Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T22:27:42.841Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Searching in the wrong place: Might consciousness reside in the brainstem?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2022

Marshall Devor
Affiliation:
Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, Silberman Institute of Life Sciences, and the Center for Research on Pain, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem91904, Israel. [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
Mary Koukoui
Affiliation:
Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, Silberman Institute of Life Sciences, and the Center for Research on Pain, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem91904, Israel. [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]
Mark Baron
Affiliation:
Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, Silberman Institute of Life Sciences, and the Center for Research on Pain, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem91904, Israel. [email protected]; [email protected]; [email protected]

Abstract

Doubtless, the conscious brain integrates masses of information. But declaring that consciousness simply “emerges” when enough has accumulated, doesn't really explain how first person experience is implemented by neurons. Moreover, empirical observations challenge integrated information theory's (IIT) reliance on thalamo–cortical interactions as the information integrator. More likely, the cortex streams processed information to a still-enigmatic consciousness generator, one perhaps located in the brainstem.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Feuillet, L., Dufour, H., & Pelletier, J. (2007). Brain of a white-collar worker. Lancet, 370, 262.10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61127-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, P. M., Sherman, D., Pedersen, N. P., Saper, C. B., & Lu, J. (2011). Reassessment of the structural basis of the ascending arousal system. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 519, 933956.10.1002/cne.22559CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halder, S., Juel, B. E., Nilsen, A. S., Raghavan, L. V., & Storm, J. F. (2021). Changes in measures of consciousness during anaesthesia of one hemisphere (Wada test). NeuroImage, 226, 117566.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117566CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, J. A., & Scheibel, A. B. (1980). The brainstem core: Sensorimotor integration and behavioral state control. Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin, 18, 1173.Google ScholarPubMed
Jimenez Caballero, P. E. (2010). Bilateral paramedian thalamic artery infarcts: Report of 10 cases. Journal of Stroke and Cerebrovascular Diseases, 19, 283289.10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2009.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, M. (2007). Brain architecture: A design for natural computation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences, 365, 30333045.10.1098/rsta.2007.0007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNamara, P., Johnson, P., McLaren, D., Harris, E., Beauharnais, C., & Auerbach, S. (2010). REM and NREM sleep mentation. International Review of Neurobiology. 92, 6986.10.1016/S0074-7742(10)92004-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minert, A., Yatziv, S. L., & Devor, M. (2017). Location of the mesopontine neurons responsible for maintenance of anesthetic loss of consciousness. Journal of Neuroscience, 37, 93209331.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0544-17.2017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pal, D., Li, D., Dean, J. G., Brito, M. A., Liu, T., Fryzel, A. M., … Mashour, G. A. (2020). Level of consciousness is dissociable from electroencephalographic measures of cortical connectivity, slow oscillations, and complexity. Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 605618.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1910-19.2019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penfield, W. (1975). The mystery of the mind. A critical study of consciousness and the human brain. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ramon-Moliner, E., & Nauta, W. J. H. (1966). The isodendritic core of the brainstem. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 126, 311335.Google Scholar
Sakamoto, Y., Okubo, S., Kanamaru, T., Suzuki, K., & Kimura, K. (2015). A case report of bilateral paramedian thalamic and occult midbrain infarctions without disturbance of consciousness. Neurological Sciences, 36, 15111513.10.1007/s10072-015-2128-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tononi, G., & Sporns, O. (2003). Measuring information integration. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 31. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-4-31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volgushev, M., Chauvette, S., & Timofeev, I. (2011). Long-range correlation of the membrane potential in neocortical neurons during slow oscillation. Progress in Brain Research, 193, 181199.10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00012-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed