Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T13:54:47.201Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The logic of moral outrage

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 December 2012

Eric Luis Uhlmann*
Affiliation:
Management and Human Resources Department, HEC Paris – School of Management, 78351 Jouy-en-Josas, France. [email protected]

Abstract

McCullough et al.'s functionalist model of revenge is highly compatible with the person-centered approach to moral judgment, which emphasizes the adaptive manner in which social perceivers derive character information from moral acts. Evidence includes act–person dissociations in which an act is seen as less immoral than a comparison act, yet as a clearer indicator of poor moral character.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Becker, K. D., Steuwig, J., Herrera, V. M. & McCloskey, L. A. (2004) A study of firesetting and animal cruelty in children: Family influences and adolescent outcomes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 43:905–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaertner, S. L. & Dovidio, J. F. (1986) The aversive form of racism. In: Prejudice, discrimination, and racism, ed. Dovidio, J. F. & Gaertner, S. L., pp. 6189. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108:814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J., Koller, S. & Dias, M. (1993) Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65:613–28.Google Scholar
Hsee, C. K., Loewenstein, G. F., Blount, S. & Bazerman, M. H. (1999) Preference reversals between joint and separate evaluation of options: A review and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bulletin 125:576–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, J. D. (2005) Finding useful questions: On Bayesian diagnosticity, probability, impact, and information gain. Psychological Review 112:979–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, J. D., McKenzie, C. R. M., Cottrell, G. W. & Sejnowski, T. J. (2010) Experience matters: Information acquisition optimizes probability gain. Psychological Science 21:960–69.Google Scholar
Pizarro, D. A. & Tannenbaum, D. (2011) Bringing character back: How the motivation to evaluate character influences judgments of moral blame. In: The social psychology of morality: Exploring the causes of good and evil, ed. Shaver, P. & Mikulincer, M., APA Books.Google Scholar
Pizarro, D. A. & Uhlmann, E. L. (2005) Do normative standards advance our understanding of moral judgment? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28:558–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reeder, G. D. & Brewer, M. B. (1979) A schematic model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review 86:6179.Google Scholar
Tannenbaum, D., Uhlmann, E.L. & Diermeier, D. (2011) Moral signals, public outrage, and immaterial harms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47:1249–54.Google Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L. & Zhu, L. (under review) Acts, persons, and intuitions: Person-centered cues and gut reactions to harmless transgressions.Google Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L., Zhu, L. & Diermeier, D. (under review a) When actions speak volumes: Person-centered cues and moral outrage over racial bigotry.Google Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L., Zhu, L. & Tannenbaum, D. (under review b) When it takes a bad person to do the right thing.Google Scholar