We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
REFERENCES
Beck, B. (1973) Cooperative tool use by captive hamadryas baboons. Science182:596–97. [SS-R]Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975) The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language. 2:1–19. [SS-R]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullowu, M. (1979) Introduction: Prelinguistic Communication: A field for scientific research. In: Before Speech; The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication, ed., Bullowa, M., pp. 1–62. London: Cambridge University Press. [SS-RJ]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959) A review of B.F. Skinner's “Verbal Behavior,”Language35:26–58. [BS]Google Scholar
Clark, R. A. (1978) The transition from Action to Gesture. In: Action, gesture, and symbol: the emergence of language, ed. Lock, A., pp. 231–50. London: Academic Press. [SS-R]Google Scholar
Delius, J. D. & Habers, C. (1978) Symmetry: can pigeons conceptualize it?Behavioral Biology22:336–42. [ML]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabricius, E. & Jansson, A. M. (1963) Laboratory observation on the reproductive behavior of the pigeon. Animal Behavior11:534–47. [SS-R]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisch, K. Von (1967) The dance language and orientation of bees, Chadwick, L., trans. Cambridge Mass.: Harvard University Press. [DRG]Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G.Jr (1975) Towards an operational definition of self-awareness. In: Socio-ecology and psychology of primates, ed., Tuttle, R.. The Hague: Mouton. [BS]Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G.Jr (1977) Self-recognition in primates. A comparative approach to the bidirectional properties of consciousness. American Psychologist32:329–38. [BS]Google Scholar
Griffin, D. R. (1978) Prospects for a cognitive ethology. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences1:527–38. [ML, BS, RS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1979) One child's protolanguage. In: Before speech; the beginning of interpersonal communication, ed., Bullowa, M., pp. 171–90. London: Cambridge University Press. [SS-R]Google Scholar
Katz, D. (1939) Animals and men: studies in comparative psychology. London: Longmans, Green. [SS-R]Google Scholar
Mackay, O. M. (1972) Formal analysis of communicative processes. In: Nonverbal Communication, ed., Hinde, R. A., pp. 3–25. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [SS-R]Google Scholar
Mazochin-Porshnyakov, Georg (1969) Die Fähigkeit der Bienen, visuelle Reize zu generalisieren. Z. vergl. Physiol. 65:15–28. [ML]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?The Behavioral and Brain Sciences1:515–26. [ML, BS, RS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978) Author's response to Parts I and II of Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?Behavioral and Brain Sciences1:616–629. [DRG]Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., & Boysen, S. (1978) Linguistically mediated tool use and exchange by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) The Behavioral and Brain Sciences1:539–54.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms: an experimental analysis. New York: D. Appleton-Century Co. [SS-R]Google Scholar
Whitehead, A. N. (1938) Modes of thought. New York: Macmillan. [DRG]Google Scholar
Wurtz, R. H. (1978) Visual-Motor Interaction in Monkey Superior Colliculus. Paper presented at Sixth International Congress of Biophysics, Kyoto. [ML]Google Scholar