual memory tasks. Animal Learning & Behavior, 24(4), 384–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plebanek, D. J., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2017). Costs of selective attention: When children notice what adults miss. Psychological Science, 28(6), 723732. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617693005.Google ScholarPubMed
Poch, R., & Martin, B. (2015). Effects of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation on user-generated content. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 23(4), 305317. https://doi.org/10.1080/0965254X.2014.926966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polizzi di Sorrentino, E., Sabbatini, G., Truppa, V., Bordonali, A., Taffoni, F., Formica, D., Baldassarre, G., … Visalberghi, E. (2014). Exploration and learning in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.): The role of action–outcome contingencies. Animal Cognition, 17(5), 10811088. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0740-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poor, N. (2014). Computer game modders’ motivations and sense of community: A mixed-methods approach. New Media & Society, 16(8), 12491267. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444813504266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Postel, P. (2019). Comment la fiction vint aux chinois (Colloque “Fiction et culture”).Google Scholar
Proctor, W., & McCulloch, R. (2016). Exploring imaginary worlds: Audiences, fan cultures and geographies of the imagination. 13(1), 9.Google Scholar
Purhonen, S., Gronow, J., & Rahkonen, K. (2009). Social differentiation of musical and literary taste patterns in Finland. Research on Finnish Society, 2, 3949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebora, S. (2016). On fantasy's transmediality: A cognitive approach. Comparatismi, 1, 217–232. https://doi.org/10.14672/2016872.Google Scholar
Reed, P., & Adams, L. (1996). Influence of salient stimuli on rats’ performance in an eight-arm radial maze. Learning and Motivation, 27(3), 294306. https://doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1996.0016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, P., Mitchell, C., & Nokes, T. (1996). Intrinsic reinforcing properties of putatively neutral stimuli in an instrumental two-lever discrimination task. Animal Learning & Behavior, 24(1), 3845. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198952.Google Scholar
Rich, A. S., & Gureckis, T. M. (2018). The limits of learning: Exploration, generalization, and the development of learning traps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(11), 15531570. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rojas-Ferrer, I., Thompson, M. J., & Morand-Ferron, J. (2020). Is exploration a metric for information gathering? Attraction to novelty and plasticity in black-capped chickadees. Ethology, 126(4), 383392. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosati, A. G., & Hare, B. (2012). Chimpanzees and bonobos exhibit divergent spatial memory development: Spatial memory development in chimpanzees and bonobos. Developmental Science, 15(6), 840853. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01182.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosati, A. G., Rodriguez, K., & Hare, B. (2014). The ecology of spatial memory in four lemur species. Animal Cognition, 17(4), 947961. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0727-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubenstein, J. (1967). Maternal attentiveness and subsequent exploratory behavior in the infant. Child Development, 38(4), 1089. https://doi.org/10.2307/1127106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruso, B., Renninger, L., & Atzwanger, K. (2003). Human habitat preferences: A generative territory for evolutionary aesthetics research. In Voland, E. & Grammer, K. (Eds.), Evolutionary Aesthetics (pp. 279294). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-07142-7_10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, M.-L. (1991). Possible worlds, artificial intelligence, and narrative theory. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, M.-L. (2014). Space. The living handbook of narratology. https://www.lhn.uni-hamburg.de/node/55.html.Google Scholar
Saad, G. (2012). Nothing in popular culture makes sense except in the light of evolution. Review of General Psychology, 16(2), 109120. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saad, G., & Gill, T. (2000). Applications of evolutionary psychology in marketing. Psychology & Marketing, 17(12), 1005–1034.3.0.CO;2-H>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Safra, L., Chevallier, C., Grèzes, J., & Baumard, N. (2020). Tracking historical changes in trustworthiness using machine learning analyses of facial cues in paintings. Nature Communications, 11(1), 4728. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-18566-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saler, M. T. (2012). As if: Modern enchantment and the literary pre-history of virtual reality. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Salmon, C., & Symons, D. (2004). Slash fiction and human mating psychology. Journal of Sex Research, 41(1), 94100. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490409552217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaeffer, J.-M. (1999). Pourquoi la fiction? Seuil.Google Scholar
Schniter, E., Wilcox, N. T., Beheim, B. A., Kaplan, H. S., & Gurven, M. (2018). Information transmission and the oral tradition: Evidence of a late-life service niche for tsimane amerindians. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 94105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.10.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholes, R., & Rabkin, E. S. (1977). Science fiction: History, science, vision. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(1), 127. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W. (2015). Neuronal reward and decision signals: From theories to data. Physiological Reviews, 95(3), 853951. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00023.2014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, E., & Gershman, S. J. (2019). The algorithmic architecture of exploration in the human brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 55, 714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.11.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, E., Wu, C. M., Ruggeri, A., & Meder, B. (2019). Searching for rewards like a child means less generalization and more directed exploration. Psychological Science, 30(11), 1561–1572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T., Blancke, S., & Heintz, C. (2018). Four misunderstandings about cultural attraction. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues. News, and Reviews, 27(4), 162173. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21716.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1975). The logical Status of fictional discourse. New Literary History, 6(2), 319. https://doi.org/10.2307/468422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serafini, L. (1981). Codex Seraphinianus. Rizzoli.Google Scholar
Sibley, C. G., & Duckitt, J. (2008). Personality and prejudice: A meta-analysis and theoretical review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(3), 248279. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308319226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvey, R., & MacKeith, S. (1988). The paracosm: A special form of fantasy. Organizing early experience: Imagination and cognition in childhood (pp. 173–179). Routledge.Google Scholar
Sim, Z. L., & Xu, F. (2017a). Learning higher-order generalizations through free play: Evidence from 2- and 3-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 642651. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sim, Z. L., & Xu, F. (2017b). Infants preferentially approach and explore the unexpected. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 596608. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2020). Subjective selection and the evolution of complex culture. 21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2021). The sympathetic plot, its psychological origins, and implications for the evolution of fiction. Emotion Review, 13(3), 183198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snell-Rood, E., & Snell-Rood, C. (2020). The developmental support hypothesis: Adaptive plasticity in neural development in response to cues of social support. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1803), 20190491. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sol, D., Sayol, F., Ducatez, S., & Lefebvre, L. (2016). The life-history basis of behavioural innovations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 20150187. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0187.Google ScholarPubMed
Song, H. (2013). Chinese Science fiction: A response to modernization. Science Fiction Studies, 40. https://doi.org/10.5621/SCIEFICTSTUD.40.1.0015.Google Scholar
Sorić, I., Penezić, Z., & Burić, I. (2017). The Big five personality traits, goal orientations, and academic achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 54, 126134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sotamaa, O. (2010). When the game is not enough: Motivations and practices among computer game modding culture. Games and Culture, 5(3), 239255. https://doi.org/10.1177/1555412009359765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 4046. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spivey, J., Barrett, D., Padilla, E., & Gonzalez-Lima, F. (2008). Mother–infant separation leads to hypoactive behavior in adolescent Holtzman rats. Behavioural Processes, 79(1), 5965. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2008.05.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stansfield, K. H., & Kirstein, C. L. (2006). Effects of novelty on behavior in the adolescent and adult rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(1), 1015. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, D. W., Brown, J. S., & Ydenberg, R. C. (2014). Foraging Behavior and Ecology.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. R., Rosati, A. G., Ross, K. R., & Hauser, M. D. (2005). Will travel for food: Spatial discounting in two new world monkeys. Current Biology, 15(20), 18551860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.09.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugiyama, M. (2021). Co-occurrence of ostensive communication and generalizable knowledge in forager storytelling: Cross-cultural evidence of teaching in forager societies. Human Nature, 32(1), 279–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, S. (1996). On the origins of narrative: Storyteller bias as a fitness-enhancing strategy. Human Nature, 7(4), 403425. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02732901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugiyama, S. (2001). Food, foragers, and folklore: The role of narrative in human subsistence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(4), 221240. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00063-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sumner, E., Li, A. X., Perfors, A., Hayes, B. K., Navarro, D. J., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2019a). The Exploration Advantage: 11.Google Scholar
Sumner, E., Steyvers, M., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2019b). It's not the treasure, it's the hunt: Children are more explorative on an explore/exploit task than adults. In A. K. Goel, C. M. Seifert, & C. Freksa (Eds.), Proceedings of the 41st annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2891–2897). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Sun, Y., Dong, X., & McIntyre, S. (2017). Motivation of user-generated content: Social connectedness moderates the effects of monetary rewards. Marketing Science, 36(3), 10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton, R. S. (1990). Integrated architectures for learning, planning, and reacting based on approximating dynamic programming. In Machine learning proceedings 1990 (pp. 216224). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-1-55860-141-3.50030-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, E. S. (2018). A psychology of the film. Palgrave Communications, 4(1), 82. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-018-0111-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanenbaum, K., & Tanenbaum, J. (2009). Commitment to Meaning: A Reframing of Agency in Games. 9.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Mottweiler, C. M., Aguiar, N. R., Naylor, E. R., & Levernier, J. G. (2020). Paracosms: The imaginary worlds of middle childhood. Child Development, 91(1), 164–178. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todorov, T. (2015). Introduction à la littérature fantastique. Éditions du Seuil.Google Scholar
Tolkien, J. R. R., & Tolkien, C. (2006). The monsters and the critics and other essays. HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tottenham, N., Shapiro, M., Flannery, J., Caldera, C., & Sullivan, R. M. (2019). Parental presence switches avoidance to attraction learning in children. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(10), 10701077. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0656-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, R. S. (1979). Visual landscapes and psychological well-being. Landscape Research, 4(1), 1723. https://doi.org/10.1080/01426397908705892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Burkart, J., Damerius, L., Forss, S. I. F., Koops, K., van Noordwijk, M. A., & Schuppli, C. (2016). The reluctant innovator: Orangutans and the phylogeny of creativity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 20150183. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanderbeke, D. (2019). On love and marriage in popular genres. In Vanderbeke, D. & Cooke, B. (Eds.), Evolution and popular narrative (pp. 83–105). Brill Rodopi. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004391161_006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veblen, T. (1899). Theory of the leisure class (New ed).Google Scholar
Verdolin, J. L. (2006). Meta-analysis of foraging and predation risk trade-offs in terrestrial systems. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 60(4), 457464. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0172-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verpooten, J., & Nelissen, M. (2010). Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: The late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution. Theory in Biosciences, 129(2–3), 211221. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12064-010-0095-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veyne, P. (1988). Did the Greeks believe in their myths? An essay on the constitutive imagination. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wade, S., & Kidd, C. (2019). The role of prior knowledge and curiosity in learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(4), 13771387. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01598-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walton, K. L. (1993). Mimesis as make-believe: On the foundations of the representational arts. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wang, M. Z., & Hayden, B. Y. (2019). Monkeys are curious about counterfactual outcomes. Cognition, 189, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.03.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisberg, S. M., Schinazi, V. R., Newcombe, N. S., Shipley, T. F., & Epstein, R. A. (2014). Variations in cognitive maps: Understanding individual differences in navigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 669682. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035261.Google ScholarPubMed
Wikipedia. (2021). List of highest-grossing media franchises. In Wikipedia. https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=List_of_highest-grossing_media_franchises&oldid=1028024361.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. C., Geana, A., White, J. M., Ludvig, E. A., & Cohen, J. D. (2014). Humans use directed and random exploration to solve the explore–exploit dilemma. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(6), 20742081. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M. J. P. (2012). Building imaginary worlds: The theory and history of subcreation. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. J. P. (2017a). Revisiting imaginary worlds: A subcreation studies anthology. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. J. P. (2017b). World design. The Routledge companion to imaginary worlds. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M. J. P. (Ed.) (2021). Exploring imaginary worlds: Essays on media, structure, and subcreation. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. J. P., & Perron, B. (Eds.) (2014). The Routledge companion to video game studies. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35(2), 151175. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zunshine, L. (2006). Why we read fiction: Theory of mind and the novel. Ohio State University Press.Google Scholar