No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
The evolution of analytic thought?
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 15 August 2017
Abstract
We argue that the truly unique aspect of human intelligence is not the variety of cognitive skills that are ontogenetically constructed, but rather the capacity to decide when to develop and apply said skills. Even if there is good evidence for g in nonhuman animals, we are left with major questions about how the disposition to think analytically can evolve.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017
References
Bear, A. & Rand, D. G. (2016a) Intuition, deliberation, and the evolution of cooperation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
113:1–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517780113.Google Scholar
Bear, A. & Rand, D. G. (2016b) Modeling intuition's origins. Journal of Applied Memory & Cognition
5:341–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bear, A., Kagan, A. & Rand, D. G. (2016) Co
-evolution of cooperation and cognition: The impact of imperfect deliberation and context-sensitive intuition. (November 1, 2016). Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=2862717.Google Scholar
Browne, M., Thomson, P., Rockloff, M. & Pennycook, G. (2015) Going against the herd: Understanding the psychosocial factors underlying the “vaccination confidence gap”. PLoS One
10:e1032562.Google Scholar
Frederick, S. (2005) Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives
19:25–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gervais, W. (2015) Override the controversy: Analytic thinking predicts endorsement of evolution. Cognition
142:312–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, W. M. & Norenzayan, A. (2012) Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science
336:493–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, V. & Mengel, F. (2011) Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in ultimatum games. Economics Letters
111:113–15.Google Scholar
Halali, E., Bereby-Meyer, Y. & Meiran, N. (2014) Between self-interest and reciprocity: The social bright side of self-control failure. Journal of Experiment Psychology: General
143:745–54.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Frederick, S. (2005) A model of heuristic judgement. In: The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning, ed. Holyoak, K. J. & Morrison, R. G., pp. 267–93. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R. C., Aureli, F., Baker, J. M., Bania, A. E., Barnard, A. M., Boogert, N. J., Brannon, E. M., Bray, E. E., Bray, J., Brent, L. J. N., Burkart, J. M., Call, J., Cantlon, J. F., Cheke, L. G., Clayton, N. S., Delgado, M. M., DiVinventi, L. J., Fujita, K., Herrmann, E., Hiramatsu, C., Jacobs, L. F., Jordan, K. E., Laude, J. R., Leimgruber, K. L., Messer, E. J. E., Moura, A. C. de, A., Ostojic, L., Picard, A., Platt, M. L., Plotnik, J. M., Range, F., Reader, S. M., Reddy, R. B., Sandel, A. A., Santos, L. R., Schumann, K., Seed, A. M., Sewall, K. B., Shaw, R. C., Slocombe, K. E., Yanjie, S., Takimoto, A., Tan, J., Tao, R., van Schaik, C. P., Viranyi, Z., Visalberghi, E., Wade, J. C., Watanabe, A., Widness, J., Young, J. K., Zentall, T. R. & Zhao, Y. (2014) The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA
111:E2140–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014) The role of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking & Reasoning
20:188–214.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015a) On the reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit. Judgment and Decision Making
10:549–63.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. (2016a) Is the cognitive reflection test a measure of both reflection and intuition?
Behavior Research Methods
48:341–48.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. (2012) Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition
123:335–46.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A. & Koehler, D. J. (2015b) Everyday consequences of analytic thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science
24:425–43.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Ross, R., Koehler, D. J. & Fugelsang, J. A. (2016b) Atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers: Four empirical studies and a meta-analysis. PLoS One
11:e0153039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G. (2016) Cooperation, fast and slow: Meta-analytic evidence for a theory of social heuristics and self-interested deliberation. Psychological Science
27:1192–206. doi: 10.1177/0956797616654455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., Brescoll, V. L., Everett, J. A. C., Capraro, V. & Barcelo, H. (2016) Social heuristics and social roles: Altruism is intuitive for women but not for men. Journal of Experiment Psychology: General
145:389–96.Google Scholar
Rand, D. G., Greene, J. D. & Nowak, M. A. (2012) Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature
498:427–30.Google Scholar
Rand, D. G., Peysakhovich, A., Kraft-Todd, G. T., Newman, G. E., Wurzbacher, O., Nowak, M. A. & Greene, J. D. (2014) Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation. Nature Communications
5:3677. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4677.Google Scholar
Rosati, A. G. & Santos, L. R. (2016) Spontaneous metacognition in Rhesus monkeys. Psychological Science
27:1181–91. doi: 10.1177/0956797616653737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Royzman, E. B., Landy, J. F. & Goodwin, G. P. (2014) Are good reasoners more incest-friendly? Trait cognitive reflection predicts selective moralization in a sample of American adults. Judgment and Decision Making
9:176–90.Google Scholar
Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G. & Greene, J. D. (2012) Divine intuition: Cognitive style influences belief in God. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
141:423–28.Google Scholar
Shtulman, A. & McCallum, K. (2014) Cognitive reflection predicts science understanding. In: Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Quebec, ON, July 2014, pp. 2937–42. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2009a) Is it time for a tri-process theory? Distinguishing the reflective and algorithmic mind. In: In two minds: Dual processes and beyond, ed. Evans, J. St. B. T. & Frankish, K., pp. 55–88. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2009b) What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of rational thought. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (1998) Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
127:161–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2000) Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate?
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
23:645–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutter, M., Kocher, M. & Strauß, S. (2003) Bargaining under time pressure in an experimental ultimatum game. Economics Letters
81:341–47.Google Scholar
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S. & Furnham, A. (2014) Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition
133:572–85.Google Scholar
Tomlin, D. A., Rand, D. G., Ludvig, E. A. & Cohen, J. D. (2015) The evolution and devolution of cognitive control: The costs of deliberation in a competitive world. Scientific Reports
5:11002. doi: 10.1038/srep11002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toupo, D. F. P., Strogatz, S. H., Cohen, J. D. & Rand, D. G. (2015) Evolutionary game dynamics of controlled and automatic decision-making. Chaos
25:073120. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1063/1.4927488.Google Scholar
Target article
The evolution of general intelligence
Related commentaries (28)
G and g: Two markers of a general cognitive ability, or none?
G but not g: In search of the evolutionary continuity of intelligence
A pointer's hypothesis of general intelligence evolved from domain-specific demands
An all-positive correlation matrix is not evidence of domain-general intelligence
Coexistence of general intelligence and specialized modules
Contemporary evolutionary psychology and the evolution of intelligence
Disentangling learning from knowing: Does associative learning ability underlie performances on cognitive test batteries?
Domains of generality
Evolution, brain size, and variations in intelligence
General intelligence does not help us understand cognitive evolution
General intelligence is a source of individual differences between species: Solving an anomaly
General intelligence is an emerging property, not an evolutionary puzzle
Genomic data can illuminate the architecture and evolution of cognitive abilities
Habit formation generates secondary modules that emulate the efficiency of evolved behavior
Hierarchy, multidomain modules, and the evolution of intelligence
It's time to move beyond the “Great Chain of Being”
Negative results are needed to show the specific value of a cultural explanation for g
Of mice and men, nature and nurture, and a few red herrings
Taking a multiple intelligences (MI) perspective
The evolution of analytic thought?
The evolution of fluid intelligence meets formative g
The evolution of general intelligence in all animals and machines
The false dichotomy of domain-specific versus domain-general cognition
Theory of mind: A foundational component of human general intelligence
Understanding the relationship between general intelligence and socio-cognitive abilities in humans
When does cultural transmission favour or instead substitute for general intelligence?
Where is the evidence for general intelligence in nonhuman animals?
“Birdbrains” should not be ignored in studying the evolution of g
Author response
Future directions for studying the evolution of general intelligence